Next Article in Journal
The Dynamics of Mastery Motivation and Its Relationship with Self-Concept in Music Education
Previous Article in Journal
Critical Thinking, Generalized Anxiety in Satisfaction with Studies: The Mediating Role of Academic Self-Efficacy in Medical Students
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Landscape Preference: The Role of Attractiveness and Spatial Openness of the Environment

Faculty of Informatics and Management, University of Hradec Králové, Rokitanského 62, 500 03 Hradec Králové, Czech Republic
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(8), 666; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13080666
Submission received: 14 June 2023 / Revised: 28 July 2023 / Accepted: 8 August 2023 / Published: 9 August 2023

Abstract

:
To live a healthy lifestyle, urban residents need contact with nature and restoration in a natural environment. Environmental psychology has investigated the types and features of natural environments that could be optimal for restoration. Using a sample of undergraduates from the Czech Republic, the present study explored whether attractive and open natural environments are liked more and perceived as more restorative than unattractive and closed environments. Furthermore, this study explored which spatio-cognitive dimensions and emotional qualities of the environments are associated with the liking and perceived restoration of the environments. It was found that attractive and open environments were liked significantly more and had a significantly higher level of perceived restoration than attractive closed environments, but in the nonattractive environments, the openness of the environments had no significant effects on liking and perceived restoration. Although we only found a significant contribution of the spatio-cognitive dimension of mystery to liking and perceived restoration, emotional qualities of the environment were a good predictor for the liking and perceived restoration of natural environments. The effects of the aesthetic qualities of images and the photographic techniques used should also be considered. The results are discussed in connection with the fact that preference for attractive landscapes may lead to an underestimation of the value of ordinary nature in neighborhoods.

1. Introduction

In recent decades, a gradual movement of residents from rural areas to urban centers has occurred [1]. Among the solutions to many problems linked with extensive urbanization, a healthy lifestyle requires contact with nature and restoration in a natural environment. One of the research goals of environmental psychology is to investigate the features of natural environments that are optimal for restoration. For example, an important question involves the differences in the perception of the attractiveness and restorative potential of ordinary nature from our neighborhoods vs. natural environments from attractive tourist destinations. In this context, de Groot and van den Born [2] investigated preferences for different types of landscapes in a sample of Dutch residents. The majority of respondents preferred attractive environments where the greatness and power of nature can be experienced. The preference was related to education and age; people with higher education and younger people preferred this type of landscape far more than people with less education and older people. The problem is that these preference patterns may lead to an underestimation of the restorative potential of the easily accessible domestic landscapes in a neighborhood and may lead to the need for vacations in attractive distant destinations. However, intensive and heavy tourism has negative consequences for the environment. The present study aims to explore preferences for different types of landscapes and their estimated restorative potential among a sample of undergraduates from the Czech Republic and to analyze the factors associated with these preferences.

1.1. Environmental Preference

One of the key distinctions between people’s environmental perceptions and preferences is the difference between natural and built scenes [3]. There is mounting evidence that Western people prefer unthreatening natural environments over urban environments [4,5,6,7]. It has been found in a number of studies that landscapes that are perceived as natural are considered more scenic than clearly human-influenced (cultural) landscapes [8,9,10,11,12,13]. Apart from evolutionary assumptions [14], an explanation for this difference in environmental preference is that natural environments support more positive (aesthetic, recreational) experiences and, thus, psychological wellbeing more generally than built ones (e.g., [3]). Research suggests that environmental preference and restoration are closely related [15,16]; moreover, higher preferences for environments seem to be associated with greater affective restoration (e.g., [16,17]).
A number of studies have explored environmental features that influence aesthetic landscape values. Existing literature suggests that plants or water can enhance aesthetic preference or mental restoration [18,19]. Flowers can improve aesthetic preference [20,21] and contribute to psychological well-being [22]. Water is an important factor that determines landscape preference [5,23,24,25,26]. Research shows differences in the degree of preference for types of water. The top-rated types are oceans [27,28], water with high mountains [29], and waterfalls [30]. Landscapes with a more structured appearance are preferred over those that are perceived as less structured [31]. Mean levels of diversity and complexity are generally preferred over highly complex or homogeneous patterns [32,33,34].

1.2. “Mundane” vs. Attractive Natural Scenes

De Groot and van den Born [2] investigated the preferences for different types of landscapes among a representative sample of the Dutch population. The choice was among the following types of landscape: (1) a landscape modified for human needs, (2) a scenic cultural landscape (meadows, fields, forests), (3) an unmodified natural setting, allowing the visitor many interactions, and (4) a landscape where one can experience the greatness and power of nature (sea, mountains). The majority of respondents preferred the fourth type of landscape. Interestingly, this type of landscape was preferred not only by younger people but also by middle-aged people. The preference was related to education, with people with higher education preferring the fourth type of landscape far more than people with less education.
As de Groot and van den Born’s study [2] showed, there are individual differences in preference between managed rural and wild landscapes. It seems that a place of residence is the most important factor. Rural residents often express negative attitudes toward protecting nearby wilderness areas (e.g., [35,36]) and express lower preferences for wilderness landscapes than urban residents [2,37,38]. Preferences for natural landscapes also vary with age [37,39,40]. Older people display low preferences for wild nature that may be explained by their greater physical vulnerability, which may make them more sensitive to the various dangers of wilderness areas. Moreover, socioeconomic status is another factor that influences preferences for rural versus wild landscapes. People with high incomes and education prefer wild nature more [41,42].

1.3. Spatio-Cognitive Dimensions Associated with Environmental Preference

Several decades ago, in their pilot work, Kaplan and Kaplan [3] explored the dichotomy of natural vs. built environments and proposed that natural environments dispose with specific visual cognitive dimensions and therefore are liked and preferred over built ones. They proposed four basic spatio-cognitive dimensions: (1) coherence, or the degree to which elements of the environments are related and logically organized: the more coherent an environment is, the greater the preference for it; (2) legibility, or the extent to which elements allow an observer to understand the environment and its content: the more legible it is, the greater the preference; (3) complexity, which involves the number or diversity of elements in the environment: the more complexity there is, the greater the preference; and (4) mystery, or the degree to which the environment contains hidden information. These dimensions have been employed widely in environmental preference research (for a review, see [43]). Kaplan and Kaplan’s work was continued by Herzog [30,44], and in addition to these four basic dimensions, he proposed an additional spatio-cognitive dimension.

1.4. Openness of the Environment

Another important physical feature of the environment that influences people’s perceptions and preferences is spatial openness. Appleton’s [14] evolutionary prospect–refuge theory proposed that a landscape allowing a prospect is important for survival and is preferred. Furthermore, according to Fisher and Nasar [45], a level of prospect and refuge for a potential offender are taken into consideration when people judge how safe they feel in an environment. This was supported by a number of studies that showed that the feeling of danger from a possible attack may increase in a closed environment (e.g., [46,47,48,49]). For instance, it was shown that in tree alleys, there was greater perceived danger than in open landscapes with fields [48]. For alleys, danger was a more common reaction than perceived mystery of the environment. Simultaneously, a number of studies have confirmed that the spatial openness of the landscape is a factor that positively influences its preference. For instance, how well residents can see the landscape with an open view is a significant predictor of perceived landscape quality [50]; similarly, choices of preferred landscape may be driven by the preference for landscape openness [51,52]. Moreover, a recent investigation from the field of neuroscience showed that spatial information about “openness” plays an important role in the scene-responsive parahippocampal area in the brain, even in different scene categories (e.g., [53,54]).

1.5. Affective Qualities of the Environment

Physical environments possess affective qualities that induce expressions of pleasure, happiness, attraction, enjoyment, preference, etc. [55]. Landscape preference can also be predicted by diverse emotional responses [56]. The related stress reduction theory [10] emphasizes the role of the natural environment in reducing stress and the association of perception of this type of environment with positive emotional responses. According to this theory, exposure to environments with vegetation, water, and other natural features produces a response characterized by decreased physiological arousal, increases in positive affect, and decreases in negative affect. This theory has been supported by a large body of research [57,58,59,60,61]. The research was mostly directed by the circumplex model of Russell and Pratt [62], which proposed two dimensions, pleasure (joy, happiness, etc.) and arousal (interest, activity, surprise, etc.), to explain the differences in emotional responses to the physical environment.

1.6. The Present Study

The present study aims to contribute to research on preferred and restorative environments. The first goal was to explore the effect of the attractiveness and spatial openness of environments on people’s liking and perceived restoration. We predicted that attractive environments are liked more and perceived as more restorative than unattractive environments and that spatially open environments are liked more and perceived as more restorative than spatially closed environments. The second goal was to explore the association of visual spatio-cognitive dimensions of the environment with the likeability and perceived restoration of the environments. We predict that a high perceived level of the dimension coherence, complexity, and mystery of images will be associated with higher likeability and a higher level of perceived restoration. The third goal was to explore which emotional qualities are associated with the liking and perceived restoration of the environments. We predict that emotional categories with positive valence will be positively associated with liking and perceived restoration, while emotional categories with negative valence will be negatively associated with liking and perceived restoration. Finally, we explored gender differences in liking and perceived restoration of the selected environments without any specific prediction. This study follows de Groot’s and van den Born’s [2] investigation. While this study used a questionnaire, where particular types of landscapes were described, our investigation is based on the perception and evaluation of visual stimuli in laboratory conditions.

2. Study 1: Evaluation of Attractiveness of Selected Images

The perception of environmental attractiveness is somewhat individual. Thus, before exploring the effect of the attractiveness of the environments on individual’s liking and feeling of restoration, the estimated level of attractiveness of selected images was assessed. The second variable, the spatial openness/closedness of a natural scene, was defined as an open natural landscape vs. a closed scene inside a forest. The perceived level of spatial openness was not tested because we selected well-marked and distinct examples of spatial openness; therefore, openness of the environment was an unambiguous feature of the images used.

2.1. Material and Methods

2.1.1. Sample

Fifty-three undergraduates participated in the study. The sample comprised undergraduates between the ages of 18 and 25 (mean = 21.2, SD = 1.69, 27 males, 26 females). The participants were enrolled in various psychology courses, and they were students in informatics, financial management, or tourism at the University of Hradec Králové.

2.1.2. Stimulus Material

The stimuli were photographs representing diverse natural settings. The intention was to present attractive or unattractive mundane natural scenes that were either open or closed. Based on these criteria, we selected 16 images from the set of visual stimuli used in our previous study [63]. They consisted of four images of attractive and open environments, four images of attractive and closed environments, four images of unattractive and open environments, and four images of unattractive and closed environments (Figure 1).
The attractive images were downloaded from the Pixabay internet server, which shares copyright-free images (https://pixabay.com/, accessed on 10 June 2023), and the unattractive images were taken by one of the authors. The attractive open images were represented by high mountains, lakes, or coastal scenery. These scenes were substantially different from the landscape of the Czech Republic, where participants of the study were living. In contrast, the unattractive open scenes represented the typical landscape of the Czech Republic. Attractive closed scenes were also downloaded from Pixabay and depicted outdoor environments of the Czech Republic, while unattractive closed scenes were taken in Czech forests. The attractive scenes were professional photographs taken and adjusted with the aim of great visual effect, while the images of unattractive scenes were taken under normal lighting conditions, without any further digital adjustment.

2.1.3. Procedure

Images were presented online via Google Forms in a random order. The participants evaluated the attractiveness of the images through agreement/disagreement with the phrase “I find this environment very attractive”. Their responses were assessed on a five-point scale (1 = not at all, 5 = completely).

2.2. Results and Discussion

The mean ratings of individual categories of photographs are shown in Table 1. Next, a two-way ANOVA was conducted to assess the effects of the attractiveness and openness of presented images, as well as gender, on their estimated attractiveness. Attractiveness (attractive vs. unattractive) and openness (open vs. closed) were selected as within-subject factors, gender as a between-subject factor, and the estimated attractiveness as a dependent variable. There was a statistically significant two-way interaction between attractiveness and openness (F1,204 = 12.852, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.059). The main effects of the attractiveness of images had a significant effect on the estimated attractiveness (F1,204 = 62.113, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.233), but the main effects of the openness of images (F1,204 = 2.509, p = 0.115, partial η2 = 0.012) and gender (F1,204 = 0.059, p = 0.807, partial η2 = 0.001) had no significant effects on estimated attractiveness. In the attractive environments, there was a statistically significant difference in the estimated attractiveness between open (mean = 4.44, SD = 0.48) and non-open (mean = 3.85, SD = 0.84) environments, with a mean difference of 0.559 (95% CI, 0.162 to 1.030) estimated attractiveness points. In the nonattractive environments, estimated attractiveness was not statistically significantly different in the open environments (mean = 3.12, SD = 0.88) compared to non-open environments (mean = 3.35, SD = 1.04), with a difference of −0.23 (95% CI, −0.663 to 0.200) estimated attractiveness points.
The results revealed that the images predicted to be attractive were rated higher in attractiveness. The attractive images were estimated to be significantly more attractive than the unattractive images when they represented open environments, but the difference in the attractiveness ratings of attractive and unattractive environments was not significant if they represented closed environments.

3. Study 2: The Effect of the Attractiveness and Openness of Images on Their Likeability and Perceived Restoration

The study investigated the effect of the attractiveness and spatial openness of environments on their likeability and perceived restoration. Furthermore, associations of visual spatio-cognitive dimensions and emotional qualities of the environment with the liking and perceived restoration of the environments were explored.

3.1. Material and Methods

3.1.1. Sample

Fifty-one undergraduates participated in the experiment. The sample comprised undergraduates between the ages of 19 and 25 (mean = 20.9, SD = 1.28, 29 males, 22 females). The participants were enrolled in various psychology courses, and they were students in informatics, financial management, or tourism at the University of Hradec Králové. The sample was not identical to the sample from Study 1.

3.1.2. Procedure

The study was conducted in a laboratory. After arrival, the participants signed the informed consent form. Afterward, the following instruction appeared on the computer screen: “You will see various natural images. Look at them, try to imagine that you are in this natural environment right now, and describe your perceptions and feelings by the statements, which will be given under each image”. Sixteen images tested in Study 1 were presented. The stimuli were presented in a random order on a computer screen with a 1366 × 768-pixel resolution screen and a diagonal of 38 cm.

3.1.3. Measures

A questionnaire registered assessment of (1) three spatio-cognitive dimensions: coherence, complexity, and mystery; (2) four basic emotions: joy, sadness, surprise, and fear; and finally, (3) whether the participants liked the image and whether (4) the image represented a restorative environment. Each item was assessed on a five-point scale (1 = not at all, 5 = completely). The questionnaire was presented to the participants on a computer screen. The data are available in the Supplementary Material Table S1: dataset.
  • Spatio-cognitive dimensions: The item “The individual features of this place are in harmony; they belong together” was related to the spatio-cognitive dimension of coherence, the item “This place contains a large number of various elements” was related to the spatio-cognitive dimension of complexity, and the item “I would like to explore this interesting place more” was related to the spatio-cognitive dimension of mystery. These items were selected from the study by Herzog and Bosley [30].
  • Emotions: Two items were related to the pleasure dimension. The item “I feel happy here” was related to joy and the item “This is a pretty sad place” was related to sadness. The next two items were related to the arousal dimension. The item “I feel amazed here” was related to surprise and the item “This place scares me a little” was related to fear.
  • Liking: Liking was assessed by the item “I like this place”.
  • Restoration: Restoration was assessed by the item “This environment offers relaxation, calming and an escape from everyday stress.”

3.2. Results

3.2.1. Influence of Attractiveness and Openness on Liking the Images

Two-way ANOVA was conducted to assess the effects of attractiveness, openness, and gender on liking the images. Attractiveness (attractive vs. unattractive) and openness (open vs. closed) were selected as within-subject factors, gender was selected as a between-subject factor, and liking was chosen as a dependent variable. There was a statistically significant two-way interaction between attractiveness and openness (F1,200 = 8.426, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.400) and an almost statistically significant two-way interaction between attractiveness and gender (F1,200 = 7.729, p = 0.059, partial η2 = 0.030). The main effects of attractiveness (F1,200 = 73.449, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.277) and openness (F1,200 = 8.117, p < 0.01, partial η2 = 0.039) were significant, but the main effect of gender (F1,200 = 0.902, p = 0.343, partial η2 = 0.004) was not significant. In the attractive environments, there was a statistically significant difference in liking between open (mean = 4.130, SD = 0.69) and non-open (mean = 3.572, SD = 0.67) environments, with a mean difference of 0.558 (95% CI, 0.189 to 0.927) liking evaluation points. In the nonattractive environments, liking was not statistically significantly different in the open environments (mean = 2.995, SD = 0.74) compared to non-open environments (mean = 3.000, SD = 0.73), with a difference of −0.005 (95% CI, −0.037 to 0.364) liking evaluation points. Thus, attractiveness and openness had a combined effect, and attractive and open environments were liked more than attractive closed environments. However, in nonattractive environments, the openness/closedness of the environment had no effect on liking. Moreover, an almost significant interaction between gender and attractiveness showed that, while in the liking of attractive images there was no difference between males and females, females (mean = 3.192, SD = 0.69) liked unattractive images more than males (mean = 2.830, SD = 0.73), with a mean difference of 0.362 (95% CI, −0.749 to 0.025) liking evaluation points.

3.2.2. Influence of Attractiveness and Openness on Perceived Restoration

Two-way ANOVA was conducted to assess the effects of attractiveness and openness on the perceived restoration of the environment that the images contained. Attractiveness (attractive vs. unattractive) and openness (open vs. closed) were within-subject factors, gender was a between-subject factor, and perceived restoration was chosen as a dependent variable. There was a statistically significant two-way interaction between attractiveness and openness (F1,200 = 12.436, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.059). The main effects of attractiveness (F1,200 = 66.341, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.249) and openness (F1,200 = 9.260, p < 0.01, partial η2 = 0.044) were also significant. The main effect of gender (F1,200 = 0.320, p = 0.572, partial η2 = 0.002) was not significant. In the attractive environments, there was a statistically significant difference in restoration between open (mean = 4.111, SD = 0.51) and non-open (mean = 3.538, SD = 0.55) environments, with a mean difference of 0.572 (95% CI, 0.249 to 0.895) evaluation points. In the nonattractive environments, restoration was not statistically significantly different in the open environments (mean = 3.091, SD = 0.72) compared to non-open environments (mean = 3.013, SD = 0.66), with a mean difference of −0.043 (95% CI, −0.366 to 0.279) evaluation points. Thus, attractiveness and openness had a combined effect, and attractive and open environments had a higher level of perceived restoration than attractive closed environments. However, in nonattractive environments, the openness/closedness of the environment had no effect on perceived restoration.

3.2.3. Associations between Spatio-Cognitive Dimensions and Environment Liking

Multiple regressions were carried out to find the contributions of spatio-cognitive dimensions of coherence, complexity, mystery, and gender to image liking. The calculations were conducted separately for four types of environments—attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed. The results (Table 2) showed that only the dimension of mystery significantly contributed to liking, but only in attractive closed (R2 = 0.196, F4,47 = 2.863, p < 0.05), unattractive open (R2 = 0.343, F4,47 = 6.141, p < 0.001), and unattractive closed environments (R2 = 0.386, F4,7 = 7.395, p < 0.05).

3.2.4. Associations between Spatio-Cognitive Dimensions and Perceived Restoration

Multiple regressions were carried out to find the contributions of the spatio-cognitive dimensions of coherence, complexity, mystery, and gender to perceived restoration. The calculations were conducted separately for different types of environments—attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed. Similar to the previous analysis, the results (Table 3) showed that only the dimension of mystery significantly contributed to perceived restoration, but only in unattractive open (R2 = 0.277, F4,47 = 4.490, p < 0.01) and unattractive closed environments (R2 = 0.307, F4,47 = 5.214, p < 0.01).

3.2.5. Associations between Emotional Categories and Environment Liking

Multiple regressions were carried out to find the contributions of the emotional categories of joy, surprise, fear, sadness, and gender to image liking. The calculations were conducted separately for four types of environments—attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed (Table 4). In attractive open environments, the results showed a statistically significant association between the emotional category of surprise and liking of images (R2 = 0.403, F5,46 = 6.337, p < 0.001). In attractive closed environments, the results also showed a statistically significant association between the emotional category of surprise and the liking of images (R2 = 0. 365, F5,46 = 5.288, p < 0.001). In the unattractive open environment, the results showed a statistically significant association between the emotional categories of joy and surprise and the liking of images (R2 = 0.640, F5,46 = 16.284, p < 0.001). In an unattractive closed environment, the results showed a statistically significant association between the emotional category of joy and the liking of images (R2 = 0.626, F5,46 = 15.426, p < 0.001).

3.2.6. Associations between Emotional Categories and Perceived Restoration

Multiple regressions were carried out to find the contributions of the emotional categories of joy, surprise, fear, sadness, and gender to perceived restoration. The calculations were conducted separately for four types of environments—attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed (Table 5). In attractive open environments, the results showed a statistically significant positive association between the emotional category of surprise and perceived restoration and a statistically significant negative association between the emotional category of fear and perceived restoration (R2 = 0.686, F5,46 = 20.516, p < 0.001). In attractive closed environments, the results showed a statistically significant association between the emotional category of joy and perceived restoration (R2 = 0.553, F5,46 = 11.3703, p < 0.001). In the unattractive open environments, the results showed a statistically significant positive association between the emotional categories of joy and sadness and perceived restoration and an almost significant (p = 0.054) negative association between fear and perceived restoration (R2 = 0.685, F5,46 = 20.024, p < 0.001). In an unattractive closed environment, the results showed a statistically significant positive association between the emotional categories of joy and sadness and perceived restoration and a significant negative association between the emotional category of fear and perceived restoration (R2 = 0.710, F5,46 = 22.473, p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

The present study aimed to explore whether attractive environments are liked more and perceived as more restorative than unattractive environments and whether spatially open environments are liked more and perceived as more restorative than spatially closed environments. Furthermore, we explored which spatio-cognitive dimensions and emotional qualities of an environment are associated with the likeability and perceived restoration of the environment.
We found that the effect of the attractiveness of the environment mediated the effect of openness. Attractive and open environments were liked significantly more and had a significantly higher level of perceived restoration than attractive closed environments and both unattractive environments, but in the nonattractive environments, the openness of the environments had no significant effects on liking and perceived restoration. In accordance with these findings, the attractive images were perceived as significantly more attractive than unattractive images, but only if they represented open environments.
However, the preference for landscape openness is likely to be a more complex issue. For instance, certain findings show that liking for open landscapes can be culturally dependent. A more recent study [64] that compared landscape preferences among Swedish and non-Western samples (East Timor, Malaysia, Colombia, Venezuela, and Guianas) showed that participants from the non-Western samples favored more forested settings, i.e., a low degree of openness. Furthermore, it seems that mental states can also influence landscape preferences in terms of their naturalness and openness. People in a fatigued state [65] have a greater preference for landscapes with a higher naturalness and those in an indignant state have a greater preference for relatively private, less-open landscapes. Our data are consistent with the findings by de Groot and van den Born [2], who showed that the majority of Dutch respondents preferred an attractive environment, where the greatness and power of nature can be experienced. Similarly, our respondents preferred similar environments over unattractive ones. Our images of attractive open environments consisted of photographs of high mountains, rocks, or seashores with high rocks, while attractive closed environments represented scenes amidst dense vegetation and forests. In contrast, both unattractive open and closed images represented scenes from common, slightly undulating landscapes of central areas of the Czech Republic. Consistently, de Groot and van den Born [2] registered that younger people with higher education preferred this type of landscape more than people with less education and older people. Our participants were university undergraduates.
Although environmental preference studies did not reveal systematic gender difference, our data in Study 2 showed that females tended to like unattractive images more than males did. However, the effect was small and was at the border of the conventionally respected value of statistical significance (p = 0.059). This may be explained in terms of traditional gender stereotypes—males prefer more adventures provided by attractive landscapes (e.g., climbing in high mountains) than females. Clearly, because the effect was small and almost significant, this finding can reflect individual differences in the specific sample and cannot be generalized.
It is worth considering how the environment in which people live can influence their environmental preferences. The Netherlands has a flat landscape, and therefore, it could be predicted that people may wish to spend their holidays and vacations in entirely different landscapes. While the landscape of the Czech Republic is undulating and more varied, although it lacks very high mountains, it is mostly not considered to be an attractive environment where “the greatness and power of nature can be experienced”. In addition, the participants of our study mostly live in a flat and rural area of the country where the university is located. Therefore, it would be interesting to explore landscape preferences for the restoration of people living in substantially different landscapes, for instance, in the wilderness, high mountains, or attractive coastal areas.
Furthermore, we investigated which spatio-cognitive dimensions and emotional qualities are associated with the liking and perceived restoration of the environments. We explored associations with basic spatio-cognitive dimensions of coherence, complexity, and mystery proposed by Kaplan and Kaplan [3] that should be associated with environmental preference. However, more recently, Joye and Dewitte [66] argued that the framework of this theory, although it has been highly influential in the field of restoration studies, has important empirical and conceptual shortcomings which require further empirical elaboration. It is not surprising that our study did not fully confirm that the liking and perceived restoration of the environments should be associated with these dimensions. We only found a significant contribution of the spatio-cognitive dimension of mystery for liking and perceived restoration in both unattractive open and closed environments. Thus, the mystery of “mundane”, ordinary natural environments increases their liking and perceived restoration. Although we did not test the association between attractiveness and mystery, it is possible that some form of mystery increases the attractiveness of unattractive environments, making them more attractive for restoration purposes.
In contrast, the emotional qualities of the environment were good predictors for liking and perceived restoration of natural environments. It is not surprising that the emotions of joy and surprise are associated positively with liking and perceived restoration and that the emotion of fear was negatively associated with these metrics. Curiously, sadness was associated with perceived restoration in both types of unattractive environments but not with liking the environment. Perhaps sadness was associated with some form of nostalgia, perhaps with memories of experiences from the neighboring environment from childhood. However, this association needs to be more deeply analyzed, and our data cannot provide a clearly convincing explanation.
It is worth commenting on the extent to which the photographic techniques used and the visual quality of the photographs themselves could influence an individual’s liking of presented images. The study by de Groot and van den Born [2] used a questionnaire where the landscapes were described verbally, which is an appropriate method in a sociological survey but not in a study based on visual perception. In an ideal situation, the investigation of environmental preference should be conducted in situ. Although there are studies that investigate participants’ reactions in situ, either comparing differences between urban and natural walks (e.g., [58,67]) or forest experiences (e.g., [68,69]), this research procedure is not very frequent, and for understandable reasons, it is not applicable in this type of study. For a long time, environmental preference research has used environmental simulations, from simple black-and-white photos to color slides, videos, and virtual reality. Stamps [70], in his meta-analysis carried out in the 1990s, concluded that the different simulations (drawings and black-and-white and color photographs) produced very similar preferences. Environmental simulations in various forms are still frequently used in environmental psychology research (for a review, see [71]); however, the issue of the aesthetic quality of environmental simulations and its influence on preference has not been adequately explored. There is only one study by Tinia and Leder [72], in which the authors performed manipulations of photographs of natural and built environments to verify whether image quality influenced environment preference. A degraded version of a photograph was produced by manipulations of the sharpness, noise and grain, contrast, color fidelity, and color saturation. They found that the visual quality of the images could influence preference judgments, but it had this effect for built scenes rather than natural ones. Specifically, built scenes of high image quality were liked more than natural scenes of low image quality. This shows that the quality of a photograph can influence an individual’s evaluation of an environment. On the other hand, a photograph is only a representation of a specific environment, which a person can imagine in its real form. That is why we gave the instruction “Try to imagine that you are in this natural environment right now, and describe your perceptions and feelings”. This should, at least, partially ensure that the participants did not describe their perception of the specific photograph, but the environment which is represented by the photographs. However, the influence of visual quality of the environmental simulation is a question that should be further investigated. Finally, we considered the problem that a preference for attractive nature that can only be reached by traveling to distant tourist destinations may lead to the need for vacations in attractive distant destinations. Moreover, it might lead to underestimation of the restorative potential of easily accessible domestic landscapes in a neighborhood. Clearly, “heavy” tourism has negative consequences for the environment. In this context, several decades ago, Knighton [73] called the tendency to prefer attractive nature and underestimate the value of ordinary nature in one’s own neighborhood “eco-porn”. Similarly, Levi and Kocher [74] documented that this tendency may result in support for the protection of attractive natural environments in national parks but, on the other hand, a decrease in interest in the natural environment where one resides.
The findings from recent post-industrial societies revealed that school children are not able to identify even the most common plants (e.g., [75]) and, curiously, they are sometimes more familiar with exotic flagship species than with their local fauna (e.g., [76]). This is the challenge for various forms of environmental education, although on the other hand, the effect of environmental education can be reduced by the rapid development of modern digital technologies, namely in many members of the younger generation. Thus, the implication is to further investigate environmental preferences, including the factors that influence them, and explore how to promote the value of local natural landscapes, even if they are not as attractive as others.
This study has several limitations. The research was conducted with only a small sample of young people who were university students; thus, it does not give information about the preferences of the whole population of one country, as the study by de Groot and van den Born [2] provided. On the other hand, the research (Study 2) was conducted individually in laboratory conditions, which enabled us to control possible side effects, but limited the number of participants. Although a student sample might be a limitation, the number of undergraduates in the population is gradually increasing; they are an active social group and, moreover, their preferences for future life are formed in their youth.
A further limitation could also be the small number of images used in our study. On the other hand, after an extensive number of investigations of environmental preference and the effects of natural environments, specific types of environments, their perception, evaluation, and preference have already been identified; thus, we suppose that the small number of visual stimuli in our study has not been an essential drawback.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bs13080666/s1, Table S1: dataset.

Funding

This research was funded by the Faculty of Informatics and Management at the University of Hradec Králové, the Student Specific Research Grants 1/2023.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Committee for Research Ethics at the University of Hradec Králové, No. 4/2018, 8 January 2019.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets supporting this article have been uploaded as part of the Supplementary Material.

Acknowledgments

We thank Tadeáš Němec, Jan Nálevka, Jan Petružálek, and Michal Voda for their help in conducting the experiment.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. World Urbanization Prospects: The 2018 Revision-Population; United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs: New York, NY, USA. 2018. Available online: https://www.un.org/en/desa/2018-revision-world-urbanization-prospects (accessed on 29 May 2023).
  2. de Groot, W.T.; van den Born, R.J.G. Visions of Nature and Landscape Type Preferences: An Exploration in The Netherlands. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2003, 63, 127–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Kaplan, R.; Kaplan, S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  4. Knopf, R.C. Recreational Needs and Behavior in Natural Settings. In Behavior and the Natural Environment; Altman, I., Wohlwill, J.F., Eds.; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 205–240. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ulrich, R.S. Aesthetic and Affective Response to Natural Environment. In Behavior and the Natural Environment; Altman, I., Wohlwill, J.F., Eds.; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 85–125. [Google Scholar]
  6. Hartig, T.; Evans, G.W.; Jamner, L.D.; Davis, D.S.; Gärling, T. Tracking Restoration in Natural and Urban Field Settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. van den Berg, A.E.; Hartig, T.; Staats, H. Preference for Nature in Urbanized Societies: Stress, Restoration, and the Pursuit of Sustainability. J. Soc. Issues 2007, 63, 79–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Zube, E.H.; Sell, J.L.; Taylor, J.G. Landscape Perception: Research, Application and Theory. Landsc. Plan. 1982, 9, 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hull, R.B.; Reveli, G.R.B. Cross-Cultural Comparison of Landscape Scenic Beauty Evaluations: A Case Study in Bali. J. Environ. Psychol. 1989, 9, 177–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Losito, B.D.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.A.; Zelson, M. Stress Recovery during Exposure to Natural and Urban Environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Purcell, A.T. Landscape Perception, Preference, and Schema Discrepancy. Environ. Plan. B 1987, 14, 67–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kent, R.L.; Elliott, C.L. Scenic Routes Linking and Protecting Natural and Cultural Landscape Features: A Greenway Skeleton. Landsc. Urban Plan. 1995, 33, 341–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Real, E.; Arce, C.; Manuel Sabucedo, J. Classification of Landscapes Using Quantitative and Categorical Data, and Prediction of Their Scenic Beauty in North-Western Spain. J. Environ. Psychol. 2000, 20, 355–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Appleton, J. The Experience of Landscape; Rev. ed.; Wiley: Chichester, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  15. Korpela, K.; Hartig, T. Restorative Qualities of Favorite Places. J. Environ. Psychol. 1996, 16, 221–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. van den Berg, A.E.; Koole, S.L.; van der Wulp, N.Y. Environmental Preference and Restoration: (How) Are They Related? J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 135–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Herzog, T.R.; Colleen Maguire, P.; Nebel, M.B. Assessing the Restorative Components of Environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 159–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Jiang, B.; Chang, C.-Y.; Sullivan, W.C. A Dose of Nature: Tree Cover, Stress Reduction, and Gender Differences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 132, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Graves, R.A.; Pearson, S.M.; Turner, M.G. Species Richness Alone Does Not Predict Cultural Ecosystem Service Value. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 3774–3779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Wang, R.; Zhao, J.; Meitner, M.J. Urban Woodland Understory Characteristics in Relation to Aesthetic and Recreational Preference. Urban For. Urban Green. 2017, 24, 55–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hoyle, H.; Hitchmough, J.; Jorgensen, A. All about the ‘Wow Factor’? The Relationships between Aesthetics, Restorative Effect and Perceived Biodiversity in Designed Urban Planting. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 164, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Haviland-Jones, J.D.; Hale, H.R.; Wilson, P.; McGuire, T.R. An Environmental Approach to Positive Emotion: Flowers. Evol. Psychol. 2005, 3, 104–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. White, M.; Smith, A.; Humphryes, K.; Pahl, S.; Snelling, D.; Depledge, M. Blue Space: The Importance of Water for Preference, Affect, and Restorativeness Ratings of Natural and Built Scenes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 482–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Völker, S.; Kistemann, T. Developing the Urban Blue: Comparative Health Responses to Blue and Green Urban Open Spaces in Germany. Health Place 2015, 35, 196–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Wang, R.; Zhao, J.; Meitner, M.J.; Hu, Y.; Xu, X. Characteristics of Urban Green Spaces in Relation to Aesthetic Preference and Stress Recovery. Urban For. Urban Green. 2019, 41, 6–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kaltenborn, B.P.; Bjerke, T. Associations between Environmental Value Orientations and Landscape Preferences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2002, 59, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Mangone, G.; Dopko, R.L.; Zelenski, J.M. Deciphering Landscape Preferences: Investigating the Roles of Familiarity and Biome Types. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2021, 214, 104189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Yuan, S.; Browning, M.H.E.M.; McAnirlin, O.; Sindelar, K.; Shin, S.; Drong, G.; Hoptman, D.; Heller, W. A Virtual Reality Investigation of Factors Influencing Landscape Preferences: Natural Elements, Emotions, and Media Creation. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2023, 230, 104616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Herzog, T.R. A Cognitive Analysis of Preference for Waterscapes. J. Environ. Psychol. 1985, 5, 225–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Herzog, T.R.; Bosley, P.J. Tranquility and Preference as Affective Qualities of Natural Environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1992, 2, 115–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lückmann, K.; Lagemann, V.; Menzel, S. Landscape Assessment and Evaluation of Young People: Comparing Nature-Orientated Habitat and Engineered Habitat Preferences. Environ. Behav. 2013, 45, 86–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ode, Å.; Fry, G.; Tveit, M.S.; Messager, P.; Miller, D. Indicators of Perceived Naturalness as Drivers of Landscape Preference. J. Environ. Manag. 2009, 90, 375–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Liu, X.; Tvinnereim, E.; Grimsrud, K.M.; Lindhjem, H.; Velle, L.G.; Saure, H.I.; Lee, H. Explaining Landscape Preference Heterogeneity Using Machine Learning-Based Survey Analysis. Landsc. Res. 2021, 46, 417–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Schirpke, U.; Tasser, E.; Tappeiner, U. Predicting Scenic Beauty of Mountain Regions. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 111, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Fiallo, E.A.; Jacobson, S.K. Local Communities and Protected Areas: Attitudes of Rural Residents Towards Conservation and Machalilla National Park, Ecuador. Environ. Conserv. 1995, 22, 241–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Durrant, J.O.; Shumway, J.M. Attitudes Toward Wilderness Study Areas: A Survey of Six Southeastern Utah Counties. Environ. Manag. 2004, 33, 271–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Strumse, E. Demographic Differences in the Visual Preferences for Agrarian Landscapes in Western Norway. J. Environ. Psychol. 1996, 16, 17–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Van den Berg, A.E.; Koole, S.L. New Wilderness in the Netherlands: An Investigation of Visual Preferences for Nature Development Landscapes. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2006, 78, 362–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Balling, J.D.; Falk, J.H. Development of Visual Preference for Natural Environments. Environ. Behav. 1982, 14, 5–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lyons, E. Demographic Correlates of Landscape Preference. Environ. Behav. 1983, 15, 487–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Roggenbuck, J.W.; Lucas, R.C. Wilderness Use and User Characteristics: A State-of-Knowledge Review. Gen. Tech. Rep. INT 1987, 220, 204–246. [Google Scholar]
  42. Virden, R.J. A Comparison Study of Wilderness Users and Nonusers: Implications for Managers and Policymakers. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 1990, 8, 3–24. [Google Scholar]
  43. Yang, G.; Yu, Z.; Zhang, J.; Søderkvist Kristensen, L. From Preference to Landscape Sustainability: A Bibliometric Review of Landscape Preference Research from 1968 to 2019. Ecosyst. Health Sustain. 2021, 7, 1948355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Herzog, T.R. A Cognitive Analysis of Preference for Urban Nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 1989, 9, 27–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Fisher, B.S.; Nasar, J.L. Fear of Crime in Relation to Three Exterior Site Features: Prospect, Refuge, and Escape. Environ. Behav. 1992, 24, 35–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Andrews, M.; Gatersleben, B. Variations in Perceptions of Danger, Fear and Preference in a Simulated Natural Environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 473–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Chiang, Y.-C.; Nasar, J.L.; Ko, C.-C. Influence of Visibility and Situational Threats on Forest Trail Evaluations. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 125, 166–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gatersleben, B.; Andrews, M. When Walking in Nature Is Not Restorative—The Role of Prospect and Refuge. Health Place 2013, 20, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Nasar, J.L.; Jones, K.M. Landscapes of Fear and Stress. Environ. Behav. 1997, 29, 291–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Wartmann, F.M.; Frick, J.; Kienast, F.; Hunziker, M. Factors Influencing Visual Landscape Quality Perceived by the Public. Results from a National Survey. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2021, 208, 104024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Sahraoui, Y.; Clauzel, C.; Foltête, J.-C. Spatial Modelling of Landscape Aesthetic Potential in Urban-Rural Fringes. J. Environ. Manag. 2016, 181, 623–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Liu, Q.; Zhu, Z.; Zeng, X.; Zhuo, Z.; Ye, B.; Fang, L.; Huang, Q.; Lai, P. The Impact of Landscape Complexity on Preference Ratings and Eye Fixation of Various Urban Green Space Settings. Urban For. Urban Green. 2021, 66, 127411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Kravitz, D.J.; Peng, C.S.; Baker, C.I. Real-World Scene Representations in High-Level Visual Cortex: It’s the Spaces More Than the Places. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 7322–7333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Zhang, H.; Houpt, J.W.; Harel, A. Establishing Reference Scales for Scene Naturalness and Openness: Naturalness and Openness Scales. Behaviral Res. 2019, 51, 1179–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Russell, J.A.; Snodgrass, K. Emotion and the Environment. In Handbook of Environmental Psychology; Stokols, D., Altman, I., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1987; pp. 245–281. [Google Scholar]
  56. Hull, R.B.; Harvey, A. Explaining the Emotion People Experience in Suburban Parks. Environ. Behav. 1989, 21, 323–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Hartig, T.; Korpela, K.; Evans, G.W.; Gärling, T. A Measure of Restorative Quality in Environments. Scand. Hous. Plan. Res. 1997, 14, 175–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Berman, M.G.; Jonides, J.; Kaplan, S. The Cognitive Benefits of Interacting with Nature. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 1207–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Kjellgren, A.; Buhrkall, H. A Comparison of the Restorative Effect of a Natural Environment with That of a Simulated Natural Environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Martínez-Soto, J.; Gonzales-Santos, L.; Barrios, F.A.; Lena, M.E.M.-L. Affective and Restorative Valences for Three Environmental Categories. Percept. Mot. Skills 2014, 119, 901–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. McMahan, E.A.; Estes, D. The Effect of Contact with Natural Environments on Positive and Negative Affect: A Meta-Analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 2015, 10, 507–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Russell, J.A.; Pratt, G. A Description of the Affective Quality Attributed to Environments. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 38, 311–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Franěk, M.; Petružálek, J.; Šefara, D. Facial Expressions and Self-Reported Emotions When Viewing Nature Images. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 10588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Hägerhäll, C.M.; Ode Sang, Å.; Englund, J.-E.; Ahlner, F.; Rybka, K.; Huber, J.; Burenhult, N. Do Humans Really Prefer Semi-open Natural Landscapes? A Cross-Cultural Reappraisal. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Li, K.; Zhai, Y.; Dou, L.; Liu, J. A Preliminary Exploration of Landscape Preferences Based on Naturalness and Visual Openness for College Students with Different Moods. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 629650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Joye, Y.; Dewitte, S. Nature’s Broken Path to Restoration. A Critical Look at Attention Restoration Theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 59, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Bratman, G.N.; Daily, G.C.; Levy, B.J.; Gross, J.J. The benefits of nature experience: Improved affect and cognition. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015, 138, 41–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Lee, H.J.; Son, Y.H.; Kim, S.; Lee, D.K. Healing experiences of middle-aged women through an urban forest therapy program. Urban For. Urban Green. 2019, 38, 383–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Yu, C.P.; Lin, C.M.; Tsai, M.J.; Tsai, Y.C.; Chen, C.Y. Effects of Short Forest Bathing Program on Autonomic Nervous System Activity and Mood States in Middle-aged and Elderly Individuals. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Stamps, A.E., III. Simulation Effects on Environmental Preference. J. Environ. Manag. 1993, 38, 115–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Browning, M.H.; Saeidi-Rizi, F.; McAnirlin, O.; Yoon, H.; Pei, Y. The Role of Methodological Choices in the Effects of Experimental Exposure to Simulated Natural Landscapes on Human Health and Cognitive Performance: A Systematic Review. Environ. Behav. 2021, 53, 687–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Tinio, P.P.; Leder, H. Natural Scenes Are Indeed preferred, but Image Quality Might Have the Last Word. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2009, 3, 52–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Knighton, J. Eco-Porn and the Manipulation of Desire. Harper’s 1993, 287, 14–15. [Google Scholar]
  74. Levi, D.; Kocher, S. Virtual Nature: The Future Effects of Information Technology on Our Relationship to Nature. Environ. Behav. 1999, 31, 203–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Bebbington, A. The Ability of A-level Students to Name Plants. J. Biol. Educ. 2005, 39, 63–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Genovart, M.; Tavecchia, G.; Enseñat, J.J.; Laiolo, P. Holding up a Mirror to the Society: Children Recognize Exotic Species Much More than Local Ones. Biol. Conserv. 2013, 159, 484–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Examples of the stimulus material: (a) attractive open environment, (b) attractive closed environment, (c) unattractive open environment, and (d) unattractive closed environment.
Figure 1. Examples of the stimulus material: (a) attractive open environment, (b) attractive closed environment, (c) unattractive open environment, and (d) unattractive closed environment.
Behavsci 13 00666 g001
Table 1. Mean rating of individual categories of photographs. The scale ranged from 1 to 5 (1 = not at all, 5 = completely).
Table 1. Mean rating of individual categories of photographs. The scale ranged from 1 to 5 (1 = not at all, 5 = completely).
Open Closed
MeanSDMeanSD
Attractive4.440.483.850.84
Unattractive3.120.883.351.04
Table 2. Multiple regression results for environmental liking. Predictors: spatio-cognitive dimensions of coherence, complexity, mystery, and gender. Conducted separately for attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed environments. Statistically significant results are indicated in bold.
Table 2. Multiple regression results for environmental liking. Predictors: spatio-cognitive dimensions of coherence, complexity, mystery, and gender. Conducted separately for attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed environments. Statistically significant results are indicated in bold.
BSEβtp
Attractive open
Coherence−0.0460.143−0.068−0.30200.750
Complexity0.0330.1520.0370.2150.831
Mystery0.0790.1540.1840.5100.612
Gender−0.0710.145−0.100−0.4800.626
R20.018
Adj R2−0.064
SE0.711
F4,470.218 p = 0.927
Attractive closed
Coherence0.0080.1320.0130.0670.947
Complexity0.2070.1630.1721.3360.212
Mystery0.2750.1650.2211.6700.101
Gender−0.0370.140−0.050−0.2650.792
R20.196
Adj R20.128
SE0.626
F4,472.863 p < 0.05
Unattractive open
Coherence−0.0230.130−0.030−0.1810.857
Complexity0.2640.1830.2281.4440.155
Mystery0.3440.1770.3490.94710.005
Gender0.1540.1240.2281.2430.220
R20.343
Adj R20.287
SE0.626
F4,476.141 p < 0.001
Unattractive closed
Coherence−0.0800.122−0.010−0.6540.516
Complexity0.1820.1700.1521.0700.290
Mystery0.4560.1660.4692.7500.008
Gender0.1690.1180.2451.4320.159
R20.386
Adj R20.334
SE0592
F4,477.395 p < 0.05
Table 3. Multiple regression results for perceived restoration. Predictors: spatio-cognitive dimensions of coherence, complexity, mystery, and gender. Conducted separately for attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed environments. Statistically significant results are indicated in bold.
Table 3. Multiple regression results for perceived restoration. Predictors: spatio-cognitive dimensions of coherence, complexity, mystery, and gender. Conducted separately for attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed environments. Statistically significant results are indicated in bold.
BSEβtp
Attractive open
Coherence0.0760.1370.0840.5560.581
Complexity0.0850.1450.0720.5910.557
Mystery0.2240.1460.1991.5300.132
Gender−0.1370.1390.140−0.9900.329
R20.107
Adj R20.032
SE0.502
F4,471.430 p = 0.238
Attractive closed
Coherence0.2060.1380.2501.6660.141
Complexity0.1530.1700.1170.8970.374
Mystery0.1850.1720.1231.0790.286
Gender0.0110.1440.0120.0800.936
R20.128
Adj R20.073
SE0.535
F4,471.721 p = 0.161
Unattractive open
Coherence0.0560.1370.0670.4070.686
Complexity0.0980.1920.0820.5110.610
Mystery0.4420.1860.4322.3810.021
Gender0.0040.1300.0060.0320.975
R20.277
Adj R20.215
SE0.634
F4,474.490 p < 0.01
Unattractive closed
Coherence0.0140.1300.0150.1080.915
Complexity0.0840.1810.0640.4640.644
Mystery0.4900.1760.4602.7840.008
Gender0.0030.1250.0040.0270.979
R20.307
Adj R20.248
SE0.575
F4,475.214 p < 0.01
Table 4. Multiple regression results for environmental liking. Predictors: emotional categories of joy, surprise, fear, sadness, and gender. Conducted separately for attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed environments. Statistically significant results are indicated in bold.
Table 4. Multiple regression results for environmental liking. Predictors: emotional categories of joy, surprise, fear, sadness, and gender. Conducted separately for attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed environments. Statistically significant results are indicated in bold.
BSEβtp
Attractive open
Joy0.3080.1690.3601.1820.075
Surprise0.4100.1570.4602.6130.012
Fear0.04760.1630.0510.840.777
Sadness0.0330.1700.0380.1940.846
Gender−0.0220.127−0.030−0.1700.866
R20.403
Adj R20.339
SE0.560
F5,466.337 p < 0.001
Attractive closed
Joy0.3210.1730.3501.8890.069
Surprise0.3750.1460.3402.5650.017
Fear−0.0040.224−0.032−0.1910.849
Sadness0.0410.2060.0310.1990.842
Gender−0.0520.135−0.071−0.3590.699
R20.365
Adj R20.296
SE0.562
F5,465.288 p < 0.001
Unattractive open
Joy0.5730.1270.5794.5000.000
Surprise0.2770.1290.2772.1490.037
Fear−0.1090.120−0.169−0.9130.366
Sadness0.1640.1200.23101.3650.178
Gender0.1240.0900.1841.3740.176
R20.640
Adj R20.600
SE0.469
F5,4616.284 p < 0.001
Unattractive closed
Joy0.5600.1300.6324.5930.000
Surprise0.2340.1310.2271.7840.081
Fear−0.1150.131−0.184−0.8730.387
Sadness0.1720.1300.2001.3300.1920
Gender0.1740.0940.2531.8510.071
R20.626
Adj R20.586
SE0.467
F5,4615.426 p < 0.001
Table 5. Multiple regression results for perceived restoration. Predictors: emotional categories of joy, surprise, fear, sadness, and gender. Conducted separately for attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed environments. Statistically significant results are indicated in bold.
Table 5. Multiple regression results for perceived restoration. Predictors: emotional categories of joy, surprise, fear, sadness, and gender. Conducted separately for attractive open, attractive closed, unattractive open, and unattractive closed environments. Statistically significant results are indicated in bold.
BSEβtp
Attractive open
Joy0.7000.1220.6055.6840.000
Surprise−0.0780.114−0.065−0.6880.494
Fear−0.4240.118−0.348−3.6000.001
Sadness0.2000.1230.1701.06240.111
Gender−0.1710.092−0.175−1.8460.071
R20.686
Adj R20.652
SE0.301
F5,4620.516 p < 0.001
Attractive closed
Joy0.6630.1450.6004.5760.000
Surprise0.1940.1230.1071.5810.121
Fear0.2760.1880.1691.4690.149
Sadness−0.1490.173−0.093−0.8600.394
Gender−0.0060.113−0.007−0.0550.956
R20.553
Adj R20.504
SE0.386
F5,4611.370 p < 0.001
Unattractive open
Joy0.7730.1190.7546.5000.000
Surprise0.0730.1200.0700.6040.549
Fear−0.2210.112−0.310−1.9770.054
Sadness0.3100.1120.3742.7620.008
Gender−0.0190.084−0.028−0.2300.819
R20.685
Adj R20.651
SE0.422
F5,4620.024 p < 0.001
Unattractive closed
Joy0.7280.1150.7046.43520.000
Surprise0.1470.1160.1301.2740.210
Fear−0.2370.116−0.346−2.0400.047
Sadness0.3580.1140.3773.1330.003
Gender0.0250.0830.0330.3000.768
R20.710
Adj R20.78
SE0.376
F5,4622.473 p < 0.001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Franěk, M. Landscape Preference: The Role of Attractiveness and Spatial Openness of the Environment. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 666. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13080666

AMA Style

Franěk M. Landscape Preference: The Role of Attractiveness and Spatial Openness of the Environment. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(8):666. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13080666

Chicago/Turabian Style

Franěk, Marek. 2023. "Landscape Preference: The Role of Attractiveness and Spatial Openness of the Environment" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 8: 666. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13080666

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop