Next Article in Journal
Role of Oligodendrocytes and Myelin in the Pathophysiology of Autism Spectrum Disorder
Next Article in Special Issue
Evolution of the Autism Literature and the Influence of Parents: A Scientific Mapping in Web of Science
Previous Article in Journal
A Review of Machine Learning Methods of Feature Selection and Classification for Autism Spectrum Disorder
Previous Article in Special Issue
Emotional Availability and Play in Mother–Child Dyads with ASD: Changes during a Parental Based Intervention
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Parental Education, Household Income, Race, and Children’s Working Memory: Complexity of the Effects

by
Golnoush Akhlaghipour
1 and
Shervin Assari
2,3,*
1
Department of Neurology, University of California Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
2
Department of Family Medicine, Charles R Drew University of Medicine and Science, Los Angeles, CA 90059, USA
3
Department of Urban Public Health, Charles R Drew University of Medicine and Science, Los Angeles, CA 90059, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2020, 10(12), 950; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120950
Submission received: 7 October 2020 / Revised: 1 November 2020 / Accepted: 4 December 2020 / Published: 7 December 2020

Abstract

:
Background. Considerable research has linked social determinants of health (SDoHs) such as race, parental education, and household income to school performance, and these effects may be in part due to working memory. However, a growing literature shows that these effects may be complex: while the effects of parental education may be diminished for Blacks than Whites, household income may explain such effects. Purpose. Considering race as sociological rather than a biological construct (race as a proxy of racism) and built on Minorities’ Diminished Returns (MDRs), this study explored complexities of the effects of SDoHs on children’s working memory. Methods. We borrowed data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. The total sample was 10,418, 9- and 10-year-old children. The independent variables were race, parental education, and household income. The primary outcome was working memory measured by the NIH Toolbox Card Sorting Test. Age, sex, ethnicity, and parental marital status were the covariates. To analyze the data, we used mixed-effect regression models. Results. High parental education and household income were associated with higher and Black race was associated with lower working memory. The association between high parental education but not household income was less pronounced for Black than White children. This differential effect of parental education on working memory was explained by household income. Conclusions. For American children, parental education generates unequal working memory, depending on race. This means parental education loses some of its expected effects for Black families. It also suggests that while White children with highly educated parents have the highest working memory, Black children report lower working memory, regardless of their parental education. This inequality is mainly because of differential income in highly educated White and Black families. This finding has significant public policy and economic implications and suggests we need to do far more than equalizing education to eliminate racial inequalities in children’s cognitive outcomes. While there is a need for multilevel policies that reduce the effect of racism and social stratification for middle-class Black families, equalizing income may have more returns than equalizing education.

Graphical Abstract

1. Background

Working memory is believed to be a core element of human cognition. Baddeley’s original work on the multiple-component model [1] and executive function conceptualizes working memory as people’s ability to bring information “online”, which is core to thinking and thought. Working memory is our ability to hold information in short-term memory and maintain the required information “in mind” while processing them [2]. Over the past decades, working memory has received much scientific interest, which has resulted in a large body of empirical evidence. This research suggests that working memory is essential for cognitive tasks, math ability, and school performance. Working memory is closely associated with executive functioning [3] and is mainly performed in higher cortical areas, especially the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [4]. Because of the close correlation with cognitive tasks, working memory is believed to be the primary determinant of children’s educational success [5,6,7,8].
With cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control, working memory is a part of the executive function, also called cognitive control [9,10]. Executive function refers to the top-down neurocognitive processes involved in the conscious, goal-directed control of thought, action, and emotions. Effective executive function and functional working memory are both reliant upon the integrity of neural networks involving the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the anterior cingulate cortex, and other regions [11,12,13], and they are required for keeping the information in mind, attending selectively, ignoring distractions, and solving problems flexibly [14].
High socioeconomic status (SES) is associated with better school performance [15] and working memory [16,17,18]. These are in line with the overall positive effects of high SES on childhood cognition, emotions, and behaviors [19]. For example, children from higher SES families are less likely to show school drop-out [20] and emotional [21,22,23] and behavioral problems [24,25]. These SES effects are non-specific and are attributed to the protective effects of resources and lower stress in childhood.
The SES–memory/health scarcity hypothesis can be seen through SES effects on healthy children’s brain development. According to the scarcity hypothesis, low SES is a proxy of early adversity, stress, economic insecurity, and lack of resources, increasing the risk of low child development. In this view, stress, adversity, and scarce resources explain the SES–brain development link [26]. Low parental education and household income are proxies of living in stressful environments, food insecurity, environmental toxins, and parental risk behaviors that can jeopardize healthy brain development in children [27,28,29]. As a result of inadequate brain development, children from low SES are at an increased risk of poor memory, emotion regulation, learning disorders, and psychopathology [30,31,32]. In contrast, children from high SES backgrounds experience less stress and have more access to stimulating environments and better parenting [33,34,35].
According to the Minorities’ Diminished Returns (MDRs) [36,37], Black and White families differ in the protective effect of high SES on health outcomes. Compared to their White counterparts, Black children show lower parental education effects on a wide range of developmental outcomes [38], such as school performance [38], mental health [39], emotion regulation [40,41], aggression [42], and substance use [39,43]. While income may also generate differential effects for Black and White children [44], most research has shown that parental education generates fewer Blacks outcomes than Whites [45,46,47,48,49].
MDRs are not due to behaviors or personalities but societal barriers. For Black families, high SES increases vulnerability to the effect of discrimination, meaning that if discrimination occurs, it is more likely to result in depression [50]. High SES is also associated with high discrimination, which is partly due to the increased proximity of high SES Blacks to Whites [51,52]. High SES Black families experience higher discrimination because they are at higher proximity to White people [51,52,53,54]. The positive link between SES and discrimination [51,52,55,56,57,58] reduces high SES Black families’ health. As a result, SES effects are weaker for Blacks than Whites.
Research has established racial/ethnic differences in each SES indicator’s role in children’s brain development [33,59,60,61]. In several studies, the magnitude of parental education’s effects on a wide range of developmental and health outcomes is weaker for Blacks than Whites [49,55,58,62,63,64,65,66]. As a result of MDRs, middle-class ethnic minority children remain at risk for poor developmental and health outcomes [45,67,68,69,70]. For example, high SES Black children remain at risk of anxiety [71], depression [44], poor health [62], poor school performance [72,73], and high-risk behaviors [45] such as aggression [45] and tobacco use [74,75]. Differential effects of SES across racial and ethnic groups of children are robust [49,62,63,76,77]. Data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS) shows that high parental education and family income is associated with better outcomes in impulsivity, school performance, school bonding, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), obesity, aggression, depression, and self-rated health for White children than Black American children [69,77,78,79]. Subjective SES and parental education each impact brain imaging findings in a certain way [33,59,60,61]. Various SES indicators may also be the underlying mechanisms by which racial and ethnic disparities emerge in children’s development [49,62,80].
At least some of the effects of high SES on school performance [81] can be attributed to the role of family SES on structure and function of the brain [33] and SES effect on memory [82,83,84]. Many brain structures, such as the amygdala [33,60,85], hippocampus [86], and PFC, carry the effects of SES on cognitive and behavioral outcomes. The amygdala is more involved in emotion regulation [87,88,89,90,91], while the hippocampus [86] and PFC [92,93,94] are more involved in cognitive tasks, executive function, and memory.

Aims

To investigate the complexities of social determinants of children’s brain development in the US, we explored racial variation in the effects of two family SES indicators, namely parental education and household income, on working memory among 9- and 10-year-old children. We expected racial differences in the magnitude of the association between parental education on working memory, in line with the observed MDRs [36,37,45]. More specifically, we expected the weaker effects of parental education on working memory for Black than White children. This expectation is in line with the other research on a wide range of phenotypes and behaviors [36,37,45].

2. Methods

2.1. Design and Settings

This secondary analysis was a cross-sectional analysis of the baseline data of the ABCD study [95,96,97,98,99]. ABCD is a national, state-of-the-art brain imaging study of childhood brain development [95,100].

2.2. Participants and Sampling

The ABCD study sample was recruited from 21 cities across states. ABCD sampling was primarily through school systems. For sampling in the ABCD study, school selection was informed by race, ethnicity, sex, SES, and urbanicity [101]. Inclusion criteria were having data on our variables. Participants could be included regardless of race or ethnicity (n = 10,418). As this is a general population study of children, participants have been enrolled regardless of their psychopathologies. That means participants were not included or excluded from the sample based on the presence of psychopathology.

2.3. Study Variables

2.3.1. Primary Outcome

The primary outcome was working memory, measured by NIH Toolbox, the Dimensional Change Card Sort [102]. This measure [103] has shown high reliability and validity [9,104]. The NIH Toolbox card-sorting test is a part of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB). This measure evaluates the executive function. The NIHTB-CB is designed for use in epidemiologic studies and clinical trials for ages 3 to 85. Some studies have documented very acceptable psychometric properties of the NIHTB-CB and card sort test. These are computer-based instruments assessing executive function: the Dimensional Change Card Sort, which measures cognitive flexibility, and a flanker task, which measures inhibitory control and selective attention. These measures show convergent and discriminant validity and correlate with SES. These measures also show excellent sensitivity to age-related changes during adulthood, excellent test-retest reliability. As a result, the Dimensional Change Card Sort can be used effectively in epidemiologic and clinical studies. Our outcome was a continuous variable in this study, with a higher score indicating higher cognitive flexibility [103,105].

2.3.2. Independent Variable

Parental Educational Attainment. Participants reported their years of schooling. This variable was operationalized as a five-level nominal variable: less than a high school diploma, high school diploma, some college, bachelor degree, graduate studies.
Household Income. Parents reported their overall annual income. This was a three-level nominal variable: <$50,000, $50,000–$100,000, and $100,000+.

2.3.3. Moderator

Race. Race was reported by parents, and operationalized as a nominal variable: Black, Asian, Other/Mixed, and White (reference group).

2.3.4. Confounders

Ethnicity. Parents were asked if they were of Latino ethnic background. This variable was coded as Latino = 1 and non-Latino = 0.
Age. Age was a dichotomous variable coded 1 or 0 for 10 years and 9 years of age. Parents reported the age of the children.
Sex. Sex was 1 for males and 0 for females.
Parental marital status. Parental marital status was 1 for married and 0 for any other condition (reference).

2.4. Data Analysis

We used SPSS for data analysis. Frequencies (n and %) and mean [standard deviations (SDs)] were reported for descriptive purposes. To estimate bivariate associations between the study variables, we used the Chi-square and Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) test in the pooled sample. To perform our multivariable analyses, we performed mixed-effects regressions. First, we tested the assumptions. We excluded collinearity between the study variables. We also tested the distribution of our outcome and error terms and quantiles (Figure 1). We ran six models. All models were performed in the pooled sample. Model 1 to Model 3 did not have interaction effects. Model 1 had education but not income. Model 2 had income but not education. Model 3 had both education and income. Model 4 had interactive effects of education and race but not income. Model 5 had interactive effects of income and race. Model 6 had interactive effects of race and education and also controlled for income. Box 1 lists our model formulas. Unstandardized regression coefficient (b), standard error (SE), and p-values were reported for each model. A p-value of equal or less than 0.05 was significant.
Box 1. Model Formula.
Model 1
nihtbx_cardsort_agecorrected ~ race.4level + sex + age + married.bl + hisp + high.educ.bl
Model 2
nihtbx_cardsort_agecorrected ~ + race.4level + sex + age + married.bl + hisp + household.income.bl
Model 3
nihtbx_cardsort_agecorrected ~ + race.4level + sex + age + married.bl + hisp + high.educ.bl + household.income.bl
Model 4
nihtbx_cardsort_agecorrected ~ + race.4level + sex + age + married.bl + hisp + high.educ.bl + high.educ.bl × race.4level
Model 5
nihtbx_cardsort_agecorrected ~ + race.4level + sex + age + married.bl + hisp + high.educ.bl + high.educ.bl × race.4level + household.income.bl
Model 6
nihtbx_cardsort_agecorrected~ race.4level + sex + age + married.bl + hisp + high.educ.bl + household.income.bl + high.educ.bl × race.4level
Random: ~(1|abcd_site/rel_family_id)

2.5. Ethical Aspect

Our analysis was exempt from a full review. However, the ABCD study protocol was approved by the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) Institutional Review Board (IRB) [100].

3. Results

3.1. Descriptives

The sample included 10,418 9- and 10-year-old children. Our participants were White (n = 6897; 66.2%), Black (n = 1515; 14.5%), Asian (n = 234; 2.2%), or other/mixed race (n = 1768; 17.0%). Card sorting was significantly different across racial groups. While Asian and White children had the highest card sorting scores, Black children scored worst in card sorting (Table 1).

3.2. Regression Results

Table 2 and Table 3 report the results of six pooled sample mixed-effects regression models. All models are significant. Effect sizes as shown in Table 2. Model 1, which only included the main effect of race and parental education and covariates, showed that high parental education is associated with higher working memory. Model 2 showed that high income is associated with higher working memory. Model 3 showed that parental education and household income have both associations with working memory. Model 4 showed that parental education and race interact, meaning that parental education’s boosting effect on working memory was less pronounced for Black than White children. Model 5 did not show an interaction between race and household income. Model 6 showed that household income explains why parental education and race interact with our outcome. (Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5).

4. Discussion

This study had three primary findings. Although higher parental education and household income predicted higher working memory (1st finding), parental education and household income showed weaker effects on Black children than White children’s working memory (2nd finding). Third, income differentials (lower income levels of Black families with the same parental education and family structure) explain why parental education shows a weaker effect on working memory for Black than White children (3rd finding).
Our first results can be compared with the literature on the protective effects of high SES on children’s cognitive outcomes [106]. A large body of literature has also documented poor educational outcomes in low SES than high SES children [107,108,109,110]. High SES is associated with better school performance [15] and working memory [16,17,18]. Literature has shown the effects of family SES indicators such as poverty and household income on the brain [111] and behavior [33,59,60,61,112]. The SES–health link may be because SES is a proxy of stress, adversities, trauma. Thus, many brain structures such as PFC, hippocampus, and amygdala [85,113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120,121,122] correlate with SES. The prefrontal cortex [33], hippocampus [86], and amygdala [123] have been shown to be under the influence of trauma. SES impacts reduced connectivity in neural networks involved in memory and emotion regulation [124]. In a recent study by Brody et al., data of 119 African American youths living in the rural South were used. The study measured poverty status and supportive parenting at ages 11–13 and 16–18. The study conducted brain imaging at age 25. This study applied resting-state fMRI to study two brain networks’ functional connectivity: (1) central-executive and (2) emotion-regulation. The authors found that more years spent in poverty was associated with lower levels of connectivity in both neural networks; however, this was more robust among young adults who received low levels of supportive parenting. The study did not show an effect of income on connectivity in the presence of high levels of positive and supportive parenting [124]. In a study that analyzed data from a prospective longitudinal study of emotion development showed that lower income-to-needs ratio at preschool age was associated with reduced connectivity between hippocampus and amygdala and several regions at school age, including the cortex, lingual gyrus, posterior cingulate, and putamen. This study included preschoolers 3–5 years of age selected from the St. Louis area. Participants were followed for up to 12 years. Individuals underwent annual behavioral assessments. Participants also underwent neuroimaging at school age to measure brain resting-state functional connectivity with the left and right hippocampus and amygdala. This study showed that a lower income-to-needs ratio predicted a greater connectivity between the left hippocampus and the right superior frontal cortex and between the right amygdala and the right lingual gyrus. As this study showed, brain functional connectivity mediated the relationship between SES and depression [125]. Thus, low SES predicts reduced connectivity between the amygdala and hippocampus with brain regions, including the lingual gyrus, superior frontal cortex, posterior cingulate, as well as putamen [125]. Social adversities have cumulative (additive) effects on brain structures and functions that govern emotion regulation [85] and memory [126]; however, these effects may differ across demographic groups [60]. In part, these are due to parents’ health [127] and behaviors [128].
While our first finding documented a link between SES and working memory, our second suggested that this effect differed across demographic groups. Most of the literature on the effects of race, parental education, and income on memory have focused on additive rather than multiplicative effects of SES indicators and race. Our second finding can also be seen as a reflection of the MDRs. Many studies have shown more significant effects of SES on outcomes for White than Black American children [49,62,129]. For example, family SES has shown larger effects on ADHD [79], anxiety [71], aggression [45], tobacco dependence [45], school bonding [130], school performance [73,131], obesity [69], and health [68] for White than Black American children.
As a result of our second finding, parental education shows a more salient role in shaping White’s impulsivity than Black American children [67]. As a result of this pattern, higher than expected risk of poor self-rated health, obesity, poor mental health, chronic disease, impulsivity, aggression, smoking, and low school performance are observed in high SES Black American children [69,77,79]. These patterns are also called MDRs and seem robust as they hold across SES indicators, outcomes, population groups, birth cohorts, age groups, and settings [36,37]. The findings observed in this analysis, however, did not support MDRs.
As shown by this study and previous works [36], family SES differently influences Black and White children’s outcomes [76,132], children [54], adults [133], and older adults [134,135]. A society is equal only if parental education [45], educational attainment [74,78,136], employment [137], marital status [63], and coping [138,139] generate equal outcomes for Blacks and Whites. Parental education seems to generate unequal effects for Blacks and Whites, a pattern that indicates inequality due to social stratification, segregation, and racism [140,141,142,143,144,145,146,147,148,149].
MDRs, differential effects of parental education across racial groups, maybe due to racial discrimination in high SES Black families. Racial and ethnic discrimination affect the amygdala’s structure and function [150,151,152,153,154,155]. In a study in the US, 74 adults (43% women; 72% African American; 23% Hispanic; 32% homosexual/bisexual) reported their discrimination experience. The study also measured spontaneous amygdala activity and functional connectivity between the amygdala and other brain regions during resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). In this study, greater experience of discrimination was associated with an increased level of spontaneous amygdala activity. Similarly, an increase in discrimination was associated with stronger functional connectivity between the amygdala and several neural regions such as the anterior insula, putamen, caudate, anterior cingulate, medial frontal gyrus. The most robust effect of discrimination was seen for the connectivity between the amygdala and thalamus [156]. As high SES, particularly high subjective SES, is a proxy of high not low discrimination [50,51,52,53,55,56,57,58,137], high SES Black American children still report lower than expected brain and behavior outcomes because of the effect of discrimination.
Differential effects of family SES indicators for Black and White families contribute to the transgenerational transmission of inequalities [45,67,68,69,70]. Differential effects of SES mean the same level of SES may generate unequal outcomes for the next generation, which results in the reproduction of inequalities across generations. However, most of the previous studies on MDRs have relied on self-reported outcomes. Thus, the evidence lacked biological studies that test the differential effects of SES on children’s brain imaging. This paper documented complex, non-linear, multiplicative effects of SES, and race on working memory.
The observed MDRs suggest that Black American children suffer from three jeopardies. The first risk is that they live in low-SES families. The second risk is that they have worse outcomes (working memory in this study). The third jeopardy is their SES shows a weaker impact on their brain development. The weakened effect of SES for Black children suggests that it is very difficult to improve the health outcomes and close the Black-White gaps. Policymakers should not expect drastic effects as a result of their interventions. These diminishing returns are likely to be due to unique stressors in Black people’s lives across all SES levels.
It should be emphasized that we see race as a social factor (as a proxy of social status, treatment by society, access to the opportunity structure, interpersonal discrimination, environmental injustices, societal obstacles, and historical injustice) on how people are treated by society. As our results suggested, race alters the implications of family SES for working memory not because Blacks are inferior or different than Whites, but because society has historically oppressed them and continues to discriminate against them. All these injustices take a toll in terms of health and development.
Our paper was on complex and multiplicative effects of social determinants on 9- and 10-year-old American children. Working memory is closely associated with executive functioning and is mainly performed in higher cortical areas, especially the prefrontal cortex (PFC) [157,158,159]. It is recognized that the PFC is one of the last regions of the brain to mature [157]. During preadolescence, the increase in gray matter volume is observed, especially in the PFC, among other frontal lobe regions [157]. Furthermore, it is recognized that females’ brains develop about, on average two years earlier than male brains, so they are more likely to have a late developing male brain than females [157].

Limitations

All studies have some methodological and conceptual limitations. This study, which was a secondary analysis of existing data, is not an exception to this rule. Our first limitation was a cross-sectional analysis. As a result, we can only conclude associations, not causal effects. Our second limitation was a lack of inclusion of many confounders such as psychopathologies, learning disabilities, or physical health. Third, all our SES measures were reported by parents. Some measurement bias should be expected in the measurement of SES in this study. Also, the sample was not balanced regarding race and SES. Racial groups were also not comparable in their SES. Finally, brain development is behind in male than female children. This study, however, did not explore race by sex differences in social determinants of working memory. Built on intersectionality, future research may explore how groups based on the intersection of race, sex, SES, and place differ in the effects of SDoH on memory function.

5. Conclusions

Among American children, high parental education and household income correlate with better working memory. However, the effect of parental education is unequal across racial groups, with the marginal return of parental education being smaller for Black families than White people. Income, however, generates a similar outcome for Black and White families. Finally, parental education generates less outcome than the differential income of highly educated Black and White families. These findings have policy solutions for achieving equality. First, the solution to racial gaps lies beyond closing the SES inequalities. We should address barriers that interfere with the SES from generating equal outcomes for Blacks. Equalizing income may be a more effective way of equalizing outcomes than equalizing education, because more processes can interfere with the return of education than income. More research is needed on how we can equalize Black and White families for the effect of SES on brain development. Social determinants’ influence on children’s brain development is complicated and multiplicative rather than simple and additive. Moderated mediation and mediated moderation models are more realistic than simple additive models.

Author Contributions

G.A.: Conceptual design, first draft, and revision. S.A.: Conceptual design, data analysis, supervision, and revision. Both authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Author Funding: Assari received support from the following NIH grants: 2U54MD007598, U54 TR001627; CA201415-02, 5S21MD000103, R25 MD007610, 4P60MD006923, and 54MD008149. ABCD Funding: The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, U24DA041147, U01DA041093, and U01DA041025. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/Consortium_Members.pdf. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The current paper used the Curated Annual Release 2.0, also defined in NDA Study 634 (doi:10.15154/1503209).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Baddeley, A.D.; Logie, R.H. Working Memory: The Multiple-Component Model; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baddeley, A.D.; Hitch, G. Working memory. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1974; Volume 8, pp. 47–89. [Google Scholar]
  3. Gathercole, S.E.; Baddeley, A.D. Working Memory and Language; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  4. Braver, T.S.; Cohen, J.D.; Nystrom, L.E.; Jonides, J.; Smith, E.E.; Noll, D.C. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage 1997, 5, 49–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Vandenbroucke, L.; Verschueren, K.; Desoete, A.; Aunio, P.; Ghesquière, P.; Baeyens, D. Crossing the bridge to elementary school: The development of children’s working memory components in relation to teacher-student relationships and academic achievement. Early Child. Res. Q. 2018, 42, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Owens, M.; Stevenson, J.; Hadwin, J.A.; Norgate, R. Anxiety and depression in academic performance: An exploration of the mediating factors of worry and working memory. Sch. Psychol. Int. 2012, 33, 433–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Dehn, M.J. Working Memory and Academic Learning: Assessment and Intervention; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  8. Lara, A.H.; Wallis, J.D. The role of prefrontal cortex in working memory: A mini review. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Zelazo, P.D.; Anderson, J.E.; Richler, J.; Wallner-Allen, K.; Beaumont, J.L.; Conway, K.P.; Gershon, R.; Weintraub, S. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): Validation of executive function measures in adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS 2014, 20, 620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Zelazo, P.D. The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nat. Protoc. 2006, 1, 297–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Miller, E.K.; Cohen, J.D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 24, 167–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Zelazo, P.; Muller, U.; Goswami, U. Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development; Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  13. Rosas, R.; Espinoza, V.; Porflitt, F.; Ceric, F. Executive functions can be improved in preschoolers through systematic playing in educational settings: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cowan, N. Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. Psychol. Bull. 1988, 104, 163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Alexander, K.L.; Entwisle, D.R.; Bedinger, S.D. When expectations work: Race and socioeconomic differences in school performance. Soc. Psychol. Q. 1994, 57, 283–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Engel, P.M.J.; Santos, F.H.; Gathercole, S.E. Are working memory measures free of socioeconomic influence? J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2008, 51, 1580–1587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Sturge-Apple, M.L.; Suor, J.H.; Skibo, M.A. Maternal child-centered attributions and harsh discipline: The moderating role of maternal working memory across socioeconomic contexts. J. Fam. Psychol. 2014, 28, 645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Hackman, D.A.; Betancourt, L.M.; Gallop, R.; Romer, D.; Brodsky, N.L.; Hurt, H.; Farah, M.J. Mapping the trajectory of socioeconomic disparity in working memory: Parental and neighborhood factors. Child Dev. 2014, 85, 1433–1445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Oshri, A.; Hallowell, E.; Liu, S.; MacKillop, J.; Galvan, A.; Kogan, S.M.; Sweet, L.H. Socioeconomic hardship and delayed reward discounting: Associations with working memory and emotional reactivity. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019, 37, 100642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Sirin, S.R. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Rev. Educ. Res. 2005, 75, 417–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Mendelson, T.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Datta, G.D.; Buka, S.L. Relation of female gender and low socioeconomic status to internalizing symptoms among adolescents: A case of double jeopardy? Soc. Sci. Med. 2008, 66, 1284–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Yildiz, M.; Demirhan, E.; Gurbuz, S. Contextual socioeconomic disadvantage and adolescent suicide attempts: A multilevel investigation. J. Youth Adolesc. 2019, 48, 802–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Eisenberg, D.; Gollust, S.E.; Golberstein, E.; Hefner, J.L. Prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety, and suicidality among university students. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2007, 77, 534–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Kaleta, D.; Usidame, B.; Dziankowska-Zaborszczyk, E.; Makowiec-Dabrowska, T. Socioeconomic disparities in age of initiation and ever tobacco smoking: Findings from Romania. Cent. Eur. J. Public Health 2015, 23, 299–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Barreto, S.M.; de Figueiredo, R.C.; Giatti, L. Socioeconomic inequalities in youth smoking in Brazil. BMJ Open 2013, 3, e003538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Yaple, Z.A.; Yu, R. Functional and structural brain correlates of socioeconomic status. Cereb. Cortex 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Assari, S.; Bazargan, M. Unequal associations between educational attainment and occupational stress across racial and ethnic groups. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Assari, S.; Bazargan, M. Second-hand exposure home second-hand smoke exposure at home in the united states; Minorities’ diminished returns. Int. J. Travel Med. Glob. Health 2019, 7, 135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Assari, S.; Bazargan, M. Unequal effects of educational attainment on workplace exposure to second-hand smoke by race and ethnicity; Minorities’ diminished returns in the national health interview survey (NHIS). J. Med. Res. Innov. 2019, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Chassin, L.; Presson, C.C.; Sherman, S.J.; Edwards, D.A. Parent educational attainment and adolescent cigarette smoking. J. Subst. Abus. 1992, 4, 219–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kocaoglu, B.; Moschonis, G.; Dimitriou, M.; Kolotourou, M.; Keskin, Y.; Sur, H.; Hayran, O.; Manios, Y. Parental educational level and cardiovascular disease risk factors in schoolchildren in large urban areas of Turkey: Directions for public health policy. BMC Public Health 2005, 5, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Padilla-Moledo, C.; Ruiz, J.R.; Castro-Pinero, J. Parental educational level and psychological positive health and health complaints in Spanish children and adolescents. Child. Care Health Dev. 2016, 42, 534–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Javanbakht, A.; King, A.P.; Evans, G.W.; Swain, J.E.; Angstadt, M.; Phan, K.L.; Liberzon, I. Childhood poverty predicts adult amygdala and frontal activity and connectivity in response to emotional faces. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Masten, C.L.; Telzer, E.H.; Eisenberger, N.I. An FMRI investigation of attributing negative social treatment to racial discrimination. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 1042–1051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Wu, X.; Zou, Q.; Hu, J.; Tang, W.; Mao, Y.; Gao, L.; Zhu, J.; Jin, Y.; Wu, X.; Lu, L.; et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity patterns predict consciousness level and recovery outcome in acquired brain injury. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 12932–12946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Assari, S. Unequal gain of equal resources across racial groups. Int. J. Health Policy Manag. 2017, 7, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Assari, S. Health disparities due to diminished return among black Americans: Public policy solutions. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2018, 12, 112–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Spera, C.; Wentzel, K.R.; Matto, H.C. Parental aspirations for their children’s educational attainment: Relations to ethnicity, parental education, children’s academic performance, and parental perceptions of school climate. J. Youth Adolesc. 2009, 38, 1140–1152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Goodman, E.; Slap, G.B.; Huang, B. The public health impact of socioeconomic status on adolescent depression and obesity. Am. J. Public Health 2003, 93, 1844–1850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Morris, A.S.; Silk, J.S.; Steinberg, L.; Myers, S.S.; Robinson, L.R. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc. Dev. 2007, 16, 361–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Park, S.; Holloway, S.D. No parent left behind: Predicting parental involvement in adolescents’ education within a sociodemographically diverse population. J. Educ. Res. 2013, 106, 105–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Pabayo, R.; Molnar, B.E.; Kawachi, I. The role of neighborhood income inequality in adolescent aggression and violence. J. Adolesc. Health 2014, 55, 571–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Wills, T.A.; McNamara, G.; Vaccaro, D. Parental education related to adolescent stress-coping and substance use: Development of a mediational model. Health Psychol. 1995, 14, 464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H. High risk of depression in high-income African American boys. J. Racial Ethn. Health Dispar. 2018, 5, 808–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Bazargan, M. Association between parental educational attainment and youth outcomes and role of race/ethnicity. JAMA Netw. Open 2019, 2, e1916018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Assari, S.; Boyce, S.; Bazargan, M.; Caldwell, C.H. Diminished Returns of Parental Education in Terms of Youth School Performance: Ruling out Regression toward the Mean. Children 2020, 7, 74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Assari, S. Parental education and spanking of American children: Blacks’ diminished returns. World J. Educ. Res. 2020, 7, 19–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Assari, S. American Children’s Screen Time: Diminished Returns of Household Income in Black Families. Information 2020, 11, 538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Assari, S. Parental education attainment and educational upward mobility; Role of race and gender. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Assari, S.; Preiser, B.; Lankarani, M.M.; Caldwell, C.H. Subjective socioeconomic status moderates the association between discrimination and depression in African American youth. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Assari, S. Does School racial composition explain why high income black youth perceive more discrimination? A gender analysis. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Assari, S.; Moghani Lankarani, M. Workplace racial composition explains high perceived discrimination of high socioeconomic status African American men. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Assari, S.; Gibbons, F.X.; Simons, R.L. Perceived discrimination among black youth: An 18-year longitudinal study. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Assari, S.; Gibbons, F.X.; Simons, R. Depression among black youth; Interaction of class and place. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Hudson, D.L.; Bullard, K.M.; Neighbors, H.W.; Geronimus, A.T.; Yang, J.; Jackson, J.S. Are benefits conferred with greater socioeconomic position undermined by racial discrimination among African American men? J. Mens. Health 2012, 9, 127–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Hudson, D.L.; Puterman, E.; Bibbins-Domingo, K.; Matthews, K.A.; Adler, N.E. Race, life course socioeconomic position, racial discrimination, depressive symptoms and self-rated health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013, 97, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Hudson, D.L.; Neighbors, H.W.; Geronimus, A.T.; Jackson, J.S. Racial discrimination, John Henryism, and depression among African Americans. J. Black Psychol. 2016, 42, 221–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Assari, S.; Lankarani, M.M.; Caldwell, C.H. Does discrimination explain high risk of depression among high-income African American men? Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. D’Angiulli, A.; Lipina, S.J.; Olesinska, A. Explicit and implicit issues in the developmental cognitive neuroscience of social inequality. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Javanbakht, A.; Kim, P.; Swain, J.E.; Evans, G.W.; Phan, K.L.; Liberzon, I. Sex-specific effects of childhood poverty on neurocircuitry of processing of emotional cues: A neuroimaging study. Behav. Sci. 2016, 6, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Kim, P.; Evans, G.W.; Angstadt, M.; Ho, S.S.; Sripada, C.S.; Swain, J.E.; Liberzon, I.; Phan, K.L. Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 18442–18447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Assari, S. Parental educational attainment and mental well-being of college students; Diminished returns of blacks. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Assari, S. Race, intergenerational social mobility and stressful life events. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Fuller-Rowell, T.E.; Doan, S.N. The social costs of academic success across ethnic groups. Child. Dev. 2010, 81, 1696–1713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Fuller-Rowell, T.E.; Curtis, D.S.; Doan, S.N.; Coe, C.L. Racial disparities in the health benefits of educational attainment: A study of inflammatory trajectories among African American and white adults. Psychosom. Med. 2015, 77, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Hudson, D.L.; Neighbors, H.W.; Geronimus, A.T.; Jackson, J.S. The relationship between socioeconomic position and depression among a US nationally representative sample of African Americans. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2012, 47, 373–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Mincy, R. Family socioeconomic status at birth and youth impulsivity at age 15; Blacks’ diminished return. Child 2018, 5, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Mincy, R.B. Maternal educational attainment at birth promotes future self-rated health of white but not black youth: A 15-year cohort of a national sample. J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7, 93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Assari, S.; Thomas, A.; Caldwell, C.H.; Mincy, R.B. Blacks’ diminished health return of family structure and socioeconomic status; 15 years of follow-up of a national urban sample of youth. J. Urban Health 2018, 95, 21–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Assari, S.; Boyce, S.; Bazargan, M.; Mincy, R.; Caldwell, C.H. Unequal protective effects of parental educational attainment on the body mass index of black and white youth. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Zimmerman, M.A. Family structure and subsequent anxiety symptoms; Minorities’ diminished return. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Assari, S. Mental Rotation in American Children: Diminished Returns of Parental Education in Black Families. Pediatr. Rep. 2020, 12, 130–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H. Parental educational attainment differentially boosts school performance of American adolescents: Minorities’ diminished returns. J. Fam. Reprod. Health 2019, 13, 7–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Assari, S.; Mistry, R. Educational attainment and smoking status in a national sample of American adults; Evidence for the blacks’ diminished return. Int. J. Env. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Assari, S.; Mistry, R.; Bazargan, M. Race, educational attainment, and e-cigarette use. J. Med. Res. Innov. 2020, 4, e000185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Assari, S.; Moghani Lankarani, M. Poverty status and childhood asthma in white and black families: National survey of children’s health. Healthcare 2018, 6, 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  77. Assari, S. Multiplicative effects of social and psychological risk factors on college students’ suicidal behaviors. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  78. Assari, S. Socioeconomic status and self-rated oral health; Diminished return among Hispanic whites. Dent. J. 2018, 6, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  79. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H. Family income at birth and risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder at age 15: Racial differences. Children 2019, 6, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  80. Assari, S. Parental education better helps white than black families escape poverty: National survey of children’s health. Economies 2018, 6, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Sackett, P.R.; Kuncel, N.R.; Arneson, J.J.; Cooper, S.R.; Waters, S.D. Does socioeconomic status explain the relationship between admissions tests and post-secondary academic performance? Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Brito, N.H.; Fifer, W.P.; Myers, M.M.; Elliott, A.J.; Noble, K.G. Associations among family socioeconomic status, EEG power at birth, and cognitive skills during infancy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 19, 144–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Last, B.S.; Lawson, G.M.; Breiner, K.; Steinberg, L.; Farah, M.J. Childhood socioeconomic status and executive function in childhood and beyond. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0202964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Moorman, S.M.; Carr, K.; Greenfield, E.A. Childhood socioeconomic status and genetic risk for poorer cognition in later life. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018, 212, 219–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Evans, G.W.; Swain, J.E.; King, A.P.; Wang, X.; Javanbakht, A.; Ho, S.S.; Angstadt, M.; Phan, K.L.; Xie, H.; Liberzon, I. Childhood cumulative risk exposure and adult amygdala volume and function. J. Neurosci. Res. 2016, 94, 535–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Assari, S.; Boyce, S.; Bazargan, M.; Caldwell, C.H. Family income mediates the effect of parental education on adolescents’ hippocampus activation during an n-back memory task. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Dotterer, H.L.; Hyde, L.W.; Swartz, J.R.; Hariri, A.R.; Williamson, D.E. Amygdala reactivity predicts adolescent antisocial behavior but not callous-unemotional traits. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 24, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Gard, A.M.; Waller, R.; Swartz, J.R.; Shaw, D.S.; Forbes, E.E.; Hyde, L.W. Amygdala functional connectivity during socioemotional processing prospectively predicts increases in internalizing symptoms in a sample of low-income, urban, young men. Neuroimage 2018, 178, 562–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Morawetz, C.; Bode, S.; Baudewig, J.; Heekeren, H.R. Effective amygdala-prefrontal connectivity predicts individual differences in successful emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017, 12, 569–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Szczepanik, J.; Nugent, A.C.; Drevets, W.C.; Khanna, A.; Zarate, C.A., Jr.; Furey, M.L. Amygdala response to explicit sad face stimuli at baseline predicts antidepressant treatment response to scopolamine in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2016, 254, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Venta, A.; Sharp, C.; Patriquin, M.; Salas, R.; Newlin, E.; Curtis, K.; Baldwin, P.; Fowler, C.; Frueh, B.C. Amygdala-frontal connectivity predicts internalizing symptom recovery among inpatient adolescents. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 225, 453–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Lei, Y.; Su, J.; Jiang, H.; Guo, Q.; Ni, W.; Yang, H.; Gu, Y.; Mao, Y. Aberrant regional homogeneity of resting-state executive control, default mode, and salience networks in adult patients with moyamoya disease. Brain Imaging. Behav. 2017, 11, 176–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Rougemont-Bucking, A.; Linnman, C.; Zeffiro, T.A.; Zeidan, M.A.; Lebron-Milad, K.; Rodriguez-Romaguera, J.; Rauch, S.L.; Pitman, R.K.; Milad, M.R. Altered processing of contextual information during fear extinction in PTSD: An fMRI study. CNS Neurosci. 2011, 17, 227–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Reijmer, Y.D.; Schultz, A.P.; Leemans, A.; O’Sullivan, M.J.; Gurol, M.E.; Sperling, R.; Greenberg, S.M.; Viswanathan, A.; Hedden, T. Decoupling of structural and functional brain connectivity in older adults with white matter hyperintensities. Neuroimage 2015, 117, 222–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews Editorial Staff. NIH’s adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study. Alcohol Res. 2018, 39, 97.
  96. Casey, B.J.; Cannonier, T.; Conley, M.I.; Cohen, A.O.; Barch, D.M.; Heitzeg, M.M.; Soules, M.E.; Teslovich, T.; Dellarco, D.V.; Garavan, H.; et al. The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 43–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Karcher, N.R.; O’Brien, K.J.; Kandala, S.; Barch, D.M. Resting-state functional connectivity and psychotic-like experiences in childhood: Results from the adolescent brain cognitive development study. Biol. Psychiatry 2019, 86, 7–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Lisdahl, K.M.; Sher, K.J.; Conway, K.P.; Gonzalez, R.; Feldstein Ewing, S.W.; Nixon, S.J.; Tapert, S.; Bartsch, H.; Goldstein, R.Z.; Heitzeg, M. Adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Overview of substance use assessment methods. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 80–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Luciana, M.; Bjork, J.M.; Nagel, B.J.; Barch, D.M.; Gonzalez, R.; Nixon, S.J.; Banich, M.T. Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: Overview of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 67–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Auchter, A.M.; Hernandez Mejia, M.; Heyser, C.J.; Shilling, P.D.; Jernigan, T.L.; Brown, S.A.; Tapert, S.F.; Dowling, G.J. A description of the ABCD organizational structure and communication framework. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Garavan, H.; Bartsch, H.; Conway, K.; Decastro, A.; Goldstein, R.Z.; Heeringa, S.; Jernigan, T.; Potter, A.; Thompson, W.; Zahs, D. Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 16–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Tulsky, D.S.; Carlozzi, N.; Chiaravalloti, N.D.; Beaumont, J.L.; Kisala, P.A.; Mungas, D.; Conway, K.; Gershon, R. NIH toolbox cognition battery (NIHTB-CB): The list sorting test to measure working memory. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS 2014, 20, 599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Heaton, R.K.; Akshoomoff, N.; Tulsky, D.; Mungas, D.; Weintraub, S.; Dikmen, S.; Beaumont, J.; Casaletto, K.B.; Conway, K.; Slotkin, J. Reliability and validity of composite scores from the NIH toolbox cognition battery in adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS 2014, 20, 588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Hessl, D.; Sansone, S.M.; Berry-Kravis, E.; Riley, K.; Widaman, K.F.; Abbeduto, L.; Schneider, A.; Coleman, J.; Oaklander, D.; Rhodes, K.C. The NIH toolbox cognitive battery for intellectual disabilities: Three preliminary studies and future directions. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2016, 8, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Weintraub, S.; Dikmen, S.S.; Heaton, R.K.; Tulsky, D.S.; Zelazo, P.D.; Bauer, P.J.; Carlozzi, N.E.; Slotkin, J.; Blitz, D.; Wallner-Allen, K. Cognition assessment using the NIH toolbox. Neurology 2013, 80, S54–S64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Roberts, E.; Bornstein, M.H.; Slater, A.M.; Barrett, J. Early cognitive development and parental education. Infant Child Dev. Int. J. Res. Pract. 1999, 8, 49–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Grant, M.D.; Kremen, W.S.; Jacobson, K.C.; Franz, C.; Xian, H.; Eisen, S.A.; Toomey, R.; Murray, R.E.; Lyons, M.J. Does parental education have a moderating effect on the genetic and environmental influences of general cognitive ability in early adulthood? Behav. Genet. 2010, 40, 438–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  108. Parisi, P.; Verrotti, A.; Paolino, M.C.; Miano, S.; Urbano, A.; Bernabucci, M.; Villa, M.P. Cognitive profile, parental education and BMI in children: Reflections on common neuroendrocrinobiological roots. J. Pediatric Endocrinol. Metab. 2010, 23, 1133–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Barnett, W.S. Long-term cognitive and academic effects of early childhood education on children in poverty. Prev. Med. 1998, 27, 204–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Barajas, R.G.; Philipsen, N.; Brooks-Gunn, J. Cognitive and emotional outcomes for children in poverty. In Handbook of Families and Poverty; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2007; pp. 311–333. [Google Scholar]
  111. Finn, A.S.; Minas, J.E.; Leonard, J.A.; Mackey, A.P.; Salvatore, J.; Goetz, C.; West, M.R.; Gabrieli, C.F.O.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Functional brain organization of working memory in adolescents varies in relation to family income and academic achievement. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Silverman, M.E.; Muennig, P.; Liu, X.; Rosen, Z.; Goldstein, M.A. The impact of socioeconomic status on the neural substrates associated with pleasure. Open Neuroimag. J. 2009, 3, 58–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Calem, M.; Bromis, K.; McGuire, P.; Morgan, C.; Kempton, M.J. Meta-analysis of associations between childhood adversity and hippocampus and amygdala volume in non-clinical and general population samples. Neuroimage. Clin. 2017, 14, 471–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Gianaros, P.J.; Sheu, L.K.; Matthews, K.A.; Jennings, J.R.; Manuck, S.B.; Hariri, A.R. Individual differences in stressor-evoked blood pressure reactivity vary with activation, volume, and functional connectivity of the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 990–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  115. Gilliam, M.; Forbes, E.E.; Gianaros, P.J.; Erickson, K.I.; Brennan, L.M.; Shaw, D.S. Maternal depression in childhood and aggression in young adulthood: Evidence for mediation by offspring amygdala-hippocampal volume ratio. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2015, 56, 1083–1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Merz, E.C.; Tottenham, N.; Noble, K.G. Socioeconomic status, amygdala volume, and internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2018, 47, 312–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Morey, R.A.; Haswell, C.C.; Hooper, S.R.; De Bellis, M.D. Amygdala, hippocampus, and ventral medial prefrontal cortex volumes differ in maltreated youth with and without chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016, 41, 791–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  118. Rojas, D.C.; Smith, J.A.; Benkers, T.L.; Camou, S.L.; Reite, M.L.; Rogers, S.J. Hippocampus and amygdala volumes in parents of children with autistic disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2004, 161, 2038–2044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Taren, A.A.; Creswell, J.D.; Gianaros, P.J. Dispositional mindfulness co-varies with smaller amygdala and caudate volumes in community adults. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e64574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Assari, S.; Boyce, S.; Bazargan, M. Subjective Socioeconomic Status and Children’s Amygdala Volume: Minorities’ Diminish Returns. NeuroSci 2020, 1, 59–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Trotman, G.P.; Gianaros, P.J.; Veldhuijzen van Zanten, J.; Williams, S.E.; Ginty, A.T. Increased stressor-evoked cardiovascular reactivity is associated with reduced amygdala and hippocampus volume. Psychophysiology 2019, 56, e13277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  122. Walton, E.; Cecil, C.A.M.; Suderman, M.; Liu, J.; Turner, J.A.; Calhoun, V.; Ehrlich, S.; Relton, C.L.; Barker, E.D. Longitudinal epigenetic predictors of amygdala:hippocampus volume ratio. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 1341–1350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Tottenham, N.; Hare, T.A.; Quinn, B.T.; McCarry, T.W.; Nurse, M.; Gilhooly, T.; Millner, A.; Galvan, A.; Davidson, M.C.; Eigsti, I.M. Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev. Sci. 2010, 13, 46–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  124. Brody, G.H.; Yu, T.; Nusslock, R.; Barton, A.W.; Miller, G.E.; Chen, E.; Holmes, C.; McCormick, M.; Sweet, L.H. The protective effects of supportive parenting on the relationship between adolescent poverty and resting-state functional brain connectivity during adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Barch, D.; Pagliaccio, D.; Belden, A.; Harms, M.P.; Gaffrey, M.; Sylvester, C.M.; Tillman, R.; Luby, J. Effect of hippocampal and amygdala connectivity on the relationship between preschool poverty and school-age depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 2016, 173, 625–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Qian, W.; Schweizer, T.A.; Fischer, C.E. Impact of socioeconomic status on initial clinical presentation to a memory disorders clinic. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2014, 26, 597–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Lupien, S.J.; Parent, S.; Evans, A.C.; Tremblay, R.E.; Zelazo, P.D.; Corbo, V.; Pruessner, J.C.; Séguin, J.R. Larger amygdala but no change in hippocampal volume in 10-year-old children exposed to maternal depressive symptomatology since birth. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 14324–14329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  128. Moutsiana, C.; Johnstone, T.; Murray, L.; Fearon, P.; Cooper, P.J.; Pliatsikas, C.; Goodyer, I.; Halligan, S.L. Insecure attachment during infancy predicts greater amygdala volumes in early adulthood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2015, 56, 540–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  129. Assari, S. Educational attainment better protects African American women than African American men against depressive symptoms and psychological distress. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  130. Assari, S. Family socioeconomic position at birth and school bonding at age 15; Blacks’ diminished returns. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  131. Assari, S. Parental educational attainment and academic performance of American college students; Blacks’ diminished returns. J. Health Econ. Dev. 2019, 1, 21–31. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  132. Assari, S. Family income reduces risk of obesity for white but not black children. Children 2018, 5, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  133. Assari, S. Blacks’ diminished return of education attainment on subjective health; Mediating effect of income. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  134. Assari, S.; Lankarani, M.M. Education and alcohol consumption among older Americans; Black-white differences. Front. Public Health 2016, 4, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  135. Assari, S.; Moghani Lankarani, M.; Caldwell, C.H.; Zimmerman, M.A. Fear of neighborhood violence during adolescence predicts development of obesity a decade later: Gender differences among African Americans. Arch. Trauma. Res. 2016, 5, e31475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  136. Assari, S. Education attainment and obesity differential returns based on sexual orientation. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  137. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H. Social determinants of perceived discrimination among black youth: Intersection of ethnicity and gender. Children 2018, 5, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  138. Assari, S.; Lankarani, M.M. Reciprocal associations between depressive symptoms and mastery among older adults; Black-white differences. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016, 8, 279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Assari, S. General self-efficacy and mortality in the USA.; Racial differences. J. Racial Ethn. Health Dispar. 2017, 4, 746–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  140. Krieger, N.; Williams, D.; Zierler, S. “Whiting out” white privilege will not advance the study of how racism harms health. Am. J. Public Health 1999, 89, 782–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  141. Krieger, N. Epidemiology, racism, and health: The case of low birth weight. Epidemiology 2000, 11, 237–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  142. Rich-Edwards, J.; Krieger, N.; Majzoub, J.; Zierler, S.; Lieberman, E.; Gillman, M. Maternal experiences of racism and violence as predictors of preterm birth: Rationale and study design. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2001, 15 (Suppl. 2), 124–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Krieger, N. Does racism harm health? Did child abuse exist before 1962? On explicit questions, critical science, and current controversies: An ecosocial perspective. Am. J. Public Health 2003, 93, 194–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Parrott, R.L.; Silk, K.J.; Dillow, M.R.; Krieger, J.L.; Harris, T.M.; Condit, C.M. Development and validation of tools to assess genetic discrimination and genetically based racism. J. Natl. Med. Assoc. 2005, 97, 980–990. [Google Scholar]
  145. Krieger, N.; Smith, K.; Naishadham, D.; Hartman, C.; Barbeau, E.M. Experiences of discrimination: Validity and reliability of a self-report measure for population health research on racism and health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2005, 61, 1576–1596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  146. Assari, S. Age-Related Decline in Children’s Reward Sensitivity: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Res Health Sci. 2020, 5, 112–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Krieger, N. Living and dying at the crossroads: Racism, embodiment, and why theory is essential for a public health of consequence. Am. J. Public Health 2016, 106, 832–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Bassett, M.T.; Krieger, N.; Bailey, Z. Charlottesville: Blatant racism, not grievances, on display. Lancet 2017, 390, 2243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Bailey, Z.D.; Krieger, N.; Agenor, M.; Graves, J.; Linos, N.; Bassett, M.T. Structural racism and health inequities in the USA: Evidence and interventions. Lancet 2017, 389, 1453–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Clark, U.S.; Miller, E.R.; Hegde, R.R. Experiences of discrimination are associated with greater resting amygdala activity and functional connectivity. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2018, 3, 367–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Fourie, M.M.; Stein, D.J.; Solms, M.; Gobodo-Madikizela, P.; Decety, J. Effects of early adversity and social discrimination on empathy for complex mental states: An fMRI investigation. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Han, S.D.; Lamar, M.; Fleischman, D.; Kim, N.; Bennett, D.A.; Lewis, T.T.; Arfanakis, K.; Barnes, L.L. Self-reported experiences of discrimination in older black adults are associated with insula functional connectivity. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  153. Phelps, E.A.; O’Connor, K.J.; Cunningham, W.A.; Funayama, E.S.; Gatenby, J.C.; Gore, J.C.; Banaji, M.R. Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 729–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  154. Wheeler, M.E.; Fiske, S.T. Controlling racial prejudice: Social-cognitive goals affect amygdala and stereotype activation. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 56–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Chekroud, A.M.; Everett, J.A.; Bridge, H.; Hewstone, M. A review of neuroimaging studies of race-related prejudice: Does amygdala response reflect threat? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  156. Assari, S. Socioeconomic Status Inequalities Partially Mediate Racial and Ethnic Differences in Children’s Amygdala Volume. Stud. Soc. Sci. Res. 2020, 1, 62–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Uytun, M.C. Development period of prefrontal cortex. In Prefrontal Cortex; IntechOpen: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  158. Pennington, B.F.; Bennetto, L.; McAleer, O.; Roberts, R.J., Jr. Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. In Attention, Memory, and Executive Function; Lyon, G.R., Krasnegor, N.A., Eds.; Paul H Brookes Publishing Co.: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  159. Kesner, R.P.; Churchwell, J.C. An analysis of rat prefrontal cortex in mediating executive function. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2011, 96, 417–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Testing our model’s assumptions: (a) distribution of our outcome, (b) residuals, and (c) quantiles.
Figure 1. Testing our model’s assumptions: (a) distribution of our outcome, (b) residuals, and (c) quantiles.
Brainsci 10 00950 g001aBrainsci 10 00950 g001b
Figure 2. Parental education effects overall.
Figure 2. Parental education effects overall.
Brainsci 10 00950 g002
Figure 3. Income effects overall.
Figure 3. Income effects overall.
Brainsci 10 00950 g003
Figure 4. Parental education across groups.
Figure 4. Parental education across groups.
Brainsci 10 00950 g004
Figure 5. Income effects across groups.
Figure 5. Income effects across groups.
Brainsci 10 00950 g005
Table 1. Presents the descriptive statistics of the pooled sample and by race.
Table 1. Presents the descriptive statistics of the pooled sample and by race.
LevelAllWhiteBlackAsianOther/Mixedp
n = 10,414n = 6897n = 1515n = 234n = 1768
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
Age (Months)118.96 (7.46)119.03 (7.49)118.89 (7.23)119.40 (7.77)118.65 (7.51)0.187
Card Sorting Score97.10 (15.26)98.29 (15.07)91.31 (13.98)102.36 (17.94)96.73 (15.46)<0.001
n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
Parental education
<HS Diploma385 (3.7)145 (2.1)123 (8.1)6 (2.6)111 (6.3)<0.001
HS Diploma/GED862 (8.3)327 (4.7)340 (22.4)3 (1.3)192 (10.9)
Some College2674 (25.7)1462 (21.2)600 (39.6)18 (7.7)594 (33.6)
Bachelor2766 (26.6)2057 (29.8)230 (15.2)65 (27.8)414 (23.4)
Post Graduate Degree3727 (35.8)2906 (42.1)222 (14.7)142 (60.7)457 (25.8)
Household Income
<50 K2997 (28.8)1259 (18.3)999 (65.9)36 (15.4)703 (39.8)<0.001
>= 50 K & <100 K2974 (28.6)2104 (30.5)335 (22.1)54 (23.1)481 (27.2)
>= 100 K4443 (42.7)3534 (51.2)181 (11.9)144 (61.5)584 (33.0)
Latino
No8451 (81.2)5737 (83.2)1439 (95.0)215 (91.9)1060 (60.0)<0.001
Yes1963 (18.8)1160 (16.8)76 (5.0)19 (8.1)708 (40.0)
Sex
Female4996 (48.0)3254 (47.2)760 (50.2)117 (50.0)865 (48.9)0.128
Male5418 (52.0)3643 (52.8)755 (49.8)117 (50.0)903 (51.1)
Married Family
No3165 (30.4)1415 (20.5)1058 (69.8)33 (14.1)659 (37.3)<0.001
Yes7249 (69.6)5482 (79.5)457 (30.2)201 (85.9)1109 (62.7)
Table 2. Effect sizes and % variance explained.
Table 2. Effect sizes and % variance explained.
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4Model 5Model 6
n11,31510,41810,41410,41811,31510,414
R-squared0.04540.039470.045450.039630.046960.04681
ΔR-squared0.012440.008040.00620.022560.029220.01717
% Variance1.24%0.8%0.62%2.26%2.92%1.72%
Table 3. Mixed-effects regressions in the pooled sample (n = 10418).
Table 3. Mixed-effects regressions in the pooled sample (n = 10418).
CharacteristicsbSEpSig
Model 1
Parental Education (HS Diploma)1.580.830.056#
Parental Education (Some College)2.860.75<0.001***
Parental Education (Bachelor)4.840.78<0.001***
Parental Education (Graduate Degree)6.690.77<0.001***
Model 2
Household Income (50–100 K)3.020.44<0.001***
Household Income (100 + K)4.200.46<0.001***
Model 3
Parental Education (HS Diploma)0.950.940.311
Parental Education (Some College)2.040.850.017*
Parental Education (Bachelor)3.460.91<0.001***
Parental Education (Graduate Degree)5.180.92<0.001***
Household Income (50–100 K)2.210.52<0.001***
Household Income (100 + K)1.880.47<0.001***
Model 4
Household Income (50–100 K)3.920.56<0.001***
Household Income (100 + K)2.820.57<0.001***
Race (Black)−4.960.69<0.001***
Race (Asian)3.762.550.140
Race (Other/Mixed)−0.770.720.284
Household Income (50–100 K) × Black0.261.110.818
Household Income (100 + K) × Black0.661.340.624
Household Income (50–100 K) × Asian−2.183.270.505
Household Income (100 + K) × Asian−0.082.840.977
Household Income (50–100 K) × Other/Mix0.661.050.527
Household Income (100 + K) × Other/Mix0.860.990.385
Model 5
Parental Education (HS Diploma)3.491.310.008**
Parental Education (Some College)3.871.140.001***
Parental Education (Bachelor)6.301.15<0.001***
Parental Education (Graduate Degree)7.551.14<0.001***
Race (Black)−1.771.660.286
Race (Asian)1.865.640.742
Race (Other/Mixed)0.491.640.768
Parental Education (HS Diploma) × Black−4.451.940.022*
Parental Education (Some College) × Black−2.441.770.169
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Black−3.621.930.060#
Parental Education (Graduate Degree) × Black−2.921.940.132
Parental Education (HS Diploma) × Asian−11.199.300.229
Parental Education (Some College) × Asian0.626.630.926
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Asian−1.265.900.831
Parental Education (Graduate Degree) × Asian1.835.760.751
Parental Education (HS Diploma) × Other/Mix−1.342.070.517
Parental Education (Some College) × Other/Mix−0.811.790.650
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Other/Mix−2.321.820.203
Parental Education (Graduate Degree) × Other/Mix0.801.800.659
Model 6
Parental Education (HS Diploma)2.581.510.089#
Parental Education (Some College)2.801.350.038*
Parental Education (Bachelor)4.711.370.001***
Parental Education (Graduate Degree)5.861.37<0.001***
Household Income (50–100 K)1.850.47<0.001***
Household Income (100 + K)2.140.52<0.001***
Race (Black)−2.341.890.216
Race (Asian)2.096.160.735
Race (Other/Mixed)0.801.890.673
Parental Education (HS Diploma) × Black−3.602.200.101
Parental Education (Some College) × Black−1.232.010.542
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Black−2.742.160.203
Parental Education (Graduate Degree) × Black−1.862.160.389
Parental Education (HS Diploma) × Asian−10.6010.580.316
Parental Education (Some College) × Asian0.277.090.969
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Asian−1.366.440.833
Parental Education (Graduate Degree) × Asian2.116.290.738
Parental Education (HS Diploma) × Other/Mix−1.162.330.620
Parental Education (Some College) × Other/Mix−1.002.030.621
Parental Education (Bachelor) × Other/Mix−2.362.060.252
Parental Education (Graduate Degree) × Other/Mix0.522.040.800
# p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Akhlaghipour, G.; Assari, S. Parental Education, Household Income, Race, and Children’s Working Memory: Complexity of the Effects. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 950. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120950

AMA Style

Akhlaghipour G, Assari S. Parental Education, Household Income, Race, and Children’s Working Memory: Complexity of the Effects. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(12):950. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120950

Chicago/Turabian Style

Akhlaghipour, Golnoush, and Shervin Assari. 2020. "Parental Education, Household Income, Race, and Children’s Working Memory: Complexity of the Effects" Brain Sciences 10, no. 12: 950. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120950

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop