Next Article in Journal
Does the Behavior of Pediatric Patients towards Dental Treatment Influence the Pattern of Adjacent (Sound, Decayed, and Filled) Proximal Surfaces in Primary Molars?
Next Article in Special Issue
Emotions and Colors in a Design Archiving System: Applying AI Technology for Museums
Previous Article in Journal
Semi-Active Cable Damping to Compensate for Damping Losses Due to Reduced Cable Motion Close to Cable Anchor
Previous Article in Special Issue
How to Improve the Acceptance of Autonomous Driving Technology: Effective Elements Identified on the Basis of the Kano Model
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Time to Capture a Moving Target Travelling along a Circular Trajectory

Department of Industrial & Management Engineering, Korea National University of Transportation, Chungju-si 27469, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2022, 12(4), 1911; https://doi.org/10.3390/app12041911
Submission received: 10 January 2022 / Revised: 4 February 2022 / Accepted: 8 February 2022 / Published: 11 February 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue State-of-the-Art in Human Factors and Interaction Design)

Abstract

:
This study measured the time it took to select a target moving along a circular trajectory with a computer mouse. The time was changed according to the speed of the target, the width of target and the distance from the starting point to the target. However, the effect of these independent variables on the dependent variable was different from what was expected. In the previous studies, it was assumed that the faster the moving target speed, the longer the target selection time, because increased target speed had the effect of narrowing the effective target width. However, as a result of the experiment, the target selection time was rather shortened when the moving speed of the target was increased. This may be because the subjects intend to speed up target selection while decreasing the accuracy of target selection in order to adapt to a fast-moving target. The modified Fitts’ model for the moving target selection time proposed in a previous study did not take these user responses into account. A more modified model is required to more accurately describe the selection time of moving target.

1. Introduction

The time to move a physical stylus from a home position to a stationary target depends on the size of the target and the distance from the home position to the target. The longer the distance between the home position and the target, the longer the movement time. On the other hand, the smaller the size of the target, the longer the time required for the movement. The proposed law based on this principle is Fitts’ law, which is expressed in Equation (1) [1]. It was found that the time to move the physical stylus from the home position to the stationary target is linearly proportional to log2(2A/W), called the difficulty index (ID), where W is the width of the target and A is the distance from the home position to the target. MT is the average time to complete the movement. a and b are constants that depend on the choice of input device and are usually determined empirically. The parameter a defines the intersection on the y axis and is often interpreted as a delay. The b parameter is a slope and describes an acceleration.
MT = a + b   l o g 2   2 A / W
Fitts’ law is applicable to the actual physical movement of the hand to a target, such as the movement of a cursor to a screen target [2]. It is also applicable to movement as coarse as a foot reaching for a pedal and as fine as manipulation under a microscope [3,4]. This generality provides the law great value in allowing designers to determine keyboard layouts and target sizes in a wide variety of circumstances [5,6,7]. The theoretical basis for Fitts’ law was developed and expended. Several modifications were also proposed such as a model considering the length of probe [8], a model fittable for the low index of difficulty [9] and models not using a logarithmic function [10,11].
However, most of the studies on Fitts’ law were performed on the movement time for approaching a stationary target. There have been just few studies of Fitts’ law considering moving targets. Jagacinski et al., (1980) first studied the time it takes to catch a moving target based on the Fitts’ law [12]. They found that even with a moving target, the size of the target is inversely proportional to the time taken, and the distance between the target and the home position is proportional to the time taken. A time prediction model considering the speed of a moving target were proposed as in Equation (2).
MT = c + d A + e V + 1 1 / W 1
In Equation (2), c, d, and e are constants, which are determined according to the characteristics of the work or worker’s skill, and are derived through experiments. The velocity variable V at which the target moves, which does not exist in Equation (1), is additionally considered. The number of empirical constants were increased and the logarithmic function was not used
To follow the form of the traditional Fitts’ law, Hoffmann (1991) proposed a new modified model by reanalyzing Jagacinski et al.’s experimental data without a separate experiment. Equation (3) is a mathematical model proposed by Hoffmann (1991) [13]. MT, a, b, A, and W in Equation (3), are the same as the variables and constants of the Equation (1) and V is the variable representing the moving speed of the target.
MT = a + b l o g 2   A ± V / K / W / 2 V / K
This model is a one-dimensional model and describes the time it takes to capture the target by the physical styles when the target approaches or moves away from the home position of the physical styles. The numerator of the logarithmic function of this model, A ± V/K, indicates the effect of the moving speed of the target on A. The A is the distance between the target and home position before the physical stylus movement begins.
Actual A is shortened as the target approaches the origin at a constant speed. The shortened distance was assumed to be AV/K. That is, it represents the effect that the length of A becomes shorter due to the movement of the target. On the other hand, A + V/K shows the effect that the length of A becomes longer as the target moves away from the home position at a constant speed.
The denominator of the logarithmic function, W/2 – V/K, represents the effect of the moving speed of the target on target width (W), called effective target width. Whether the target approaches the home position or moves away from the home position, it has the effect of decreasing the W value regardless of the direction of movement. In Equation (3), K is an empirically determined constant for calibrating the unit of the moving speed of the target.
After the Hoffmann (1991) study, the models extended to the case of selecting a two-dimensional moving target and a three-dimensional moving target were proposed under the premise that the Hoffmann’s model well describes the selection time of a moving target [14]. This study is to verify whether the Hoffmann (1991) model is applicable even for a target moving along a circular trajectory. Hoffmann (1991) model may be possible to describe the time required to select a moving target in the situation where the computer cursor is centered on the circular trajectory and the target moves at a constant speed along the circular trajectory. In this case, the distance the cursor moves (A) does not change between the initial state and the state the target is moving. A moving target only affects target width. In other words, it will only have the effect that the width of the target became narrower. Therefore, the modified model for capturing a target moving along a circular trajectory can be expressed as Equation (4).
MT = a + b l o g 2   ( A / W / 2 V / K

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 12 Participants (10 males, 2 females) took parts in this experiment. Their average age was 24.7 ± 2.6 years old. They had sufficient proficiency with mouse devices and basic knowledge of Fitts’ law. In addition, their corrected visual acuity was 0.8 or higher, so there was no problem in recognizing a moving target on the computer screen.

2.2. Apparatus

For this experiment, the experimental device was made in C++ language as shown in Figure 1. The target was designed to move at a fixed speed along a circular trajectory. It was designed to change the diameter of the circular trajectory and the size of the target according to the experimental conditions. The experiment started when the participant clicked the center point of the circular trajectory with the mouse. The target on a circular trajectory moved at a constant speed along the trajectory, and the participant moved the mouse to approach the target and then clicked on the target. The movement time of the mouse was the difference between the time the center point of the circular trajectory was clicked and the time the target was clicked.

2.3. Experimental Design

The three independent variables of the experiment were target width (W), moving distance (A) and target speed (V). The moving distance (A) is the distance from the starting point of a computer mouse to the center of the target, and it was set to 150, 260, and 370 pixels. The target was a square with the same width and height, and the width (W) of the target was set to 25, 33, and 41 pixels. The target velocity (V) was set at three levels 0, 100, and 200 pixels/s. All subjects participated in all experimental conditions and the number of repetitions in each experimental condition was 20. The setting of each experimental condition was adjusted so that the index of task difficulty (ID) of the traditional Fitts’ law was formed evenly without overlapping. The index of task difficulty (ID) was distributed between 2.0 and 4.9.

3. Results

3.1. Analysis of Target Selection Accuracy

Among the data collected in 20 repeated trials, data that did not accurately click the target was extracted. The mean number of accurate clicks on the target was 18.27 and the standard deviation was 1.18. As shown in Figure 2, the error of target selection was larger as the target width decreased (p < 0.001) and the speed of the moving target increased (p < 0.001). However, the distance between the home position and the moving target on the click error was not statistically significant (p = 0.157). In general, the target selection is less accurate as the difficulty of the task increases. In this experiment, as the width of the target increased, the accuracy increased, and as the speed decreased, the accuracy increased. On the other hand, although not statistically significant, the shorter the distance between the target and the home position, the greater the accuracy.
Among the three independent variables, the interaction effect on the selection accuracy was significant only between the moving speed of the target and the target width (p < 0.001), as shown in Figure 3. When the target speed was high and the width of the target was narrow, the click accuracy was very low. It was found that if the width of the target was narrow and the speed of the target was high, the difficulty of the task was greatly increased.

3.2. Analysis of Target Selection Time

Except for the cases where the target could not be accurately clicked, the averages of the target selection times were calculated according to each participant under each experimental condition were obtained. Analysis of variance was performed on the mean of the target selection times. All three main effects on the mean time of target selection were found to be statistically significant, p < 0.001 for the distance between home position and moving target(A), p < 0.001 for the width of the target (W), and p < 0.05 for the speed of the moving target (V)). Figure 4 indicates the means of target selection time according to the speed of moving target (left), the target width (center) and the distance between home position and moving target (right).
These results mean that the general principle of Fitts’ law, “Target selection time is proportional to the distance between the target and home position and inversely proportional to the width of the target”, holds true for a moving target as well. On the other hand, it shows that the time to select the target decreases as the movement speed of target increases. This result is inconsistent with Hoffman’s prediction model.
Among the three independent variables, the interaction effect on the selection time was significant only between the moving speed of the target and the target width (p < 0.05), as shown in Figure 5. In the case of a fast target speed, if the target width was wide, the cursor movement time was very short, but if the target width was narrower, the movement time increased rapidly.

3.3. Fit of the Fitts’ Model for Moving Targets with the Same Speed

It was tested whether the traditional Fitts’ model could be applied in a situation where the speed of the moving target is the same. The data collected at each three target speeds were examined to see if they could be explained by the traditional Fitts’ model. The results of the linear regression analysis are shown in Equations (5)–(7). The R2 values of the linear regression model were 0.92 or higher. This means that the traditional Fitts’ model has explanatory power for the targets moving at the same speed. However, there is a question of how to represent the targets moving at different speeds with a single model.
MT v = 0 = 160.91 + 111.96 l o g 2   2 A / W   R 2 = 0.93  
MT v = 100 = 20.66 + 146.40 l o g 2   2 A / W   R 2 = 0.92
MT v = 200 = 58.60 + 130.30 l o g 2   2 A / W   R 2 = 0.96  

3.4. Fit of the Fitts’ Model for Moving Targets with Various Speeds

It was evaluated whether the collected target selection times were well fitted to the model proposed in this study. The degree of model fit was determined by the degree of linear correlation between measured target selection times and indexes of difficulty. In regression, the R2 coefficient of determination is a statistical measure of how well the regression predictions approximate the real data points. In order to evaluate the degree of model fit, an optimal K value should be also derived. The optimal K value is an empirically determined constant for calibrating the unit of the moving speed of the target. Therefore, linear regression analysis between measured selection time and index of the difficulty was performed while changing K. The K value in the linear regression equation that provides the largest R2 value was determined as an optimal K value.
Table 1 indicates the change of Equation (4) according to K value change. If the value of K approaches negative infinity or approaches positive infinity, Equation (4) approximates the traditional Fitts’ law. This is because the speed (V) of the moving target becomes meaningless when K is large. On the other hand, if the value of K is 0, the index of difficulty in Equation (4) becomes 0 and the movement time is equal to a. If the optimal K value was determined as any negative number, it means that the participants was feeling the target width wider than the actual target width. On the other hand, if the optimal K value was positive, it indicates that the participants was feeling the target width narrower than the actual target width, due to the speed of moving target. The optimal K value in the Hoffmann’s study was positive and the result of this study was also expected to be positive.
Contrary to our expectations, the optimal K value that gave the best model fit was negative. A negative value of K means that the effective width of the target was larger than the actual target width. Table 2 shows the change of the R2 value of the proposed model according to the change of the K value. When the K value was between −95 and −100, the R2 value was the largest (R2 = 0.9173).
In some cases, linear regression analysis did not work during data analysis. For example, if W = 25, A = 150, V = 200, and K = 10, the ID value could not be calculated. If the value of each parameter is substituted into ID = log2 (A/(W/2 − V/K)), the ID becomes log2(−20). X must be a positive real number in “logb x”, which is the log function of x with respect to base b. Since the value of X is negative, calculation of ID is not possible. This case frequently occurred when the absolute value of K was small. These cases were excluded from the analysis results.
Figure 6 shows three cases of the linear regression equations shown in Table 2. The linear regression equation on the left is a case where the K value is close to infinity, so the speed of the moving target does not affect the mouse movement time. The graph presented in the center is the case of the best model fit, where the coefficient of determination of the regression equation is the largest. On the right is an example where the coefficient of determination of the linear regression equation is very low (K = −10).
The coefficient of determination of the linear regression equation was 0.9029 when the traditional Fitts’ model, which does not consider the velocity of the moving target, was applied to the collected data. This coefficient is very high enough to validate the fit of the model. However, when the model considering the speed of the moving target was applied, the coefficient of determination of the linear regression equation was further improved (R2 = 0.9173).

4. Discussion

In the case of selecting a target moving along a circular trajectory with a computer mouse, if the moving speed of the target was constant, the time to select a target was inversely proportional to the width of the target and changed in proportional to the distance between the target and the starting point, as in the traditional Fitts’ law. These results were similar to the results in the experiment of clicking a target moving in a straight line using a joystick [15]. It suggests that although the target is moving, if it is moving at the same speed, the target selection time is well explained by the traditional Fitts’ law.
However, there was a research question of how to represent the time for selecting targets moving at different speeds with a single model. Hoffman (1991) set the speed of a moving target as a variable, and included this variable in the modified Fitts’ Law. If this model is applied to the case of selecting a target moving along a circular trajectory, increasing the target speed has the effect of reducing the target width. Consequently, the model that best fits the collected data should be determined when the value of K is positive. Also, increasing the speed of a moving target should increase the time it takes to click the target.
However, the result of this experiment showed that the optimal K value was negative, and as the speed increased, the time to click the target decreased. This result was inconsistent with the assumptions of the Hoffman model. Hoffman’s (1991) model described that when clicking on a moving target, clicking on a fast-moving target was more difficult than clicking on a stationary target or slow-moving targets. He might not take in account the subjects’ tendency to speed up the movement of the mouse in order to click on the moving target. Comparing the target selection error data and the target selection time data simultaneously, when the speed of the moving target more increases, the number of click errors increases and the click time tends to be decreased. That is, it can be said that the subjects increased the mouse movement speed while sacrificing the accuracy of clicking the target.
On the other hand, when K value of proposed model was very high, the movement speed of the target had no effect on the target selection time. That is, in this case, the Fitts’ model proposed in this paper approaches to the traditional Fitts’ model. The degree of Fitts’ model fits was 0.9029, even in the case where the speed of the moving target did not affect the target selection time. Even though the target movement speed was not included in the model, it is a very high value that the coefficient of determination is more than 0.9. This result might be due to confounding factors. Gori et al. [16] argued that the high R2 between the difficulty index (ID) and the mean travel time (MT) is due to the strong confounding between the travel distance (A) and the difficulty index (ID). Anyway, in this study, a more suitable model was found by considering the speed of the moving target as an additional variable. The fit of the model was higher than when the target speed was not considered (R2 = 0.9137). However, unexpectedly, the optimal K value that gave the best model fit was negative.
A negative optimal K value may not mean that when subjects click a target, they use an effective target width that is wider than the actual target width. This can be caused by the human reaction tendency to quickly click on the target, which is affected by the speed of the moving target. In the future, more sophisticated models should be developed to include the tendency of humans to react to moving targets.

5. Conclusions

This study was conducted to find out whether the time it takes to click a moving target traveling a circular trajectory with a mouse could be well explained by the modified Fitts’ model proposed by Hoffmann (1991). As a result of this experiment, the time to click the moving target was shorter than the time to click the stationary target, and as the speed of the moving target increased, the click time became shorter. These results were contrary to those that could be inferred by Hoffman’s model. In this experiment, the subjects showed a response to increase the mouse movement speed by adapting to the speed of the moving target while sacrificing the accuracy of the mouse click. Previous Fitts’ models for moving targets did not include such human responses. In the future, it is necessary to improve Fitts’ law for a moving target in consideration of this point.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.-K.H.; methodology, S.-K.H.; software, J.L.; validation, S.-K.H. and J.L.; formal analysis, S.-K.H.; data curation, S.-K.H.; writing—original draft preparation, S.-K.H.; writing—review and editing, J.L.; visualization, S.-K.H.; supervision, S.-K.H.; funding acquisition, S.-K.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Education (Grant number #NRF-2018R1D1A1B07048479).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Fitts, P.M. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J. Exp. Psychol. 1954, 47, 381–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Card, S.K.; English, W.K.; Burr, B.J. Evaluation of mouse, rate-controlled isometric joystick, step keys, and text keys for text selection on a CRT. Ergonomics 1978, 21, 601–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Drury, C.G. Application of Fitts’ law to foot-pedal design. Hum. Factors 1975, 17, 368–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Langolf, G.D.; Chaffin, D.B.; Foulke, J.A. An investigation of Fitts’ law using a wide range of movement amplitudes. J. Mot. Behav. 1976, 8, 113–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Card, S.K.; Moran, T.P.; Newell, A. Psychology of Human-Computer Interaction; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  6. MacKenzie, I.S. Fitts’ law as a research and design tool in human–computer interaction. Hum. Comput. Interact. 1992, 7, 91–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Colle, H.A.; Hiszem, K.J. Standing at a kiosk: Effects of key size and spacing on touch screen numeric keypad performance and user experience. Ergonomics 2004, 47, 1406–1423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Baird, K.M.; Hoffman, E.R.; Drury, C.G. The effects of probe length on Fitts’ law. Appl. Ergon. 2002, 33, 9–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Gan, K.-C.; Hoffman, E.R. Geometrical conditions for ballistic and visually controlled movements. Ergonomics 1988, 31, 829–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Kvålseth, T.O. An alternative to Fitts’ law. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 1980, 16, 371–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Meyer, D.E.; Abrams, R.A.; Kornblum, S.; Wright, C.E.; Smith, K.J.E. Optimality in human motor performance: Ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol. Rev. 1988, 95, 340–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Jagacinski, R.J.; Repperger, D.W.; Ward, S.L.; Moran, M.S. A test of Fitts’ law with moving targets. Hum. Factors 1980, 22, 225–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Hoffmann, E.R. Capture of moving targets: A modification of Fitts’ law. Ergonomics 1991, 34, 211–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hajri, A.A.; Fels, S.; Miller, G.; Ilich, M. Moving Target Selection in 2D Graphical User Interfaces. In Human-Computer Interaction INTERACT; Campos, P., Graham, N., Jorge, J., Nunes, N., Palanque, P., Winckler, M., Eds.; Lecture Notes in Computer Science; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2011; Volume 6947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Chiu, T.-T.; Lin, C.-L.; Young, K.-Y.; Lin, C.-T.; Hsu, S.-H.; Yang, B.-S.; Huang, Z. A study of Fitts’ law on goal-directed aiming task with moving targets. Percept. Motor. Skills 2011, 113, 339–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Gori, J.; Rioul, O.; Guiard, Y.; Beaudouin-Lafon, M. The Perils of Confounding Factors: How Fitts’ Law Experiments Can Lead to False Conclusions. In Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’18), Montreal, QC, Canada, 21–26 April 2018; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. A target moving along the circular trajectory and home position of computer cursor.
Figure 1. A target moving along the circular trajectory and home position of computer cursor.
Applsci 12 01911 g001
Figure 2. Main effects of the speed of moving target (left), the target width (center) and the distance between home position and moving target (right) on the target section accuracy.
Figure 2. Main effects of the speed of moving target (left), the target width (center) and the distance between home position and moving target (right) on the target section accuracy.
Applsci 12 01911 g002
Figure 3. Interaction effects between the speed of moving target and the target width on the target selection accuracy.
Figure 3. Interaction effects between the speed of moving target and the target width on the target selection accuracy.
Applsci 12 01911 g003
Figure 4. Main effects of the speed of moving target (left), the target width (center) and the distance between home position and moving target (right) on the target selection time.
Figure 4. Main effects of the speed of moving target (left), the target width (center) and the distance between home position and moving target (right) on the target selection time.
Applsci 12 01911 g004
Figure 5. Interaction effects between the speed of moving target and the target width on the target selection time.
Figure 5. Interaction effects between the speed of moving target and the target width on the target selection time.
Applsci 12 01911 g005
Figure 6. Linear regression analysis between target selection times and indexes of difficulty; when the movement speed of the target does not affect the movement time (left), when the model fit is the best (center), when the model fit is poor (right).
Figure 6. Linear regression analysis between target selection times and indexes of difficulty; when the movement speed of the target does not affect the movement time (left), when the model fit is the best (center), when the model fit is poor (right).
Applsci 12 01911 g006
Table 1. Variation of Equation (4) according to K value change.
Table 1. Variation of Equation (4) according to K value change.
KModels
K = Negative infinityMT = a + b log2(2A/W)
Negative infinity < K < 0MT = a + b log2(A/(W/2 + V/K))
K = 0MT = a
0 < K < Positive infinityMT = a + b log2(A/(W/2 − V/K))
K = Positive infinityMT = a + b log2(2A/W)
Table 2. Variation of linear regression equation and R2 coefficient of determination according to K value change.
Table 2. Variation of linear regression equation and R2 coefficient of determination according to K value change.
K Linear Regression Equations IDR2
Negative infinityMT = 80.06 + 129.55 × IDLog2(2A/W) 0.9029
−200MT = 80.31 + 131.97× IDLog2(A/(W/2 + V/200)) 0.9136
−105MT = 83.80 + 131.37 × IDLog2(A/(W/2 + V/105)) 0.9172
−100MT = 84.32 + 131.36 × IDLog2(A/(W/2 + V/100)) 0.9173
−95MT = 84.93 + 131.35 × IDLog2(A/(W/2 + V/95)) 0.9173
−90MT = 85.65 + 131.33 × IDLog2(A/(W/2 + V/90)) 0.9172
−10MT = 297.05 + 88.13 × IDLog2(A/(W/2 + V/10)) 0.6311
50MT = 129.62 + 111.03 × IDLog2(A/(W/2 − V/50)) 0.7730
100MT = 93.64 + 123.04 × IDLog2(A/(W/2 − V/100)) 0.8565
200MT = 84.28 + 126.95 × IDLog2(A/(W/2 − V/200)) 0.8841
Positive infinityMT = 80.06 + 129.55 × IDLog2(2A/W) 0.9029
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lee, J.; Hong, S.-K. Time to Capture a Moving Target Travelling along a Circular Trajectory. Appl. Sci. 2022, 12, 1911. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12041911

AMA Style

Lee J, Hong S-K. Time to Capture a Moving Target Travelling along a Circular Trajectory. Applied Sciences. 2022; 12(4):1911. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12041911

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lee, Jongsung, and Seung-Kweon Hong. 2022. "Time to Capture a Moving Target Travelling along a Circular Trajectory" Applied Sciences 12, no. 4: 1911. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12041911

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop