Next Article in Journal
microRNA Biology on Brain Development and Neuroimaging Approach
Next Article in Special Issue
Exploring the Sensitivity of Prodromal Dementia with Lewy Bodies Research Criteria
Previous Article in Journal
White Button Mushroom (Agaricus bisporus) Supplementation Ameliorates Spatial Memory Deficits and Plaque Formation in an Amyloid Precursor Protein Mouse Model of Alzheimer’s Disease
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Motor Signs and Cognitive Performance in Cognitively Unimpaired Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study Using the NACC Database

by
Vasileios Siokas
1,*,
Ioannis Liampas
1,
Constantine G. Lyketsos
2 and
Efthimios Dardiotis
1,2
1
Department of Neurology, University Hospital of Larissa, Faculty of Medicine, School of Health Sciences, University of Thessaly, 41100 Larissa, Greece
2
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(10), 1365; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12101365
Submission received: 22 September 2022 / Revised: 5 October 2022 / Accepted: 6 October 2022 / Published: 8 October 2022

Abstract

:
Aiming to examine whether specific motor signs are associated with worse performance in specific cognitive domains among cognitively unimpaired (CU) individuals, we performed a cross-sectional analysis of data from the baseline evaluations of older, CU participants from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Uniform Data Set. In total, 8149 CU (≥60 years) participants were included. Of these, 905 individuals scored ≥ 2 on at least one of the motor domains of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part III (UPDRSIII). Cognitively impaired individuals, participants with psychiatric disorders and/or under treatment with antipsychotic, anxiolytic, sedative or hypnotic agents were excluded. Nine motor signs were examined: hypophonia, masked facies, resting tremor, action/postural tremor, rigidity, bradykinesia, impaired chair rise, impaired posture/gait and postural instability. Their association with performance on episodic memory, semantic memory, language, attention, processing speed or executive function was assessed using crude and adjusted linear regression models. Individuals with impaired chair rise had worse episodic memory, semantic memory, processing speed and executive function, while those with bradykinesia had worse language, processing speed and executive function. Sensitivity analyses, by excluding participants with cerebrovascular disease or PD, or other Parkinsonism, produced similar results with the exception of the relationship between bradykinesia and language performance.

1. Introduction

Individuals diagnosed with dementia often present neurologic signs and cognitive symptoms years before the diagnosis of the syndrome [1,2]. Motor signs are common clinical features of dementia syndromes [3,4]. The presence of motor signs in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) has been associated with worse cognitive decline and faster disease progression [5,6,7,8,9]. Apart from AD, motor signs have been associated with poorer cognitive performance in patients with schizophrenia [10], while specific characteristics of the motor signs may predict cognitive decline in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) [11,12].
Motor signs and cognitive performance appear to be parallel manifestations of underlying brain disease. Studies regarding trajectories of motor and cognitive performance in the general population found that decline of motor and cognitive function may vary, and that one may predate the other [13,14,15]. Moreover, motor change and cognitive decline share common neuropathology (e.g., AD pathology in motor-related brain regions, neuroinflammation, degeneration of the basal forebrain cholinergic system, Lewy body pathology) and risk factors (e.g., APOE ε4 carriage, odor identification, medical factors, psychosocial) [16,17,18].
Of note, motor signs may be useful clinical markers for better differentiation between dementia subtypes [19]. However, motor signs are under-recorded in clinical practice [3]. Deeper knowledge regarding associations between motor signs and cognitive performance could shed more light onto the underlying implicated mechanisms, and further increase awareness of the importance of their identification and clinical recording [3].
A methodological limitation of the current literature regarding motor signs and cognitive performance has been focus on specific dementia phenotypes [20]. Moreover, studies evaluating the possible role of motor signs in cognitive performance focus on specific neuropsychological tests without covering a broad range of cognitive performance. Consequently, it cannot be estimated with high certainty, which specific cognitive domains (and to what extend) are associated with specific motor signs [12,21,22,23].
To address these gaps, we cross-sectionally analyzed data from the Uniform Data Set (UDS), a standardized set of prospectively collected data from multiple Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centers (ADRCs) across the United States. The aim of this study was to examine associations between motor signs and cognitive performance in cognitively unimpaired (CU) older (≥60 years) adults. Using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part III (UPDRSIII) we examined whether specific motor signs are associated with worse cognitive performance in specific domains: episodic memory, language, attention, semantic memory, processing speed and executive function.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. General Information

The National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) has administrated the UDS initiative since 2005. UDS is a freely available repository (https://naccdata.org/, accessed on 1 July 2022) containing data (clinical, genetic and neuropathological) from all National Institute on Aging-funded ADRCs across the United States. Local Institutional Review Boards supervise each ADRC. All participants (or representatives) granted informed consent before inclusion. Details about the UDS have been previously extensively described [24]. Clinical personnel, trained physicians and other personnel used a common, standardized evaluation protocol and collected data from in-person visits or via telephone calls. Annually, follow-up assessments of the participants were also performed.

2.2. Eligibility Criteria for Participation

We included data from each participant’s first NACC visit (baseline). The recruiting period was between September 2005 through December 2021 and involved participants from 43 ADRCs aged ≥60 years. A participant was eligible for inclusion as CU because of the absence of dementia, MCI or cognitive impairment not MCI, based on physician diagnosis. For the majority of participants, the cognitive assessments were performed by interdisciplinary consensus teams. Data regarding personal history, psychosocial function and neuropsychological performance were considered in order for a diagnosis to be established. Standard clinical criteria were applied for dementia syndromes and MCI clinical diagnosis [25,26,27,28,29], while cognitively impaired participants who did not meet criteria for dementia/MCI were diagnosed as cognitively impaired—not MCI.

2.3. Exclusion Criteria

The following exclusions were applied: (1) treatment with an FDA-approved medication for AD; (2) presence of a psychiatric disorder diagnosed on clinical basis; (3) treatment with hypnotic, antipsychotic, sedative or anxiolytic drugs. The rational for these exclusions was to avoid potential confounding effects of these conditions on cognitive performance [30,31,32].

2.4. Measurement of Motor Symptoms

UPDRSIII was used to assess the presence or absence of motor signs. The UPDRSIII consists of 27 subitems rated during a neurological examination. For clinical purposes, we grouped the 27 subitems of the UPDRSIII into 9 signs [19] as follows: (1) hypophonia (single item); (2) masked facies (single item); (3) resting tremor (combined 5 items regarding tremor at rest in the face/lips/chin and 4 extremities); (4) action/postural tremor (combined 2 items regarding tremor at rest in the hands); (5) rigidity (combined 5 items regarding rigidity in the neck and 4 extremities); (6) bradykinesia (combined 9 items: bilateral finger tapping, hand movements, rapid alternating movements of the hands, leg agility and body bradykinesia); (7) impaired chair rise (single item); (8) impaired posture/gait (combined 2 items: posture and gait); and (9) postural instability (single item).
Each motor sign was graded as absent (score < 2) or present (score ≥ 2). The rational for this cutoff is as follows: (1) this level of severity is more likely to be noted by the average clinician [5]; and (2) a score of 1 is suggestive of a very mild motor change that could be observed with normal aging [19].
Then, we created a dichotomous categorical variable, such that participants were said to have an abnormal UPDRSIII if they scored ≥2 in at least one of the 9 created signs. Otherwise, they were considered to have a normal UPDRSIII. Similarly, a dichotomous motor sign variable was created for each of the 9 signs, with participants considered to have a motor domain if they scored ≥2 in at least one of the items of each domain.

2.5. Measurement of Cognitive Performance

The following cognitive domains were considered: (1) episodic memory (delayed and immediate recall) evaluated with the Logical Memory Test (Story A) from the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised (WMS-R) [33]; (2) language on total word production summing animal and vegetable fluency tasks [34]; (3) semantic memory based on the 30-item version of the Boston Naming Test (BNT-30) [35]; (4) attention assessed on the Digit Span Test (DST, forward and backward conditions) from the WMS-R [33]; (5) processing speed on the Trail Making Test—Part A (TMT-A) and (6) executive function on the Trail Making Test—Part B (TMT-B) [36]. Details about the scoring of the above-mentioned tests have been reported [24]. In brief, episodic memory (the sum of items recalled in the delayed recall (0–25 total items retrieved) and immediate (0–25 total items retrieved) tasks), language (the sum of word production in the vegetable and animal 1-min category fluency tasks), semantic memory (BNT-30 (0–30 items recalled)), attention (the sum of the longest sequences in DST backward (0–7 digits) and forward (0–8 digits) conditions), processing speed (total time in TMT-A (0–150 s)) and, finally, executive function (total time in TMT-B (0–300 s)).

2.6. Covariates

The following covariates were considered for every participant, when possible, as they may confound the relationship between motor signs and cognitive performance: age in years at the time of the first evaluation, education in years of formal schooling (as continuous variables); sex, race; history of the following: cardiovascular disease (including heart attack, cardiac arrest, congestive heart failure or heart surgical procedures (coronal angioplasty, endarterectomy, stent or cardiac bypass procedure)), cerebrovascular disease (including transient ischemic attack or stroke), Parkinson’s disease, other Parkinsonian disorder, traumatic brain injury (TBI), thyroid disease, history of epileptic seizures, vitamin B12 deficiency, alcohol or other substance abuse and current use of antidepressant agents, as categorical variables.

2.7. Statistical Analysis

Continuous variables are described with means and standard deviations (SD). Values for categorical variables are expressed as total number (n) as well as percentage of the total (%). Differences in demographic and clinical characteristics between the participants with normal UPDRSIII and those with abnormal UPDRSIII were compared with independent t-tests for the continuous variables, and Pearson’s chi-squared tests for categorical ones.
As decline of motor and cognitive function may vary, and one may predate the other, we considered in the analysis the motor signs as the independent variables and the performance in cognitive domains as the dependent variables, for presentation purposes only. Consequently, we estimated linear regression models to examine associations between the 9 motor signs and cognitive performance on the 6 neuropsychological domains. We adjusted analyses for the covariates that significantly differed between those normal and those with abnormal UPDRSIII and motor signs.
Sensitivity analyses were performed using the same approach by excluding CU participants with at least one of the following: (1) PD, (2) other Parkinsonian disorders and (3) cerebrovascular disease.
Correction for multiple comparisons was made with the conservative Bonferroni method. We divided the conventional threshold of α = 0.05 with the number of the comparisons performed in the main analysis. We examine the association between 9 motor signs and 6 cognitive domains (54 comparisons in total), leading to a statistically significant threshold of p < 0.05/54 = 0.000925. The statistical analysis was performed using the IBM SPSS Statistics Software Version 26 (Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Baseline Characteristics

The initial sample consisted of 17,605 CU participants. After the exclusion of participants with age lower that than 60 years old, those receiving FDA-approved medication for AD, antipsychotic, hypnotic, sedative or anxiolytic agents, those with a clinician-based diagnosis of a psychiatric disease and participants without UPDRSIII assessment, 8149 participants (n = 7244 with normal UPDRSIII and n = 905 with abnormal UPDRSIII) were included in the analysis. A flowchart of participant selection is in Figure S1. The baseline characteristics of the included participants and comparison between those with normal and those with abnormal UPDRSIII are in Table 1.
The number of the participants per motor sign included in the analysis for each cognitive domain are in Table S1.
The group with abnormal UPDRS ratings was older and less educated. The frequency of cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, PD, other Parkinsonian disorder, TBI, history of seizures and use of antidepressants was higher in the abnormal UPDRSIII group. Participants with abnormal UPDRS performed worse in all 6 cognitive domains assessed.

3.2. Main Analysis

3.2.1. Motor Signs and Episodic Memory

Bradykinesia, impaired rise and postural instability were associated with worse cognitive performance in episodic memory in unadjusted analysis. The statistically significant association between impaired chair rise and worse episodic memory remained in adjusted regression (Table 2).

3.2.2. Motor Signs and Language

Bradykinesia, impaired rise, impaired posture/gait and postural instability were associated with worse cognitive performance in language in unadjusted analysis. The statistically significant association between bradykinesia and worse language performance was maintained in adjusted linear regression (Table 3).

3.2.3. Motor Signs and Attention

Bradykinesia, impaired rise, impaired posture/gait and postural instability were associated with worse cognitive performance in attention in unadjusted analysis. None of these associations were maintained in adjusted linear regression (Table 4).

3.2.4. Motor Signs and Semantic Memory

Bradykinesia, impaired rise, impaired posture/gait and postural instability were associated with worse cognitive performance in semantic memory in unadjusted analysis. The statistically significant association between impaired chair rise and worse semantic memory was maintained in adjusted linear regression (Table 5).

3.2.5. Motor Signs and Processing Speed

All motor signs were associated with worse cognitive performance in processing speed in unadjusted analysis. The statistically significant associations for bradykinesia and impaired chair rise were maintained in adjusted linear regression (Table 6).

3.2.6. Motor Signs and Executive Function

Hypophonia, rigidity, bradykinesia, impaired rise, impaired posture/gait and postural instability were associated with worse cognitive performance in executive function in unadjusted analysis. The statistically significant associations for bradykinesia and impaired chair rise were maintained in adjusted linear regression (Table 7).

3.3. Sensitivity Analysis

The number of the participants per motor signs included in the sensitivity analysis for each cognitive domain are in Table S2. Overall, adjusted linear regression models (after excluding participants with at least one of the following: (1) PD, (2) other Parkinsonian disorders and (3) cerebrovascular disease) produced similar results with the main full adjusted analyses, with the exception of the association between bradykinesia and worse performance in language.
In summary, impaired chair rise was associated with worse cognitive performance in episodic memory, semantic memory, processing speed and executive function, while bradykinesia was associated with worse cognitive performance in processing speed and executive function. Results presenting the adjusted linear regression analysis for the association between motor signs and cognitive performance are presented at Table S3.

4. Discussion

We examined associations between 9 motor signs and cognitive performance in 6 domains among CU older adults. Our analyses revealed that participants with impaired chair rise or bradykinesia had worse cognitive performance in several domains: impaired chair rise was associated with worse episodic memory, semantic memory, processing speed and executive function, while bradykinesia was associated with worse language, processing speed and executive function. These results persisted after adjustment and sensitivity analyses, with the exception of the relationship between bradykinesia and language performance. Consequently, impaired chair rise and bradykinesia may be candidates as clinical indicators for worse cognitive performance among CU older adults.
Motor signs have emerged as possible predictors of dementia risk as well as markers of different types of dementia [19]. Bradykinesia has been associated with increased risk for dementia in patients with PD and in patients with MCI [37,38]. Impaired chair rise has been associated with reduced ability to perform activities of daily living in PD patients [39]. Moreover, greater gait impairment may predict dementia, and postural instability has been associated with increased dementia risk in MCI and CU individuals [38,40,41].
From a pathophysiological view, there are shared common pathologies between motor and cognitive functions, such as vascular risk factors, inflammation and neurodegeneration [42]. Moreover, motor signs in patients with autosomal dominant AD are related to the amount of fibrillar amyloid-β in the basal ganglia a [18]. Moreover, persons with AD have more β-amyloid deposition in both brain and muscle fibers compared with individuals without dementia [43]. However, the exact pathophysiological processes underlying the relationship between cortical amyloid deposit and motor performance remains elusive [44].
The current study has several strengths. First, we included of large number of clinically well-characterized participants assessed using a rigorous protocol. Second, we accounted for many potential cofounding variables. Third, motor signs were clinically assessed using the UPDRSIII [45]. Finally, the major findings were impervious to adjustment and sensitivity analyses, suggesting a generalization independent of clinical conditions that are related to motor disability.
We also acknowledge study limitations. First, this is a cross-sectional study with all inherent limitations [46]. Second, we dichotomized motor signs on UPDRSIII, based on whether a participant scored ≥2 in at least one of the items of each domain. Consequently, the analysis did not account overall motor function as a continuous variable. Despite the rationale behind the use ≥2 as a cut-off for the presence of motor sign, we bear in mind that some motor signs (e.g., resting tremor and the impaired gait) may be considered abnormal even with a value equal to 1, regardless of age [19]. Finally, despite the fact that we performed adjusted analysis for many potential cofounding variables, the possibility of the latent effect of other potential co-founders (e.g., subjective cognitive decline, history of infections or frailty) cannot totally be excluded.
From a clinical perspective, the associations between motor signs and cognitive performance raise interesting issues. Motor signs can be easily assessed by the majority of physicians independent of specialty, even in primary care. As such they may serve as a low-cost alternative to higher-cost indicators of worse cognitive performance [47,48] allowing for earlier detection of CU individuals with low cognitive performance and the opportunity to apply personalized cognitive training and rehabilitation. Therefore, longitudinal research with additional correction for age-related disorders associated with motor signs in older persons is needed. Moreover, combined cognitive and motor training seems to improve cognitive and motor performances in AD and PD [49,50]. Towards this direction, future studies should examine the clinical applicability of our finding, by measuring the effects of combined cognitive with physical rehabilitation in improving cognitive performance in CU individuals with motor signs.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci12101365/s1, Figure S1: Flowchart of participant selection; Tables S1–S3: (Table S1: Number of the subjects per 9 created motor signs and cognitive domains; Table S2: The number of the subjects per 9 created motor signs and cognitive domains, after excluding subjects with cerebrovascular disease or Parkinson’s disease or other Parkinsonian disorder; Table S3: Sensitivity analysis with linear regression, for association between motor signs (that significantly associated in the main analyses) and performance in cognitive domains, after excluding subjects with cerebrovascular disease or Parkinson’s disease or other Parkinsonian disorder.)

Author Contributions

V.S.: original draft preparation, data curation, formal analysis, design of the study, interpretation of data, review and editing of manuscript; I.L.: data curation, validation, review and editing of manuscript; C.G.L.: conceptualization, formulation of research question, design of the study, supervision, review and editing; E.D.: conceptualization, formulation of research question, design of the study, supervision, review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The NACC database is funded by NIA/NIH Grant U24 AG072122. NACC data are contributed by the NIA-funded ADRCs: P30 AG062429 (PI James Brewer, MD, PhD), P30 AG066468 (PI Oscar Lopez, MD), P30 AG062421 (PI Bradley Hyman, MD, PhD), P30 AG066509 (PI Thomas Grabowski, MD), P30 AG066514 (PI Mary Sano, PhD), P30 AG066530 (PI Helena Chui, MD), P30 AG066507 (PI Marilyn Albert, PhD), P30 AG066444 (PI John Morris, MD), P30 AG066518 (PI Jeffrey Kaye, MD), P30 AG066512 (PI Thomas Wisniewski, MD), P30 AG066462 (PI Scott Small, MD), P30 AG072979 (PI David Wolk, MD), P30 AG072972 (PI Charles DeCarli, MD), P30 AG072976 (PI Andrew Saykin, PsyD), P30 AG072975 (PI David Bennett, MD), P30 AG072978 (PI Neil Kowall, MD), P30 AG072977 (PI Robert Vassar, PhD), P30 AG066519 (PI Frank LaFerla, PhD), P30 AG062677 (PI Ronald Petersen, MD, PhD), P30 AG079280 (PI Eric Reiman, MD), P30 AG062422 (PI Gil Rabinovici, MD), P30 AG066511 (PI Allan Levey, MD, PhD), P30 AG072946 (PI Linda Van Eldik, PhD), P30 AG062715 (PI Sanjay Asthana, MD, FRCP), P30 AG072973 (PI Russell Swerdlow, MD), P30 AG066506 (PI Todd Golde, MD, PhD), P30 AG066508 (PI Stephen Strittmatter, MD, PhD), P30 AG066515 (PI Victor Henderson, MD, MS), P30 AG072947 (PI Suzanne Craft, PhD), P30 AG072931 (PI Henry Paulson, MD, PhD), P30 AG066546 (PI Sudha Seshadri, MD), P20 AG068024 (PI Erik Roberson, MD, PhD), P20 AG068053 (PI Justin Miller, PhD), P20 AG068077 (PI Gary Rosenberg, MD), P20 AG068082 (PI Angela Jefferson, PhD), P30 AG072958 (PI Heather Whitson, MD), P30 AG072959 (PI James Leverenz, MD). Dr. Lyketsos was supported by P30 AG066507.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The Institutional Review Boards overseeing each ADRC approved all procedures before the initiation of the study.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

For further information on access to the NACC database, please contact NACC (contact details can be found at https://naccdata.org/, accessed on 1 July 2022).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ewers, M.; Sperling, R.A.; Klunk, W.E.; Weiner, M.W.; Hampel, H. Neuroimaging markers for the prediction and early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Trends Neurosci. 2011, 34, 430–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Rosenberg, P.B.; Mielke, M.M.; Appleby, B.S.; Oh, E.S.; Geda, Y.E.; Lyketsos, C.G. The association of neuropsychiatric sym toms in MCI with incident dementia and Alzheimer disease. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2013, 21, 685–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Al-Harrasi, A.M.; Iqbal, E.; Tsamakis, K.; Lasek, J.; Gadelrab, R.; Soysal, P.; Kohlhoff, E.; Tsiptsios, D.; Rizos, E.; Perera, G.; et al. Motor signs in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: Detection through natural language processing, co-morbid features and relationship to adverse outcomes. Exp. Gerontol. 2021, 146, 111223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Scarmeas, N.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.M.; Papadimitriou, A.; Dubois, B.; Sarazin, M.; Brandt, J.; Albert, M.; Marder, K.; Bell, K.; Honig, L.S.; et al. Motor signs during the course of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2004, 63, 975–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Scarmeas, N.; Albert, M.; Brandt, J.; Blacker, D.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.; Papadimitriou, A.; Dubois, B.; Sarazin, M.; Wegesin, D.; Marder, K.; et al. Motor signs predict poor outcomes in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2005, 64, 1696–1703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Miller, T.P.; Tinklenberg, J.R.; Brooks, J.O., 3rd; Yesavage, J.A. Cognitive decline in patients with Alzheimer disease: Differences in patients with and without extrapyramidal signs. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 1991, 5, 251–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Chui, H.C.; Lyness, S.A.; Sobel, E.; Schneider, L.S. Extrapyramidal signs and psychiatric symptoms predict faster cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 1994, 51, 676–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Clark, C.M.; Ewbank, D.; Lerner, A.; Doody, R.; Henderson, V.W.; Panisset, M.; Morris, J.C.; Fillenbaum, G.G.; Heyman, A. The relationship between extrapyramidal signs and cognitive performance in patients with Alzheimer’s disease enrolled in the CERAD Study. Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1997, 49, 70–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Soininen, H.; Helkala, E.L.; Laulumaa, V.; Soikkeli, R.; Hartikainen, P.; Riekkinen, P.J. Cognitive profile of Alzheimer patients with extrapyramidal signs: A longitudinal study. J. Neural. Transm. Park. Dis. Dement. Sect. 1992, 4, 241–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Fervaha, G.; Agid, O.; Takeuchi, H.; Lee, J.; Foussias, G.; Zakzanis, K.K.; Graff-Guerrero, A.; Remington, G. Extrapyramidal symptoms and cognitive test performance in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2015, 161, 351–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Katzen, H.L.; Levin, B.E.; Weiner, W. Side and type of motor symptom influence cognition in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 2006, 21, 1947–1953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Wang, Y.X.; Zhao, J.; Li, D.K.; Peng, F.; Wang, Y.; Yang, K.; Liu, Z.Y.; Liu, F.T.; Wu, J.J.; Wang, J. Associations between cognitive impairment and motor dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Behav. 2017, 7, e00719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Darweesh, S.K.L.; Licher, S.; Wolters, F.J.; Koudstaal, P.J.; Ikram, M.K.; Ikram, M.A. Quantitative gait, cognitive decline, and incident dementia: The Rotterdam Study. Alzheimers Dement 2019, 15, 1264–1273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Darweesh, S.K.L.; Wolters, F.J.; Postuma, R.B.; Stricker, B.H.; Hofman, A.; Koudstaal, P.J.; Ikram, M.K.; Ikram, M.A. Association Between Poor Cognitive Functioning and Risk of Incident Parkinsonism: The Rotterdam Study. JAMA Neurol. 2017, 74, 1431–1438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. van der Willik, K.D.; Licher, S.; Vinke, E.J.; Knol, M.J.; Darweesh, S.K.L.; van der Geest, J.N.; Schagen, S.B.; Ikram, M.K.; Luik, A.I.; Ikram, M.A. Trajectories of Cognitive and Motor Function Between Ages 45 and 90 Years: A Population-Based Study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2021, 76, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Aarsland, D.; Batzu, L.; Halliday, G.M.; Geurtsen, G.J.; Ballard, C.; Ray Chaudhuri, K.; Weintraub, D. Parkinson disease-associated cognitive impairment. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 2021, 7, 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Buchman, A.S.; Bennett, D.A. Loss of motor function in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2011, 11, 665–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Vöglein, J.; Paumier, K.; Jucker, M.; Preische, O.; McDade, E.; Hassenstab, J.; Benzinger, T.L.; Noble, J.M.; Berman, S.B.; Graff-Radford, N.R.; et al. Clinical, pathophysiological and genetic features of motor symptoms in autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 2019, 142, 1429–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Kaur, B.; Harvey, D.J.; Decarli, C.S.; Zhang, L.; Sabbagh, M.N.; Olichney, J.M. Extrapyramidal signs by dementia severity in Alzheimer disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2013, 27, 226–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Kueper, J.K.; Speechley, M.; Lingum, N.R.; Montero-Odasso, M. Motor function and incident dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Age Ageing 2017, 46, 729–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Wojtala, J.; Heber, I.A.; Neuser, P.; Heller, J.; Kalbe, E.; Rehberg, S.P.; Storch, A.; Linse, K.; Schneider, C.; Gräber, S.; et al. Cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease: The impact of the motor phenotype on cognition. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2019, 90, 171–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Uc, E.Y.; Rizzo, M.; Anderson, S.W.; Qian, S.; Rodnitzky, R.L.; Dawson, J.D. Visual dysfunction in Parkinson disease without dementia. Neurology 2005, 65, 1907–1913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Leisman, G.; Moustafa, A.A.; Shafir, T. Thinking, Walking, Talking: Integratory Motor and Cognitive Brain Function. Front. Public Health 2016, 4, 94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Weintraub, S.; Salmon, D.; Mercaldo, N.; Ferris, S.; Graff-Radford, N.R.; Chui, H.; Cummings, J.; DeCarli, C.; Foster, N.L.; Galasko, D.; et al. The Alzheimer’s Disease Centers’ Uniform Data Set (UDS): The neuropsychologic test battery. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2009, 23, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Petersen, R.C.; Smith, G.E.; Waring, S.C.; Ivnik, R.J.; Tangalos, E.G.; Kokmen, E. Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 1999, 56, 303–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. McKeith, I.G.; Dickson, D.W.; Lowe, J.; Emre, M.; O’Brien, J.T.; Feldman, H.; Cummings, J.; Duda, J.E.; Lippa, C.; Perry, E.K.; et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 2005, 65, 1863–1872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. McKhann, G.; Drachman, D.; Folstein, M.; Katzman, R.; Price, D.; Stadlan, E.M. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984, 34, 939–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Neary, D.; Snowden, J.S.; Gustafson, L.; Passant, U.; Stuss, D.; Black, S.; Freedman, M.; Kertesz, A.; Robert, P.H.; Albert, M.; et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998, 51, 1546–1554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Román, G.C.; Tatemichi, T.K.; Erkinjuntti, T.; Cummings, J.L.; Masdeu, J.C.; Garcia, J.H.; Amaducci, L.; Orgogozo, J.M.; Brun, A.; Hofman, A.; et al. Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 1993, 43, 250–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Leoutsakos, J.S.; Wise, E.A.; Lyketsos, C.G.; Smith, G.S. Trajectories of neuropsychiatric symptoms over time in healthy volunteers and risk of MCI and dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2019, 34, 1865–1873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Hill, S.K.; Bishop, J.R.; Palumbo, D.; Sweeney, J.A. Effect of second-generation antipsychotics on cognition: Current issues and future challenges. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2010, 10, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Millan, M.J.; Agid, Y.; Brüne, M.; Bullmore, E.T.; Carter, C.S.; Clayton, N.S.; Connor, R.; Davis, S.; Deakin, B.; DeRubeis, R.J.; et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 2012, 11, 141–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; The Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  34. Moms, J.C.; Heyman, A.; Mohs, R.C.; Hughes, J.P.; van Belle, G.; Fillenbaum, G.; Mellits, E.D.; Clark, C. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assesment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1989, 39, 1159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Goodglass, H.; Kaplan, E.; Weintraub, S. BDAE: The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination; Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hultsch, D.F.; Strauss, E.; Hunter, M.A.; MacDonald, S.W. Intraindividual variability, cognition, and aging. In The Handbook of Aging and Cognition; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2011; pp. 497–562. [Google Scholar]
  37. Levy, G.; Tang, M.X.; Cote, L.J.; Louis, E.D.; Alfaro, B.; Mejia, H.; Stern, Y.; Marder, K. Motor impairment in PD: Relationship to incident dementia and age. Neurology 2000, 55, 539–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Aggarwal, N.T.; Wilson, R.S.; Beck, T.L.; Bienias, J.L.; Bennett, D.A. Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2006, 63, 1763–1769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bryant, M.S.; Kang, G.E.; Protas, E.J. Relation of chair rising ability to activities of daily living and physical activity in Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Physiother. 2020, 10, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Camicioli, R.; Wang, Y.; Powell, C.; Mitnitski, A.; Rockwood, K. Gait and posture impairment, parkinsonism and cognitive decline in older people. J. Neural Transm. 2007, 114, 1355–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Beauchet, O.; Annweiler, C.; Callisaya, M.L.; De Cock, A.M.; Helbostad, J.L.; Kressig, R.W.; Srikanth, V.; Steinmetz, J.P.; Blumen, H.M.; Verghese, J.; et al. Poor Gait Performance and Prediction of Dementia: Results From a Meta-Analysis. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2016, 17, 482–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Parihar, R.; Mahoney, J.R.; Verghese, J. Relationship of Gait and Cognition in the Elderly. Curr. Transl. Geriatr. Exp. Gerontol. Rep. 2013, 2, 167–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Roher, A.E.; Esh, C.L.; Kokjohn, T.A.; Castaño, E.M.; Van Vickle, G.D.; Kalback, W.M.; Patton, R.L.; Luehrs, D.C.; Daugs, I.D.; Kuo, Y.M.; et al. Amyloid beta peptides in human plasma and tissues and their significance for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2009, 5, 18–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. de Souto Barreto, P.; Cesari, M.; Rolland, Y.; Salabert, A.S.; Payoux, P.; Andrieu, S.; Vellas, B. Cross-Sectional and Prospective Associations Between β-Amyloid in the Brain and Chair Rise Performance in Nondementia Older Adults With Spontaneous Memory Complaints. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2017, 72, 278–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Maraki, M.I.; Stefanis, L.; Yannakoulia, M.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Xiromerisiou, G.; Dardiotis, E.; Hadjigeorgiou, G.M.; Sakka, P.; Scarmeas, N.; Stamelou, M. Motor function and the probability of prodromal Parkinson’s disease in older adults. Mov. Disord. 2019, 34, 1345–1353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Wang, X.; Cheng, Z. Cross-Sectional Studies: Strengths, Weaknesses, and Recommendations. Chest 2020, 158, S65–S71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lee, S.A.W.; Sposato, L.A.; Hachinski, V.; Cipriano, L.E. Cost-effectiveness of cerebrospinal biomarkers for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Res. Ther. 2017, 9, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Scarth, M.; Rissanen, I.; Scholten, R.; Geerlings, M.I. Biomarkers of Alzheimer’s Disease and Cerebrovascular Lesions and Clinical Progression in Patients with Subjective Cognitive Decline: A Systematic Review. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2021, 83, 1089–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Tay, L.; Lim, W.S.; Chan, M.; Ali, N.; Chong, M.S. A Combined Cognitive Stimulation and Physical Exercise Programme (MINDVital) in Early Dementia: Differential Effects on Single- and Dual-Task Gait Performance. Gerontology 2016, 62, 604–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Fearon, C.; Killane, I.; Newman, L.; Beck, R.; Munteanu, T.; Birsanu, D.; Butler, J.; Magennis, B.; Reilly, R.; Lynch, T. Combined Motor and Cognitive Training Improves Motor and Cognitive Function in People with Parkinson’s Disease and Freezing of Gait. In Proceedings of the Movement Disorders, Vancouver, BC, Canada, 4–8 June 2017. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Baseline differences between cognitively unimpaired individuals with normal and with abnormal UPDRSIII.
Table 1. Baseline differences between cognitively unimpaired individuals with normal and with abnormal UPDRSIII.
VariableNormal UPDRSIII (N = 7244)Abnormal UPDRSIII (N = 905)p-Value
Age in years73.19 ± 7.6878.91 ± 8.65<0.001
Formal education in years15.62 ± 3.0515.06 ± 3.37<0.001
Sex (male/female)2578 (35.6%)/4666 (64.4%)353 (39.0%)/552 (61.0%)0.043
Race (White/African American/American Indian or Alaska Native/Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander/Asian/Other)5763 (79.6%)/1214 (16.8%)/31 (0.4%)/5 (0.1%)/173 (2.4%)/37 (0.5%)714 (78.9%)/151 (16.7%)/6 (0.7%)/1 (0.1%)/23 (2.5%)/6 (0.7%)<0.001
Cardiovascular disease (No/Yes)6454 (89.1%)/785 (10.8%)730 (80.7%)/171 (18.9%)<0.001
Cerebrovascular disease (No/Yes)6793 (93.8%)/417 (5.8%)779 (86.1%)/117 (12.9%)<0.001
Parkinson’s disease (No/Yes)7231 (99.8%)/12 (0.2%)824 (91.0%)/76 (8.4%)<0.001
Other Parkinsonian disorder (No/Yes)7226 (99.8%)/10 (0.1%)868 (95.9%)/33 (3.6%)<0.001
Traumatic brain injury (No/Yes)6570 (90.7%)/623 (8.6%)801 (88.5%)/98 (10.8%)0.026
History of seizures (No/Yes)7130 (98.4%)/103 (1.4%)881 (97.3%)/21 (2.3%)0.032
Thyroid disease (No/Yes)5849 (80.7%)/1357 (18.7%)716 (79.1%)/181 (20.0%)0.332
B12 deficiency (No/Yes)6866 (94.8%)/252 (3.5%)843 (93.1%)/41 (4.5%)0.101
Alcohol abuse (No/Yes)7049 (97.3%)/187 (2.6%)870 (96.1%)/32 (3.5%)0.092
Other substance abuse (No/Yes)7171 (99.0%)/57 (0.8%)898 (99.2%)/5 (0.6%)0.444
Use of antidepressants (No/Yes)6500 (89.7%)/744 (10.3%)784 (86.6%)/121 (13.4%)0.004
Episodic memory (sum of items recalled in the immediate and delayed recall tasks)25.48 ± 7.6223.44 ± 8.32<0.001
Language (sum of word production in the animals and vegetables lists)34.56 ± 8.4331.07 ± 8.35<0.001
Semantic Memory (BNT-30)27.02 ± 3.3125.73 ± 4.42<0.001
Attention (sum of longest sequences in DST forward and backward conditions)11.57 ± 1.9911.04 ± 2.11<0.001
Processing speed (TMT-A seconds)34.71 ± 15.4245.22 ± 23.00<0.001
Executive function (TMT-B seconds)91.47 ± 49.64120.67 ± 63.36<0.001
UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; BNT: Boston naming test; DST: digit span test; TMT-A: trails making test—part A; TMT-B: trails making test—part B; n = number of participants with available data per parameter. Abnormal UPDRS: if at least one of the UPDRS motor signs with value ≥ 2. Statistically significant values are given in bold.
Table 2. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and episodic memory.
Table 2. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and episodic memory.
UnadjustedAdjusted
Motor SignsB95% CIp-ValueB95% CI p-Value
LLUL LLUL
Hypophonia−3.366−6.237−0.4960.022−1.607−4.6051.3910.293
Masked Faces−2.917−4.932−0.9020.0050.031−2.3732.4360.98
Resting tremor−1.728−3.217−0.2390.023−0.747−2.3780.8840.369
Action/Postural Tremor−1.078−2.3760.220.1030.151−1.1781.480.824
Rigidity−0.973−2.1650.2190.111.003−0.3212.3260.138
Bradykinesia−2.435−3.304−1.565<0.000001−1.169−2.172−0.1670.022
Impaired Chair Rise−4.044−5.013−3.076<0.000001−2.092−3.155−1.030.000114
Impaired Posture/Gait−1.673−2.718−0.6270.0021.4210.2542.5870.017
Postural Instability−2.237−3.383−1.0910.000131−0.345−1.5280.8380.568
B, unstandardized correlation coefficient; CI, confidence interval; LL, lower limit; UL, upper limit. Adjusted analysis for age, years of education, sex, race, cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, other Parkinsonian disorder, traumatic brain injury, history of seizures and use of antidepressants, and motor signs. Statistically significant values are given in bold.
Table 3. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and language.
Table 3. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and language.
UnadjustedAdjusted
Motor SignsB95% CIp-ValueB95% CIp-Value
LLUL LLUL
Hypophonia−4.714−7.868−1.560.003−3.066−6.2310.0990.058
Masked Faces−2.826−5.06−0.5910.013−0.731−3.3041.8410.577
Resting tremor−1.808−3.437−0.1790.03−0.143−1.8511.5660.87
Action/Postural Tremor−2.126−3.551−0.7010.0030.227−1.1741.6280.751
Rigidity−1.92−3.226−0.6140.0040.189−1.2061.5850.79
Bradykinesia−4.191−5.142−3.239<0.000001−2.013−3.068−0.9580.000185
Impaired Chair Rise−5.198−6.259−4.137<0.000001−1.428−2.55−0.3060.013
Impaired Posture/Gait−3.637−4.786−2.487<0.0000010.872−0.3592.1040.165
Postural Instability−4.088−5.341−2.834<0.000001−0.632−1.8750.6110.319
B, unstandardized correlation coefficient; CI, confidence interval; LL, lower limit; UL, upper limit. Adjusted analysis for age, years of education, sex, race, cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, other Parkinsonian disorder, traumatic brain injury, history of seizures and use of antidepressants, and motor signs. Statistically significant values are given in bold.
Table 4. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and attention.
Table 4. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and attention.
UnadjustedAdjusted
Motor SignsB95% CI p-ValueB95% CI p-Value
LLUL LLUL
Hypophonia−0.418−1.1290.2930.249−0.456−1.2050.2920.232
Masked Faces0.081−0.4360.5970.760.394−0.2311.020.217
Resting tremor−0.243−0.6290.1440.218−0.15−0.5770.2770.491
Action/Postural Tremor−0.152−0.4880.1850.3780.131−0.2190.4810.462
Rigidity−0.238−0.5470.070.129−0.019−0.3670.3290.915
Bradykinesia−0.698−0.923−0.473<0.000001−0.408−0.673−0.1440.002
Impaired Chair Rise−0.86−1.112−0.607<0.000001−0.22−0.5010.0610.125
Impaired Posture/Gait−0.554−0.828−0.280.0000740.111−0.1990.4220.483
Postural Instability−0.822−1.118−0.526<0.000001−0.314−0.625−0.0030.048
B, unstandardized correlation coefficient; CI, confidence interval; LL, lower limit; UL, upper limit. Adjusted analysis for age, years of education, sex, race, cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, other Parkinsonian disorder, traumatic brain injury, history of seizures and use of antidepressants, and motor signs. Statistically significant values are given in bold.
Table 5. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and semantic memory.
Table 5. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and semantic memory.
UnadjustedAdjusted
Motor SignsB95% CI p-ValueB95% CI p-Value
LLUL LLUL
Hypophonia−0.491−1.7620.7790.448−0.945−2.2360.3470.152
Masked Faces0.792−0.1081.6910.0850.803−0.2451.8520.133
Resting tremor−0.099−0.770.5720.772−0.437−1.1490.2750.229
Action/Postural Tremor0.14−0.4420.7230.6360.6140.0351.1930.038
Rigidity−0.365−0.9040.1730.184−0.115−0.6960.4670.699
Bradykinesia−1.237−1.63−0.844<0.000001−0.557−0.996−0.1180.013
Impaired Chair Rise−2.38−2.816−1.944<0.000001−0.849−1.316−0.3830.000358
Impaired Posture/Gait−1.87−2.342−1.398<0.000001−0.569−1.082−0.0560.03
Postural Instability−1.63−2.147−1.112<0.000001−0.397−0.9190.1240.136
B, unstandardized correlation coefficient; CI, confidence interval; LL, lower limit; UL, upper limit. Adjusted analysis for age, years of education, sex, race, cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, other Parkinsonian disorder, traumatic brain injury, history of seizures and use of antidepressants, and motor signs. Statistically significant values are given in bold.
Table 6. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and processing speed.
Table 6. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and processing speed.
UnadjustedAdjusted
Motor SignsB95% CI p-ValueB95% CI p-Value
LLUL LLUL
Hypophonia12.7386.7318.7460.0000337.571.53913.6010.014
Masked Faces8.4324.06512.7990.0001552.93−2.0447.9040.248
Resting tremor7.954.72411.1750.0000014.6491.2848.0140.007
Action/Postural Tremor6.0133.1728.8530.0000341.247−1.5414.0340.381
Rigidity6.6534.0549.2520.0000010.19−2.5742.9530.893
Bradykinesia12.76210.86914.656<0.0000016.1944.0778.31<0.000001
Impaired Chair Rise16.40214.30118.502<0.0000016.284.0298.53<0.000001
Impaired Posture/Gait14.8812.58217.179<0.0000014.1791.7086.6490.001
Postural Instability12.149.63114.65<0.0000011.547−0.9574.0520.226
B, unstandardized correlation coefficient; CI, confidence interval; LL, lower limit; UL, upper limit. Adjusted analysis for age, years of education, sex, race, cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, other Parkinsonian disorder, traumatic brain injury, history of seizures and use of antidepressants, and motor signs. Statistically significant values are given in bold.
Table 7. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and executive function.
Table 7. Linear regression analysis for association between motor signs and executive function.
UnadjustedAdjusted
Motor SignsB95% CI p-ValueB95% CI p-Value
LLUL LLUL
Hypophonia36.53617.8255.2520.00013124.8946.72443.0640.007
Masked Faces16.5472.81930.2760.0184.372−10.82719.5710.573
Resting tremor10.9910.79721.1850.0351.647−8.62911.9220.753
Action/Postural Tremor12.1173.23221.0030.0081.013−7.4199.4450.814
Rigidity14.9636.74523.1820.00036−2.234−10.6836.2160.604
Bradykinesia32.35126.33738.366<0.00000112.6926.21419.1690.000124
Impaired Chair Rise50.44343.7357.156<0.00000117.8710.93324.807<0.000001
Impaired Posture/Gait40.86233.56548.159<0.00000110.0332.47917.5860.009
Postural Instability37.58429.62745.542<0.00000110.412.7618.060.008
B, unstandardized correlation coefficient; CI, confidence interval; LL, lower limit; UL, upper limit. Adjusted analysis for age, years of education, sex, race, cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s disease, other Parkinsonian disorder, traumatic brain injury, history of seizures and use of antidepressants, and motor signs. Statistically significant values are given in bold.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Siokas, V.; Liampas, I.; Lyketsos, C.G.; Dardiotis, E. Association between Motor Signs and Cognitive Performance in Cognitively Unimpaired Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study Using the NACC Database. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12101365

AMA Style

Siokas V, Liampas I, Lyketsos CG, Dardiotis E. Association between Motor Signs and Cognitive Performance in Cognitively Unimpaired Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study Using the NACC Database. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(10):1365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12101365

Chicago/Turabian Style

Siokas, Vasileios, Ioannis Liampas, Constantine G. Lyketsos, and Efthimios Dardiotis. 2022. "Association between Motor Signs and Cognitive Performance in Cognitively Unimpaired Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study Using the NACC Database" Brain Sciences 12, no. 10: 1365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12101365

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop