Next Article in Journal
Examining the Impact of Pro-Environmental Factors on Sustainable Consumption Behavior and Pollution Control
Previous Article in Journal
Relevance of Students’ Goals for Learning Engagement and Knowledge Gains in an Online Learning Course
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Psychological Factors Explaining the COVID-19 Pandemic Impact on Mental Health: The Role of Meaning, Beliefs, and Perceptions of Vulnerability and Mortality

1
Department of Human and Social Sciences, University of Bergamo, 24129 Bergamo, Italy
2
Department of Psychology, University of Milano Bicocca, 20126 Milan, Italy
3
Department of Human Sciences “R. Massa”, University of Milano Bicocca, 20126 Milan, Italy
4
Portland Institute for Loss and Transition, Portland, OR 97223, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(2), 162; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13020162
Submission received: 1 January 2023 / Revised: 4 February 2023 / Accepted: 8 February 2023 / Published: 13 February 2023

Abstract

:
This study tested an expanded version of the explanatory model of the negative impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health proposed by Milman and colleagues. Participants (N = 680) completed an online survey on demographic variables associated with poor pandemic mental health, COVID-19 stressors, mental health symptoms, and pandemic-related psychological processes we hypothesized as mediating mechanisms explaining the negative mental health effects of the COVID-19 stressors. Results indicated that these psychological processes (core belief violation, meaning made of the pandemic, vulnerability, and mortality perception) explained the severity of mental health symptoms to a far greater extent than COVID-19 stressors and demographics combined. In addition, these psychological processes mediated the impact of COVID-19 stressors on all mental health outcomes. Specifically, COVID-19 stressors were associated with increased core belief violation, decreased meaning making, and more intense perceived vulnerability and mortality. In turn, those whose core beliefs were more violated by the pandemic, who made less meaning of the pandemic, and who perceived a more pronounced vulnerability and mortality experienced a worse mental health condition. This study’s results suggest some possible ways of intervention in pandemic-like events useful for limiting such impact at the individual, group, social and political levels.

1. Introduction

Since the inception of COVID-19, many studies have investigated the multiple negative impacts of the pandemic on mental health, at the individual level, at the family level, and at the broader societal level [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Correlations have been found between different aspects of the pandemic and increased levels of depression, anxiety, self-harm, suicidal ideation, alcohol abuse, and psychophysical symptoms such as loss of appetite, dizziness, insomnia, and nausea [7,8,9,10,11].
To reduce the spread of the virus and protect public health, national and international institutions in most countries applied preventive and restrictive regulations such as physical distancing, isolation, and quarantine, with significant psychological consequences at all the levels mentioned above [12,13,14,15,16,17]. Adverse psychological effects of the restrictions include loneliness, social isolation, anger, fear, emotional reactions, and potentially harmful behaviors such as avoidance behaviors, social phobias, and substance abuse [18,19,20,21,22].
At the family level, forced cohabitation was associated with an increase in separations and divorces, e.g., [23,24], and domestic violence, e.g., [25,26,27,28]. Studies at different stages of the pandemic investigated “direct factors” (i.e., having personally contracted the virus, having severe consequences, or being in close relationship with people who died from COVID-19) and “indirect factors” (e.g., childcare loss, job loss, caregiving a disabled or ill person, socio-economic problems, working from home, distance learning, etc.). Their results have demonstrated several adverse psychological effects of the pandemic on the mental health of individuals, families, and social groups [1,18,29,30,31,32,33].
Most studies of people’s mental health during the pandemic were descriptive and reported the heavy negative impact of the phenomenon. Relatively few explanatory models have been proposed to clarify what specific psychological factors and processes are involved in the relationship between various aspects of the pandemic (including its direct and indirect stressors) and the negative mental health impact [1,29,30,32,34,35,36,37,38]. As an exception to this trend, Milman and colleagues [1] proposed an explanatory model of the relationship between pandemic-related triggers and their adverse psychological effects. The main constructs underpinning the model were two primary psychological processes derived from stress-coping theory, grief studies, and trauma research: violation of core beliefs and disrupted meaning making [39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49].
The theoretical rationale for the first of these processes is that individuals possess a set of core beliefs, higher-order constructs that together suggest that the world is a fair place where we can influence our circumstances and be relatively confident about the future and our ability to anticipate it. Core beliefs support the meaningfulness of our existence, our identity, our personal worth, and our relational networks [50]. Stressful or traumatic events can cause mental health disorders by violating such core beliefs, leaving individuals disoriented about who they are, their expectations for the future, and the nature of the world. According to Milman et al. [1], the COVID-19 pandemic appeared “tailor-made” to violate people’s core beliefs.
The second psychological process on which this model is based is people’s meaning making of challenging events, such as the pandemic, that force them to review or revise their violated system of core beliefs. Individuals may or may not be able to develop an understanding of a novel stressful event and integrate this understanding into their previous belief system. Disrupted meaning making implies a severe impasse in attributing meaning to the pandemic event and assimilating it into one’s worldview [51].
Milman and colleagues [1] then empirically studied the role of core belief violation and disrupted meaning in regulating mental health during the pandemic. They tested the hypothesis that both psychological processes would mediate the effects of multiple pandemic stressors, accounting for adverse mental health outcomes. With a large, ethnically diverse sample from the United States, they examined the relations between demographic variables potentially associated with poor pandemic mental health (gender, age, ethnicity, education), direct COVID stressors (diagnosis, death of significant others), indirect COVID stressors (unemployment, increased living costs, loss of childcare), core belief violation, meaning attributed to the pandemic, coronavirus anxiety, depression, and general anxiety. They found that core belief violation and disrupted meaning making explained the severity of depression, general anxiety, and coronavirus anxiety (a specific kind of anxiety related to the pandemic; [20]) significantly more than demographic factors, direct COVID stressors, and indirect COVID stressors combined. Furthermore, core belief violation and disrupted meaning making significantly mediated the impact of direct COVID stressors on all mental health outcomes and completely accounted for the impact of indirect COVID stressors (See Figure 1). Their study offers an explanatory psychological model of the pandemic’s impact on mental health, suggesting that the violation of core beliefs and the inability to make meaning of the pandemic could be crucial targets for clinical intervention in the COVID context and, for those with protracted struggles in adapting, in its aftermath.
To sum up, empirical research based on this model showed that the two mediating factors (core belief violation and meaning making) explained the impact of direct and indirect pandemic-related factors on mental health outcomes (i.e., anxiety, depression, and coronavirus anxiety). On the other hand, the same model also explained how, contrary to expectations, compliance with social isolation measures did not increase the mental health burden of the pandemic but instead reduced it, apparently by reducing the impact of the pandemic on core beliefs associated with predictability and control [1]. Similarly, other investigators have found that confinement at home and the strengthening of relationships contributed, at least in well-functioning families, to reducing the violation of core beliefs and supporting functional processes of meaning making [51,52].
The present work further tested the model of Milman et al. [1] in a different context (a different country and time). Specifically, we replicated the 2020 study with the Italian population, approximately one year after the onset of the pandemic, when the “third pandemic wave” in Italy was ending and the second dose of vaccine had recently become available. Milman and colleagues [1] took a “snapshot” of the pandemic in May 2020 in the USA, while we took a snapshot in May–June 2021 in Italy (see the Supplementary Materials, Section S3, for the results of the exact replication of the analyses by Milman and colleagues). Comparing the two studies could allow us to test the robustness of the explanatory model across time and context.
As a second aim, we also sought to expand Milman et al.’s analysis [1] by considering additional factors. We added two indicators that expand and enrich their explanatory model: perceptions of being vulnerable and perceptions of one’s own mortality. Both are derived from Terror Management Theory (TMT) [53], which argues that people defend themselves against threatening events that make their mortality salient by adhering more tenaciously to a dominant cultural worldview and its prescribed attitudes and behaviors. Based on TMT, adherence to normative social prescriptions and guidelines introduced to reduce the spread of COVID-19 could reduce coronavirus anxiety [1] by offering a security-enhancing connection to the broader societal narrative of the pandemic. Thus, we hypothesized that the degree of one’s own perceived vulnerability and the salience of one’s mortality would function as additional mediators of the impact on mental health along with core belief violation and disrupted meaning making. A higher sense of vulnerability and more frequent thoughts of one’s mortality connected to the pandemic would thus lead to worse mental health outcomes.
Confirmation of this “integrative” model could help shape policy, community, and individual interventions to support mental health during the continuation of this pandemic, in its aftermath for those struggling with residual symptoms, and during similar situations in the future.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The study involved 680 participants (423 of whom were women) between the ages of 18 and 91 years (M = 52.81; SD = 15.94). Being at least 18 years old, speaking fluent Italian, and giving online informed consent were the only inclusion criteria. Detailed demographic information can be found in the results and in Table 1. All participants were included in the analyses. A sensitivity analysis showed that the sample allowed for reliable estimation of Pearson bivariate correlations as small as 0.11 with a statistical power of 0.80 (α = 0.05).

2.2. Procedure and Measures

Participants were recruited between May and June 2021 through the collaboration with four general practitioners (GPs) from Milan and Bergamo in northern Italy, two of the provinces most affected by the early outbreak of the pandemic. The GPs invited all their patients to take part in a study of the pandemic’s impact on psychological wellbeing and shared with them the link to an online set of questionnaires and questions.
All participants completed the procedure during the third pandemic wave after the harshest lockdown restrictions had been lifted. They responded to the online form at home in a single session lasting approximately 30 min. They provided their informed consent to participate after reading the study presented in the first section of the online material.
Through this procedure, we collected participants’ demographics and answers to several socio-psychological scales. The instruments were presented to all participants in the same fixed order illustrated below. Detailed questions and answer options are reported in the Supplementary Materials.
The study focused on the following variables:
Demographics and general information. Participants’ age, gender, nationality, education, marital status, living conditions (e.g., living with their children), caregiver status (i.e., acting as a caregiver of elderly or disabled people), occupation, and health (e.g., medical and psychological conditions predating the COVID-19 pandemic). The marital status variable was further treated to facilitate analysis: participants selected the option that best represented them among a set of four (i.e., single, in a relationship but not cohabiting, in a relationship and cohabiting, married, divorced or widowed) and their answer was then coded in four dummy variables, each representing one of the options (i.e., single “no/yes”, in a relationship but not cohabiting “no/yes”, etc.).
Indirect COVID-19 stressors. Conditions associated with the pandemic or with the public response to it, such as loss or reduction of employment, working with COVID patients, having to work from home or to leave the house for work even during lockdown, increased cost of living, childcare loss, and confinement measures.
Direct COVID-19 stressors. Being tested positive for COVID-19 and deaths of family members, friends, or acquaintances due to COVID-19.
Psychological factors. Psychological variables that, according to our hypothesis, play a part in shaping the mental health outcomes of the pandemic:
  • Core Belief Inventory (CBI) [39]. A brief measure of the effect of stressful experiences on an individual’s assumptive world or, in other words, a measure of violation of core beliefs. The CBI consists of nine items focusing on religious and spiritual beliefs, human nature, relationships with other people, meaning of life, and personal strengths and weaknesses. The respondents indicate the extent to which the stressful experience (in our case, the pandemic) led them to seriously examine their core beliefs. Responses are given on a six-point scale ranging from “not at all” (0) to “to a very great degree” (5). The total score is computed as the mean of individual item scores, and higher values indicate a greater degree of core beliefs violation. In this study, CBI showed excellent internal consistency (α = 0.90);
  • Integration of Stressful Life Experiences Scale—Short Form (ISLES-SF) [54]. A brief measure of an individual’s ability to make sense of stressful events. It assesses the degree of disrupted meaning making following a specific event, in the present case, the pandemic. The ISLES-SF consists of six items, and participants express their degree of agreement on a five-point scale ranging from “strongly agree” (1) to “strongly disagree” (5). The total score is computed as the sum of item scores, and lower values indicate a greater disruption in meaning making ability. In this study, ISLES-SF showed good internal consistency (α = 0.88);
  • Vulnerability. Participants reported their agreement with the statement “This pandemic made me feel vulnerable and fragile” using a 6-point scale (from 0 = “not at all” to 5 = “to a very high degree”);
  • Mortality. Participants reported their agreement with the statement, “This pandemic made me think more about my own death” (from 0 = “not at all” to 5 = “to a very high degree”).
Mental health and distress measures. We administered a broad set of tools to investigate the impact of the pandemic on participants’ mental health.
  • Four-item Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-4) [55]. An ultra-brief measure (four items) of anxiety and depression problems experienced over the last two weeks. Respondents indicate the frequency of the proposed feelings on a 4-point scale ranging from “not at all” (0) to “nearly every day” (3). The total score is determined as the sum of item scores and indicates the severity of reported symptoms. In this study, PHQ-4 showed good internal consistency (α = 0.86);
  • Coronavirus Anxiety Scale (CAS) [20]. A brief mental health screener (five items) designed to quickly and accurately identify individuals functionally impaired by their fear and anxiety over the coronavirus. Respondents reported the frequency of given situations on a 4-point scale ranging from “not at all” (0) to “nearly every day over the last two weeks” (3). The total score is computed as the sum of item scores and indicates the degree of coronavirus dysfunctional anxiety experienced over the last two weeks. In this study, CAS showed suitable internal consistency (α = 0.86);
  • General Population—Clinical Outcomes in Routine Evaluation (GP-CORE) [56]. A non-clinical 14-item self-report measure of wellbeing, psychological problems, and functioning for the general population (GP), derived from the Clinical Outcomes in Routine Evaluation-Outcome Measure (CORE-OM). For each item, the respondents rate themselves with reference to the last week on a 5-point scale ranging from “not at all” (0) to “most or all of the time” (4). A single overall score is created by calculating the mean score over the 14 items, where higher values indicate lower degrees of wellbeing. In this study, GP-CORE showed good internal consistency (α = 0.83);
  • Profile Of Mood States (POMS) [57]. A self-report mood adjective checklist in which each adjective is scored from 0 (absent) to 4 (very much) based on how well each item describes the respondent’s mood during the previous week. The 58 items are grouped into six subscales: tension, anger, fatigue, depression, confusion, and vigor. An overall measure of respondents’ mood can be calculated by subtracting the vigor score from the sum of the other scales’ scores. In this study, POMS showed excellent internal consistency (α = 0.98).

2.3. Statistical Analyses

Multiple regressions. We ran four separate multiple regressions to assess the impact of demographic characteristics, indirect and direct COVID stressors, and psychological factors (predictors) on each of the post-pandemic wellbeing measures (outcomes). Predictors were entered in a hierarchical blockwise fashion. This allowed us to test the effects of each individual predictor as well as to evaluate how different sets of predictors contributed to explaining outcome variability.
In each of the five regression analyses, we entered predictors in four blocks: (1) demographic variables (age, gender, education, marital status, number of cohabiting children, caretaker status, and pre-existing medical and psychological conditions); (2) indirect COVID stressors (loss of employment, loss of childcare, increased cost of living, working with COVID patients, forced working conditions in or outside the house, confinement); (3) direct COVID stressors (COVID diagnoses and acquaintances’ COVID-related death); (4) psychological factors (i.e., core belief violation, meaning making, vulnerability and mortality). We determined the order of the blocks based on the theoretical considerations and predictions partly derived from Milman et al. [1].
Multiple mediation analyses. We used multiple mediation analyses to test whether our data were compatible with the hypothesis that psychological factors mediate the effects of direct and indirect stressors on post-pandemic mental health. Specifically, we were interested in quantifying the indirect effects of pandemic stressors (i.e., the combination of their impact on a given mediator and that mediator’s subsequent effect on the outcome). Additionally, we quantified the stressors’ controlled direct effects; that is, their impact on the outcome keeping all potential mediators at a constant value. To do this, we implemented a regression-based approach to analyze the effects of several mediators acting simultaneously in parallel.
We developed a model for each predictor-outcome pair sharing a significant bivariate correlation and included in it four potential mediators (i.e., CBI, ISLES-SF, Vulnerability and Mortality perceptions) and all potential confounders. A confounder was identified as a variable that predates pandemic stressors and that is associated with both stressor and mediators, or stressor and outcome, or mediators and outcome. Examples of such variables are age, gender, or pre-existing pathologies. Each model included the controlled direct effect of the predictor on the outcome (Figure 2, c’) and four indirect effects (Figure 2, a1–4b1–4), all estimated while statistically controlling for potential confounders. Indirect effects, their standard errors, and confidence intervals were estimated based on 5000 bootstrap samples.
All analyses were run in the R environment using the base package for multiple regressions [58] and the Psych package for multiple mediations [59].

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

The respondents (N = 680) completed all questionnaires and questions (see Table 1); most of them had a bachelor’s degree or higher qualification (57.0%); the prevalent family arrangement was living with a partner (69.6%) and not cohabiting with children (60.9%). A subset of 51 individuals (7.5%) declared they were taking care of an elderly or disabled person. Approximately half of the participants had pre-existing physical illnesses (44.6%), whereas 1.9% had a pre-existing mental illness.
Almost all the respondents (93.7%) reported experiencing confinement, 68.9% reported working from home, and 59.7% leaving the house for work during lockdown (although these percentages appear contradictory, note that the questionnaire referred to the entire pandemic period and that the same person might have experienced both situations during that time). About half the sample declared that they experienced job loss or reduction (49.4%) and economic difficulties (53.1%) and were affected by the loss of childcare services (49.3%).
Only 12.5% of respondents had tested positive for COVID-19, 60.4% had had an acquaintance who died of COVID-19, and 19.8% had had a significant person who died of COVID-19.
Descriptive statistics of scores on pandemic-related psychological factors and mental health measures are reported in Table 2. The bivariate correlations among all the variables involved in the study are reported in Appendix A, Table A1.

3.2. Multiple Regressions

Blockwise multiple regressions (see Table 3) showed that demographic variables explained, on average, 13.3% of outcome variability. Taking into account the indirect COVID-19 stressors significantly increased the model fit when predicting mood states (POMS), whereas direct COVID-19 stressors significantly improved the COVID anxiety (CAS) model. In the GP-CORE model, the stepwise addition of indirect COVID-19 stressors first and direct COVID-19 stressors next did not lead to significant improvements in model fit. However, post hoc analyses have shown that the GP-CORE Model 3 explains significantly more variance compared to Model 1. This means that although the individual contributions of indirect and direct stressors are small, when taken together, they represent a meaningful addition to the demographic variables. Finally, psychological factors, when added to the model, provided a significant increase in the model fit in all cases. The full models, including all four blocks of predictors, explain, on average, 31.2% of outcome variability. See Table 3 to compare R2 at different steps of the blockwise regressions.
Results from the multiple regressions (see Table 4) indicated that being younger, suffering from a pre-existing mental or physical illness, and experiencing greater core belief violation and vulnerability during the pandemic consistently predicted worse post-pandemic outcomes, as assessed by the PHQ-4, GP-CORE, and POMS. In addition, economic difficulties predicted higher scores on the GP-CORE and the POMS. The CAS measure showed a slightly different pattern of results, with greater coronavirus anxiety predicted for women suffering from pre-existing mental illnesses, being exposed to COVID-related deaths, experiencing more core belief violation (CBI), or mortality perception. We also observed that those who had to leave home to work during lockdown reported less coronavirus anxiety (i.e., lower CAS scores) and that job loss or reduction was the only experience to predict better—rather than worse—scores on the PHQ-4, GP-CORE, and POMS.

3.3. Multiple Mediation

We fit 12 multiple mediation models for any predictor-outcome pair that showed significant bivariate correlations (see Table A1 in Appendix A).
Table 5 reports our effects of interest: controlled direct and natural indirect effects, estimated while controlling for potential measured confounders:
  • Violation of core beliefs (CBI) mediated the effects of several indirect COVID-19 stressors on post-pandemic mental health levels. It mediated the effects of unemployment and childcare loss on CAS and POMS, the effects of leaving home for work on POMS, the effects of exposure to COVID-related deaths on CAS, and the effects of economic difficulties on all four outcomes;
  • Perceived vulnerability mediated the effects of economic difficulties on PHQ-4, GP-CORE, and POMS but not on CAS;
  • Perception of mortality mediated the effects of one direct COVID-19 stressor (exposure to COVID-related deaths) on coronavirus anxiety level (CAS);
  • Disrupted meaning making (ISLES-SF) about the pandemic did not significantly mediate the effects of any predictor on the outcomes;
  • Exposure to COVID-related deaths was the only predictor to have a significant direct effect on one of the post-pandemic mental health measures, the coronavirus anxiety level (CAS).
These results are independent of participants’ gender, age, marital status, number of children living at home, and physical and mental illness (see Supplementary Results Section S2 for a list of confounders included in each model and estimates of predictor-mediator and mediator-outcome associations).

4. Discussion

Since the pandemic outbreak, numerous empirical studies have been published describing and demonstrating the worldwide impact of COVID-19-related events on mental health [10,60,61]. Far fewer studies, however, have proposed and tested models of psychological processes that appear to regulate the influence of the pandemic on mental health, e.g., [1,17,22,32,51,62]. Such modeling is useful and necessary to make sense of research findings and to offer operational guidance to mental health professionals on how and where to intervene to limit the negative effects of the pandemic and, if possible, turn them into possibilities for growth.
The model proposed by Milman et al. [1] was one of the first attempts to explain the psychological processes underlying the pandemic’s impact on mental health, and it received robust empirical validation by the authors investigating a U.S. population in the early months of the pandemic (May 2020). The present study is further validation of that model in a different time (May and June 2021) and country (Italy). Additional variables and a more multidimensional assessment of mental health complement the model. Specifically, as in the pioneering work of Milman et al. [1], the psychological processes explained the severity of mental health symptoms to a far greater extent than direct and indirect COVID stressors and demographics combined. In our study, to make the model more comprehensive and cover additional factors with a potential impact on mental health, we added demographic variables (marital status, living with children, the role of caregiver, previous mental or physical illness, beyond gender, age, and education, which were already considered in the original study), additional COVID-19 indirect stressors (forced working conditions—from home or outside the home, working with COVID-19 patients, confinement measures in addition to unemployment, loss of child care, and increased living costs already considered in the original study), two mediating psychological processes (awareness of mortality and perception of personal vulnerability in addition to those of disrupted meaning making and violation of core beliefs already considered in the original model), and two multidimensional mental health assessments (GP-CORE and POMS, in addition to the CAS and PHQ-4 used in the original study).
Also, in this new comprehensive model, the psychological processes (core belief violation, disrupted meaning making, perception of vulnerability, and mortality) accounted for nearly 17% of the variance in all measures of mental health (see Table 3), which is more than two times the amount of variance explained collectively by demographics (gender, age, ethnicity, education, marital status, living with children, role of caregiver, and previous mental or physical illness), direct COVID-19 stressors (diagnosis and death), and indirect COVID-19 stressors (unemployment, loss of childcare, increased living costs, forced working conditions—from home or outside the home, working with COVID-19 patients, and confinement measures).
These findings highlight the crucial role played by attributing meaning to events that otherwise have a negative and traumatic impact on an individual’s mental health. This is even more evident if we consider that when core belief violation, disrupted meaning making and perception of vulnerability and mortality were accounted for, experiencing a COVID-19 death no longer significantly predicted decreased mental wellbeing (see Table 4). Only the anxiety about being contaminated with coronavirus is directly predicted by experiencing COVID-19 deaths among the significant relational figures, a very narrow and specific part of the discomfort experienced by the individual. Thus, the results, in accordance with Milman et al.’s study [1], highlighted the crucial mediating role of these psychological processes in linking pandemic stressors with the severity of individuals’ distress. Specifically, both direct and indirect COVID-19 stressors were associated with increased core belief violation, decreased meaning making, and more intense perceived vulnerability and mortality. In turn, those whose core beliefs were more violated by the pandemic, who made less meaning of the pandemic, and who perceived a more pronounced vulnerability and mortality experienced a worse mental health condition. In other words, the psychological processes we accounted for explained how the pandemic stressors can be associated with worse mental health.
Going into the specifics of each psychological process investigated as a mediator of the impact of the pandemic on mental health, we can say that the pandemic represented an experience with traumatic characteristics insofar as it led to a violation of the core beliefs that individuals had about themselves, the world, and the future, placing them in a condition of compromised control and predictability. In fact, the assumptive world theory of trauma posits that if an event undermines the largely implicit core beliefs that govern everyday functioning, it takes on a traumatic quality [39,43,47,48,63]. In this sense, the pandemic and related events had a negative impact on mental health not because they were objectively traumatic but only to the extent that they assumed an existentially threatening nature. In particular, job loss or reduction, childcare loss, economic difficulties, forced work conditions (working from home or leaving home to work), and COVID-19 deaths among significant relational figures may have had a negative impact on mental health because they challenged the well-established and reassuring core beliefs of people.
The mediating role of perceived vulnerability is consistent with this same theoretical framework, albeit with a restricted scope. According to our results, economic difficulties led to a perception of increased vulnerability, which in turn correlated with increased depression and anxiety (as assessed by PHQ-4) and psychological distress (as assessed by GP-CORE and POMS).
The third psychological process we investigated—the capacity for meaning making—is theoretically complementary to the previous ones: the more that individuals can make sense of events, including traumatic ones, the weaker the negative impact these events have on mental health [46,63,64]. In our study, the inability to make meaning of the pandemic did not significantly mediate stressor-outcome associations. The limited role played by meaning making in our model compared to that of Milman et al. [1] could be explained by the fact that the instrument used to measure it (ISLES) is designed to evaluate the acute impact of discrete events (such as a bereavement or physical trauma) rather than prolonged and pervasive experiences such as a pandemic. On the other hand, the finding that having a mental or physical illness predating the pandemic and being younger predicts poorer mental health seems to be consistent with our theory. Young age and illnesses may make the process of integrating and reconstructing the meaning of negative and unexpected events less efficient.
The final moderating psychological process we added to extend the model was the perception of one’s own mortality. According to Terror Management Theory (TMT) [53], humans are probably the only ones who are aware of their own mortality, unlike all other living beings. However, the thought of one’s own mortality and death is one of the most difficult mental health challenges, so humans have developed several defenses against distress arising from events or feelings that bring them closer to the idea of their own death. One of these is to adhere more tenaciously to a dominant cultural worldview and its prescribed attitudes and behaviors to feel that they belong to a larger entity that will survive their own death. According to TMT, however, death anxiety, precisely because it is psychically intolerable, is mostly managed at the unconscious level. If the perception of one’s own death and mortality enters the focus of conscious attention, defenses against it have failed, or alternatively, but more rarely, there has been an effort to integrate one’s death into a larger value perspective. The question we asked participants about how close the pandemic had brought them to the idea of their own death thus concerns the level of awareness of this threat, which is usually a sign that the normal anxiety buffering against death did not work as well as they could. In this sense, the higher the mortality perception of the respondents, the worse the expected pandemic impact on mental health. This occurred only with respect to coronavirus anxiety (CAS): our results have shown that mortality perception partially mediated the negative association between the death of a significant relational figure and respondents’ coronavirus anxiety.
In sum, the results suggest that the pandemic exerted its negative influence on mental health via specific psychological processes engaged in meaning attribution to the experiences. The negative impact on mental health was directly associated with the degree to which the pandemic has violated people’s core beliefs, increased the individuals’ vulnerability and mortality perception, and challenged individuals’ ability to make sense of events and integrate the unexpected into their world of meaning.
The counterintuitive result we found that job loss or reduction predicts better mental health could be viewed as supporting this interpretation. It could be that job loss or reduction was experienced as a relief, allowing people not to risk exposure to COVID-19 in the workplace. In addition, the increased closeness with family members and the home environment may have strengthened people’s core beliefs and activated shared processes of meaning making about the pandemic in primary relational contexts. In support of this interpretation, previous studies demonstrated that when the pandemic left core beliefs intact, people experienced lower anxiety and COVID-19 stress [38]. In addition, despite the seemingly stressful nature of social isolation, the choice to socially isolate appeared to mitigate COVID-19 anxiety by facilitating meaning making and by preserving core beliefs in the controllable and predictable nature of the world [1,32].
Limitations should be considered when interpreting our results and may help inform future research. The main ones are related to the research design and enrolment of participants. The cross-sectional instead of longitudinal design does not allow the exclusion of alternative causal orders in the hypothesized relations: poor mental health could disrupt core beliefs, heighten perceptions of vulnerability and mortality, and hinder meaning making. Alternatively, mental health and other psychological processes could be directly shaped by pandemic exposures and then mutually exacerbate one another over time. However, as Milman et al. [31] state, there is already research “highlighting core belief violation and disrupted meaning making as processes driving poor mental health following stressful events [44,45,46,63,64]”. Thus, “the plausibility of these various alternatives must be assessed using prospective longitudinal designs or intervention studies that address pandemic-related mental health by targeting challenged belief systems” [31]. Furthermore, given the direct relevance of results from this field of research to the design of real-world interventions, it is crucial that future studies fully employ instruments designed to improve methodological transparency and facilitate their replicability, such as (but not limited to) pre-registration and registered reports.
The second limitation concerns the representativeness of the surveyed sample, who were patients of general practitioners and responded to their request to fill out the questionnaires. Many of them had a medical condition for which they were being treated by their physicians, such that replicating the study with different populations would increase the external validity and generalizability of the results obtained.
However, we believe that the value of this research surpasses its limitations. This study represents a second confirmation of the model proposed by Milman et al. [1] on a different population, at a time more distant from the pandemic outbreak and with a larger number of multidimensional assessment tools. The proposed explanatory model, unlike many others, was properly psychological; that is, it focused on the psychic processes that theoretically lead to a worsening mental health status. The results undoubtedly need to be explored through different research designs, but they still offer a useful reference for those who must intervene in pandemic-like events to limit such impact at the individual, group, social and political levels. The most appropriate choices at all levels of intervention should aim not at nonspecific psychological containment and support but at creating contexts that can foster the creation of individual and communitarian meanings, strengthen people’s core beliefs, and reduce preoccupation with mortality and the perception of personal vulnerability. This is certainly possible in the traditional psychotherapeutic setting [50,65,66,67,68,69], but it can also involve activating primary relational, social, and cultural contexts capable of generating collective and participatory meanings for the unexpected and often traumatic events that globalized life will increasingly lead us to experience.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bs13020162/s1, Note on measures collected and used; Questions concerning demographics and COVID-related stressors; Figures S1–S12: Multiple mediation model graphs with effects magnitude and significance; Tables S1–S2: Replication of Milman et al. (2020) mediation models.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.C., A.N., and R.A.N.; methodology, M.C., A.N., and R.A.N.; formal analysis, F.C.; investigation, A.N.; data curation, A.N. and F.C.; writing—original draft preparation, C.L.C., M.C., F.C., A.N., and R.A.N.; writing—review and editing, F.C., C.L.C., A.N., and R.A.N.; visualization, A.N. and F.C.; supervision, M.C., A.N., and R.A.N.; project administration, A.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Bergamo (protocol code: 51810/I/3—11 March 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Online informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are openly available on OSF platform and available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all respondents for giving us the opportunity to investigate the effect of the pandemic on their psychological world. We also thank the general practitioners and psychologists of the “doctor-psychologist together” project who supported the research by asking their patients to fill out the online study questionnaires during the pandemic period.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Bivariate correlations among all the variables involved in the study.
Table A1. Bivariate correlations among all the variables involved in the study.
AgeGenderMS SingleMS RelationshipMS CohabitingMS Divorced or WidowedChildrenCaretaker RolePhysical IllnessMental Illness
Age
Gender 0.16 ***
MS single−0.29 ***−0.02
MS relationship−0.27 *** 0.01 −0.11 **
MS cohabiting 0.24 *** 0.03 −0.62 ***−0.42 ***
MS divorced/widowed 0.21 ***−0.04 −0.13 ***−0.09 * −0.48 ***
Children−0.11 ** −0.12 ** −0.25 ***−0.15 *** 0.30 ***−0.05
Caretaker role 0.00 −0.05 0.17 ***−0.01 −0.11 ** −0.01 −0.05
Physical illness 0.47 *** 0.09 * −0.06 −0.10 * 0.05 0.08 * −0.19 ***−0.02
Mental illness−0.03 −0.06 0.00 0.00 −0.02 0.03 0.01 0.04 −0.13 **
COVID diagnosis−0.01 −0.01 −0.08 * −0.05 0.08 * 0.02 0.12 ** −0.04 −0.09 * 0.08 *
COVID death−0.01 −0.05 −0.01 −0.03 0.07−0.07 0.05 −0.02 −0.01 0.05
Job loss or reduction−0.17 ***−0.06 −0.04 0.11 ** −0.06 0.05 0.09 * −0.01 −0.08 * 0.10 *
Economic difficulties−0.12 ** −0.02 −0.03 0.06−0.04 0.04 0.11 ** −0.03 −0.03 0.09 *
Childcare loss−0.17 ***−0.08 * −0.13 *** 0.00 0.11 ** −0.02 0.44 ***−0.02 −0.14 *** 0.08 *
Working from home−0.29 ***−0.09 * 0.07 0.00 −0.05 0.00 0.15 *** 0.06 −0.17 *** 0.09 *
Leaving home to work−0.22 ***−0.06+ 0.00 0.00 0.01 −0.02 0.17 ***−0.02 −0.16 *** 0.09 *
Working w/COVID patients−0.08 * 0.00 0.04 0.00 −0.06 0.06 0.04 −0.01 −0.08 * −0.03
Confinement−0.03 −0.02 0.04 0.00 −0.05 0.04 0.00 −0.04 0.00 0.04
CBI−0.22 ***−0.24 *** 0.05 0.03 −0.07 0.03 0.05 −0.02 −0.08 * 0.11 **
ISLES−0.08 * −0.09 * −0.01 0.08−0.05 0.02 0.06 0.00 −0.02 0.05
Vulnerability−0.16 ***−0.21 *** 0.02 0.04 −0.06 0.04 0.06 0.00 −0.07 0.12 **
Mortality 0.03 −0.15 ***−0.03 −0.04 −0.02 0.10 ** 0.08 * 0.03 0.02 0.08
CAS 0.00 −0.17 *** 0.00 −0.02 −0.04 0.08 * 0.03 0.04 0.01 0.33 ***
PHQ4−0.26 ***−0.20 *** 0.09 * 0.07−0.10 * −0.01 0.06 0.07−0.06 0.24 ***
GP-CORE−0.19 ***−0.11 ** 0.13 *** 0.03 −0.12 ** 0.02 0.00 0.05 0.01 0.25 ***
POMS−0.24 ***−0.14 *** 0.12 ** 0.05 −0.11 ** −0.01 0.04 0.06 −0.07 0.27 ***
COVID DiagnosisCOVID DeathJob Loss Or ReductionEconomic DifficultiesChildcare LossWorking from HomeLeaving Home to WorkWorking with COVID PatientsConfinement
COVID diagnosis
COVID death 0.00
Job loss or reduction 0.08 * 0.02
Economic difficulties 0.11 ** 0.01 0.84 ***
Childcare loss 0.12 ** −0.02 0.60 *** 0.58 ***
Working from home 0.03 0.00 0.44 *** 0.39 *** 0.46 ***
Leaving home to work 0.09 * 0.00 0.54 *** 0.50 *** 0.55 *** 0.51 ***
Working with COVID patients 0.01 0.08 * 0.00 −0.01 0.00 −0.04 0.10 **
Confinement 0.03 0.00 0.04 0.07 0.10 ** 0.24 *** 0.07−0.06
CBI 0.11 ** 0.09 * 0.23 *** 0.24 *** 0.15 *** 0.11 ** 0.20 *** 0.01 0.03
ISLES−0.01 −0.03 0.19 *** 0.17 *** 0.19 *** 0.04 0.11 ** 0.00 −0.05
Vulnerability 0.09 * 0.11 ** 0.10 * 0.11 ** 0.10 * 0.10 * 0.10 ** −0.02 0.02
Mortality 0.09 * 0.14 *** 0.07 0.07 0.07 0.00 0.07 0.00 0.02
CAS 0.06 0.13 *** 0.11 ** 0.12 ** 0.10 ** 0.02 0.03 −0.02 0.05
PHQ4 0.06 0.07 0.06 0.10 ** 0.07 0.09 * 0.06 −0.01 0.01
GP-CORE 0.05 0.06 0.05 0.10 * 0.05 0.06 0.05 −0.04 0.02
POMS 0.05 0.07 0.09 * 0.14 *** 0.09 * 0.09 * 0.10 * 0.00 0.00
CBIISLESVulnerabilityMortalityCASPHQ-4GP-CORE
CBI
ISLES0.29 ***
Vulnerability0.64 ***0.24 ***
Mortality0.51 ***0.20 ***0.60 ***
CAS0.40 ***0.19 ***0.35 ***0.37 ***
PHQ40.45 ***0.17 ***0.49 ***0.30 ***0.46 ***
GP-CORE0.43 ***0.19 ***0.45 ***0.33 ***0.44 ***0.77 ***
POMS0.46 ***0.19 ***0.49 ***0.30 ***0.49 ***0.80 ***0.84 ***
Note. MS = marital status; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

References

  1. Milman, E.; Lee, S.A.; Neimeyer, R.A.; Mathis, A.A.; Jobe, M.C. Modeling pandemic depression and anxiety: The mediational role of core beliefs and meaning making. J. Affect. Disord. Rep. 2020, 2, 100023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Altig, D.; Baker, S.; Barrero, J.M.; Bloom, N.; Bunn, P.; Chen, S.; Davis, S.J.; Leather, J.; Meyer, B.; Mihaylov, E.; et al. Economic uncertainty before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Public Econ. 2020, 191, 104274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Kickbusch, I.; Leung, G.M.; Bhutta, Z.A.; Matsoso, M.P.; Ihekweazu, C.; Abbasi, K. COVID-19: How a virus is turning the world upside down. BMJ 2020, 369, m1336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Kumar, A.; Nayar, K.R. COVID 19 and its mental health consequences. J. Ment. Health 2021, 30, 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Mohammadi, F.; Oshvandi, K.; Shamsaei, F.; Cheraghi, F.; Khodaveisi, M.; Bijani, M. The mental health crises of the families of COVID-19 victims: A qualitative study. BMC Fam. Pract. 2021, 22, 94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Venuleo, C.; Marinaci, T.; Gennaro, A.; Castiglioni, M.; Caldiroli, C.L. The institutional management of the COVID-19 crisis in Italy: A qualitative study on the socio-cultural context underpinning the citizens’ evaluation. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2022, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bo, H.X.; Li, W.; Yang, Y.; Wang, Y.; Zhang, Q.; Cheung, T.; Wu, X.; Xiang, Y.T. Posttraumatic stress symptoms and attitude toward crisis mental health services among clinically stable patients with COVID-19 in China. Psychol. Med. 2020, 51, 1052–1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lima, C.K.T.; Carvalho, P.M.M.; Lima, I.A.A.S.; Nunes, J.V.A.O.; Saraiva, J.S.; De Souza, R.I.; Da Silva, C.G.L.; Neto, M.L.R. The emotional impact of Coronavirus 2019-nCoV (new Coronavirus disease). Psychiatry Res. 2020, 287, 112915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Prime, H.; Wade, M.; Browne, D.T. Risk and resilience in family well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. Psychol. 2020, 75, 631–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Schmits, E.; Glowacz, F. Changes in Alcohol Use During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Impact of the Lockdown Conditions and Mental Health Factors. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2022, 20, 1147–1158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zhang, J.; Lu, H.; Zeng, H.; Zhang, S.; Du, Q.; Jiang, T.; Du, B. The differential psychological distress of populations affected by the COVID-19 pandemic. Brain. Behav. Immun. 2020, 87, 49–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Giallonardo, V.; Sampogna, G.; Del Vecchio, V.; Luciano, M.; Albert, U.; Carmassi, C.; Carrà, G.; Cirulli, F.; Dell’Osso, B.; Nanni, M.G.; et al. The Impact of Quarantine and Physical Distancing Following COVID-19 on Mental Health: Study Protocol of a Multicentric Italian Population Trial. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Kang, L.; Li, Y.; Hu, S.; Chen, M.; Yang, C.; Yang, B.X.; Wang, Y.; Hu, J.; Lai, J.; Ma, X.; et al. The mental health of medical workers in Wuhan, China dealing with the 2019 novel coronavirus. Lancet Psychiatry 2020, 7, e14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Park, S.C.; Park, Y.C. Mental Health Care Measures in Response to the 2019 Novel Coronavirus Outbreak in Korea. Psychiatry Investig. 2020, 17, 85–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Shigemura, J.; Ursano, R.J.; Morganstein, J.C.; Kurosawa, M.; Benedek, D.M. Public responses to the novel 2019 coronavirus (2019-nCoV) in Japan: Mental health consequences and target populations. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2020, 74, 281–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Shapiro, E.; Levine, L.; Kay, A. Mental health stressors in Israel during the coronavirus pandemic. Psychol. Trauma 2020, 12, 499–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Venuleo, C.; Gelo, O.C.G.; Salvatore, S. Fear, Affective Semiosis, and Management of the Pandemic Crisis: COVID-19 as Semiotic Vaccine? Clin. Neuropsychiatry 2020, 17, 117–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Brooks, S.K.; Webster, R.K.; Smith, L.E.; Woodland, L.; Wessely, S.; Greenburg, N.; Rubin, G.J. The Psychological Impact of Quarantine and How to Reduce It: Rapid Review of the Evidence. Lancet 2020, 395, 912–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Holmes, E.A.; O’Connor, R.C.; Perry, V.H.; Tracey, I.; Wessely, S.; Arseneault, L.; Ballard, C.; Christensen, H.; Cohen Silver, R.; Everall, I.; et al. Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: A call for action for mental health science. Lancet Psychiatry 2020, 7, 547–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lee, S.A. Coronavirus Anxiety Scale: A brief mental health screener for COVID-19 related anxiety. Death Stud. 2020, 44, 393–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Pfefferbaum, B.; North, C.S. Mental Health and the COVID-19 Pandemic. N. Eng. J. Med. 2020, 383, 510–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Pietromonaco, P.R.; Overall, N.C. Implications of social isolation, separation, and loss during the COVID-19 pandemic for couples’ relationships. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2022, 43, 189–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Fallesen, P. Decline in Rate of Divorce and Separation Filings in Denmark in 2020 Compared with Previous Years. Socius 2021, 7, 23780231211009991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Goldberg, A.E.; Allen, K.R.; Smith, J.Z. Divorced and separated parents during the COVID-19 pandemic. Fam. Process 2021, 60, 866–887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Dey, T.; Thakar, R. Domestic violence: An invisible pandemic. Obstet. Gynaecol. 2022, 24, 90–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kourti, A.; Stavridou, A.; Panagouli, E.; Psaltopoulou, T.; Spiliopoulou, C.; Tsolia, M.; Sergentanis, T.N.; Tsitsika, A. Domestic Violence During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Systematic Review. Trauma Violence Abus. 2021, 15248380211038690, Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Leigh, J.K.; Peña, L.D.; Anurudran, A.; Pai, A. “Are you safe to talk?”: Perspectives of Service Providers on Experiences of Domestic Violence During the COVID-19 Pandemic. J. Fam. Violence 2022, 38, 215–225, Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Piquero, A.R.; Jennings, W.G.; Jemison, E.; Kaukinen, C.; Knaul, F.M. Domestic violence during the COVID-19 pandemic—Evidence from a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Crim. Justice 2021, 74, 101806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Brown, S.M.; Doom, J.R.; Lechuga-Peña, S.; Watamura, S.E.; Koppels, T. Stress and parenting during the global COVID-19 pandemic. Child Abuse Negl. 2020, 110 Pt 2, 104699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ciciurkaite, G.; Marquez-Velarde, G.; Brown, R.L. Stressors associated with the COVID-19 pandemic, disability, and mental health: Considerations from the Intermountain West. Stress Health 2022, 38, 304–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Fiorillo, A.; Gorwood, P. The consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health and implications for clinical practice. Eur. Psychiatry 2020, 63, e32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Milman, E.; Lee, S.A.; Neimeyer, R.A. Social isolation as a means of reducing dysfunctional coronavirus anxiety and increasing psychoneuroimmunity. Brain Behav. Immun. 2020, 87, 138–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Miyah, Y.; Benjelloun, M.; Lairini, S.; Lahrichi, A. COVID-19 Impact on Public Health, Environment, Human Psychology, Global Socioeconomy, and Education. Sci. World J. 2022, 2022, 5578284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Boden, M.; Zimmerman, L.; Azevedo, K.J.; Ruzek, J.I.; Gala, S.; Abdel Magid, H.S.; Cohen, N.; Walser, R.; Mahtani, N.D.; Hoggatt, K.J.; et al. Addressing the mental health impact of COVID-19 through population health. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2021, 85, 102006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cabello-Toscano, M.; Vaqué-Alcázar, L.; Cattaneo, G.; Solana-Sánchez, J.; Bayes-Marin, I.; Abellaneda-Pérez, K.; Macià-Bros, D.; Mulet-Pons, L.; Portellano-Ortiz, C.; Fullana, M.A.; et al. Functional Brain Connectivity Prior to the COVID-19 Outbreak Moderates the Effects of Coping and Perceived Stress on Mental Health Changes: A First Year of COVID-19 Pandemic Follow-up Study. Biol. Psychiatry Cog. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2022. Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Campos, J.A.D.B.; Campos, L.A.; Martins, B.G.; Valadão Dias, F.; Ruano, R.; Maroco, J. The Psychological Impact of COVID-19 on Individuals With and Without Mental Health Disorders. Psychol. Rep. 2022, 125, 2435–2455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Foster, J.C.; Cohodes, E.M.; Brieant, A.E.; McCauley, S.; Odriozola, P.; Zacharek, S.J.; Pierre, J.C.; Hodges, H.R.; Kribakaran, S.; Haberman, J.T.; et al. Associations between early-life stress exposure and internalizing symptomatology during the COVID-19 pandemic: Assessing the role of neurobehavioral mediators. Biol. Psychiatry Glob. Open Sci. 2022. Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Trzebiński, J.; Cabański, M.; Czarnecka, J.Z. Reaction to the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Influence of Meaning in Life, Life Satisfaction, and Assumptions on World Orderliness and Positivity. J. Loss Trauma 2020, 25, 544–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Cann, A.; Calhoun, L.G.; Tedeschi, R.G.; Kilmer, R.P.; Gil-Rivas, V.; Vishnevsky, T.; Danhauer, S.C. The Core Beliefs Inventory: A brief measure of disruption in the assumptive world. Anxiety Stress Coping 2010, 23, 19–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Currier, J.M.; Holland, J.M.; Coleman, R.A.; Neimeyer, R.A. Bereavement following violent death: An assault on life and meaning. In Perspectives on Violence and Violent Death; Stevenson, R.G., Cox, G.R., Eds.; Death, Value and Meaning Series; Baywood Publishing Co.: Amityville, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 177–202. [Google Scholar]
  41. Currier, J.M.; Holland, J.M.; Neimeyer, R.A. Assumptive worldviews and problematic reactions to bereavement. J. Loss Trauma 2009, 14, 181–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Holland, J.M.; Currier, J.M.; Neimeyer, R.A. Meaning reconstruction in the first two years of bereavement: The role of sense-making and benefit-finding. Omega J. Death Dying 2006, 53, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Janoff-Bulman, R. Assumptive worlds and the stress of traumatic events: Applications of the schema construct. Soc. Cogn. 1989, 7, 113–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Milman, E.; Neimeyer, R.A.; Fitzpatrick, M.; MacKinnon, C.J.; Muis, K.R.; Cohen, S.R. Prolonged grief symptomatology following violent loss: The mediating role of meaning. Eur. Psychotraumatol. 2018, 8, 1503522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Milman, E.; Neimeyer, R.A.; Fitzpatrick, M.; MacKinnon, C.J.; Muis, K.R.; Cohen, S.R. Prolonged grief and the disruption of meaning: Establishing a mediation model. J. Couns. Psychol. 2019, 66, 714–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Neimeyer, R.A. Meaning reconstruction in bereavement: Development of a research program. Death Stud. 2019, 43, 79–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Park, C.L.; Riley, K.E.; George, L.S.; Gutierrez, I.A.; Hale, A.E.; Cho, D.; Braun, T.D. Assessing disruptions in meaning: Development of the Global Meaning Violation Scale. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2016, 40, 831–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Park, C.L.; Kennedy, M.C. Meaning violation and restoration following trauma: Conceptual overview and clinical implications. In Reconstructing Meaning after Trauma: Theory, Research, and Practice; Altmaier, E.M., Ed.; Elsevier Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2017; pp. 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Schwartzberg, S.S.; Janoff-Bulman, R. Grief and the search for meaning: Exploring the assumptive worlds of bereaved college students. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 1991, 10, 270–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Kelly, G.A. The Psychology of Personal Constructs; Norton: New York, NY, USA, 1955. [Google Scholar]
  51. Castiglioni, M.; Gaj, N. Fostering the Reconstruction of Meaning Among the General Population During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 567419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Venuleo, C.; Marinaci, T.; Gennaro, A.; Palmieri, A. The meaning of living in the time of COVID-19. A large sample narrative inquiry. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 577077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Pyszczynski, T.; Solomon, S.; Greenberg, J. Thirty years of terror management theory: From genesis to revelation. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2015; Volume 52, pp. 1–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Holland, J.M.; Currier, J.M.; Neimeyer, R.A. Validation of the Integration of Stressful Life Experiences Scale-short form in a bereaved sample. Death Stud. 2014, 38, 234–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kroenke, K.; Spitzer, R.L.; Williams, J.B.; Löwe, B. An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: The PHQ-4. Psychosomatics 2009, 50, 613–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Evans, C.; Connell, J.; Audin, K.; Sinclair, A.; Barkham, M. Rationale and development of a general population well-being measure: Psychometric status of the GP-CORE in a student sample. Br. J. Guid. Couns 2005, 33, 153–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. McNair, D.M.; Lorr, M.; Droppleman, L.F. Manual for the Profile of Mood States; Educational and Industrial Testing Services: San Diego, CA, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  58. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 28 December 2022).
  59. Rosseel, Y. lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Penninx, B.W.J.H.; Benros, M.E.; Klein, R.S.; Vinkers, C.H. How COVID-19 shaped mental health: From infection to pandemic effects. Nat. Med. 2022, 28, 2027–2037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Kauhanen, L.; Wan Mohd Yunus, W.M.; Lempinen, L.; Peltonen, K.; Gyllenberg, D.; Mishina, K.; Gilbert, S.; Bastola, K.; Brown, J.S.; Sourander, A. A systematic review of the mental health changes of children and young people before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2022, 1, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Negri, A.; Andreoli, G.; Barazzetti, A.; Zamin, C.; Christian, C. Linguistic Markers of the Emotion Elaboration Surrounding the Confinement Period in the Italian Epicenter of COVID-19 Outbreak. Front. Psychology 2020, 11, 568281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Park, C.L. Making sense of the meaning literature: An integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 136, 257–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Boelen, P.A.; De Keijser, J.; Smid, G. Cognitive-behavioral variables mediate the impact of violent loss on post-loss psychopathology. Psychol. Trauma 2015, 7, 382–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Madigan, S. Narrative Therapy; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. May, R.; Yalom, I.D. Existential psychotherapy. In Current Psychotherapies; Corsini, R.J., Wedding, D., Eds.; F E Peacock Publishers: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1989; pp. 363–402. [Google Scholar]
  67. Neimeyer, R.A. Constructivist Psychotherapy: Distinctive Features; Routledge: London, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Neimeyer, R.A. Correspondence with the deceased. In Techniques of Grief Therapy: Creative Practices for Counseling the Bereaved; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 259–261. [Google Scholar]
  69. White, M.K. Maps of Narrative Practice; Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Model from Milman et al. [1]: the scheme represents the tested model of direct (c’) and mediated effects (a, b) of pandemic stressors (predictors) on mental health measurements (outcomes). Figure adapted here with permission.
Figure 1. Model from Milman et al. [1]: the scheme represents the tested model of direct (c’) and mediated effects (a, b) of pandemic stressors (predictors) on mental health measurements (outcomes). Figure adapted here with permission.
Behavsci 13 00162 g001
Figure 2. Schematic representation of the multiple mediation models. We fit a separate model for each predictor-outcome pair sharing a significant bivariate correlation. The paths marked a1–4 represent the effects of each predictor on each of the four mediators. The paths marked b1–4 represent the effects of each of the four mediators on each outcome variable. The path marked c’ represents the controlled direct effect of each predictor on each outcome. All effects were estimated by conditioning on relevant confounders.
Figure 2. Schematic representation of the multiple mediation models. We fit a separate model for each predictor-outcome pair sharing a significant bivariate correlation. The paths marked a1–4 represent the effects of each predictor on each of the four mediators. The paths marked b1–4 represent the effects of each of the four mediators on each outcome variable. The path marked c’ represents the controlled direct effect of each predictor on each outcome. All effects were estimated by conditioning on relevant confounders.
Behavsci 13 00162 g002
Table 1. Participants’ distribution for demographic variables and for indirect and direct stressors.
Table 1. Participants’ distribution for demographic variables and for indirect and direct stressors.
Demographic Variables
EducationPrimary school = 33 (4.8%)
Secondary school = 260 (38.2%)
Post-secondary school = 387 (57.0%)
Marital statusSingle = 97 (14.3%)
In a relationship (not living together) = 48 (7.0%)
In a relationship (living together) = 473 (69.6%)
Divorced/widowed = 62 (9.1%)
Children in the houseNone = 414 (60.9%)
One = 109 (16.0%)
Two = 127 (18.7%)
Three = 26 (3.8%)
Four = 4 (0.6%)
Caregiver role51 (7.5%)
Physical illness303 (44.6%)
Mental illness13 (1.9%)
Indirect COVID-19 Stressors
Job loss or reduction336 (49.4%)
Economic difficulties361 (53.1%)
Childcare loss335 (49.3%)
Confinement637 (93.7%)
Working from home467 (68.7%)
Leaving home to work406 (59.7%)
Work with COVID patients30 (4.4%)
Direct COVID-19 Stressors
COVID diagnosis85 (12.5%)
COVID deathNone = 134 (19.7%)
Acquaintances = 411 (60.4%)
Significant others = 135 (19.8%)
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for psychological factors and mental health measures.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for psychological factors and mental health measures.
RangeM (SD)
Pandemic-related psychological factors
CBI0; 52.26 (1.27)
ISLES-SF6; 3019.08 (6.85)
Vulnerability0; 52.74 (1.67)
Mortality0; 52.36 (1.77)
Post-pandemic mental health measures
PHQ0; 123.97 (3.09)
CAS0; 191.38 (2.71)
GP-CORE0; 3.571.51 (0.6)
POMS−29; 18733.1 (41.35)
Table 3. Proportion of variance explained at each level of the blockwise multiple regression models.
Table 3. Proportion of variance explained at each level of the blockwise multiple regression models.
OutcomeModel 1Model 2Model 3Model 4
DemographicsDemographics +
Indirect stressors
Demographics +
Indirect stressors +
Direct stressors
Demographics +
Indirect stressors +
Direct stressors +
Psychol. factors
CASR2 = 0.139R2 = 0.144R2 = 0.156 **R2 = 0.288 ***
PHQ-4R2 = 0.144R2 = 0.145R2 = 0.147R2 = 0.327 ***
GP-CORER2 = 0.115R2 = 0.119R2 = 0.122 aR2 = 0.298 ***
POMSR2 = 0.132R2 = 0.140 *R2 = 0.141R2 = 0.334 ***
MeanR2 = 0.133R2 = 0.137R2 = 0.142R2 = 0.312
Note. * a significant R2 increase compared to the previous model at the following significance levels: * p ≤ 0.05, ** p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001; a post hoc comparison showed that Model 3 explained significantly more variance than Model 1; however, single-step R2 changes (from Model 1 to 2 and from Model 2 to 3) were not significant.
Table 4. Regression coefficients for the final models in the blockwise multiple regression analyses.
Table 4. Regression coefficients for the final models in the blockwise multiple regression analyses.
PredictorsCASPHQ-4GP-COREPOMS
Estimate (b)pEstimate (b)pEstimate (b)pEstimate (b)p
(Intercept)−0.330.6853.31<0.0011.35<0.00113.900.245
Gender−0.400.036−0.310.139 0.030.449 0.950.737
Education−0.260.100 0.160.360 0.020.562 3.530.138
Age 0.010.355−0.04<0.001−0.01<0.001−0.40<0.001
MS single a 0.050.816 0.050.849 0.080.098 4.520.171
MS relationship a−0.150.587 0.070.814−0.060.379−0.880.834
MS cohabiting a−0.100.554−0.130.469−0.040.217−2.370.330
Children−0.060.625 0.110.373−0.010.819 1.030.545
Caretaker role 0.220.517 0.740.051 0.100.199 8.690.086
Physical illness 0.180.3890.580.0110.160.0016.150.044
Mental illness5.29<0.0014.34<0.0010.91<0.00166.32<0.001
Job loss or reduction 0.020.944−0.790.044−0.200.011−12.090.021
Economic difficulties 0.100.764 0.670.0740.150.04412.380.013
Childcare loss 0.460.094 0.020.950 0.030.629 1.380.732
Working from home−0.140.579 0.020.956−0.020.778−1.520.678
Leaving home to work−0.540.027−0.320.232−0.030.523−1.480.677
Confinement 0.400.287−0.280.500−0.020.803−5.100.361
Working with COVID patients−0.050.915−0.210.668−0.100.284−0.610.926
COVID diagnosis−0.030.912 0.020.950 0.010.875−1.810.654
COVID death0.320.025 0.040.815 0.010.816 0.510.810
CBI0.50<0.0010.47<0.0010.10<0.0017.26<0.001
ISLES−0.020.117−0.010.530−0.010.108−0.220.272
Vulnerability 0.050.5290.58<0.0010.08<0.0017.43<0.001
Mortality0.26<0.001−0.020.819 0.020.094−0.220.822
Note. Unstandardized regression coefficients. Bold numbers indicate significant effects p < 0.05; MS = marital status; a compared to marital status = “divorced or widowed”.
Table 5. Direct and indirect effects in multiple mediation models.
Table 5. Direct and indirect effects in multiple mediation models.
StressorOutcomeMediatorControlled Direct Effect b
CBI aISLES aVulnerability aMortality a
Job loss or reductionCAS0.24
[0.11, 0.4]
0.05
[0.00, 0.12]
0.01
[−0.02, 0.04]
0.05
[−0.02, 0.13]
0.07
0.700
Job loss or reductionPOMS3.22
[1.44, 5.5]
0.55
[−0.35, 1.62]
1.28
[−0.59, 3.25]
−0.02
[−0.56, 0.48]
−3.02
0.268
Childcare lossCAS0.16
[0.04, 0.31]
0.06
[0.00, 0.14]
0.01
[−0.02, 0.05]
0.04
[−0.04, 0.13]
0.19
0.341
Childcare lossPOMS2.10
[0.55, 4.18]
0.58
[−0.43, 1.70]
1.35
[−0.80, 3.61]
−0.01
[−0.46, 0.43]
−0.65
0.828
Economic difficultiesCAS0.28
[0.14, 0.45]
0.05
[0.00, 0.11]
0.01
[−0.02, 0.06]
0.06
[−0.01, 0.14]
0.11
0.544
Economic difficultiesPHQ-40.24
[0.11, 0.39]
0.02
[−0.05, 0.08]
0.17
[0.02, 0.33]
0.00
[−0.05, 0.04]
−0.11
0.579
Economic difficultiesGP-CORE0.05
[0.02, 0.08]
0.01
[0.00, 0.03]
0.02
[0.00, 0.05]
0.01
[0.00, 0.02]
−0.02
0.670
Economic difficultiesPOMS3.61
[1.67, 5.94]
0.45
[−0.37, 1.43]
2.16
[0.28, 4.34]
0.00
[−0.54, 0.52]
1.38
0.615
Working from homePHQ-40.03
[−0.05, 0.13]
0.00
[−0.02, 0.02]
0.07
[−0.09, 0.25]
0.00
[−0.03, 0.03]
−0.20
0.367
Working from homePOMS0.55
[−0.88, 2.18]
0.04
[−0.30, 0.46]
0.89
[−1.19, 3.13]
0.01
[−0.34, 0.38]
−2.09
0.480
Leaving home to workPOMS2.59
[1.00, 4.67]
0.20
[−0.22, 0.97]
1.38
[−0.53, 3.51]
−0.03
[−0.61, 0.47]
−2.28
0.415
COVID-19 deathsCAS0.08
[0.00, 0.17]
−0.01
[−0.04, 0.02]
0.01
[−0.03, 0.05]
0.10
[0.04, 0.19]
0.31
0.029
Note. Each model included confounders (specified in the main text) besides the stressor, mediator, and outcome variables illustrated here. a bootstrapped estimates and confidence intervals are shown; b multiple regression coefficient and p-value are shown. In bold, the significant effects (CIs do not include 0 or p < 0.05).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Negri, A.; Conte, F.; Caldiroli, C.L.; Neimeyer, R.A.; Castiglioni, M. Psychological Factors Explaining the COVID-19 Pandemic Impact on Mental Health: The Role of Meaning, Beliefs, and Perceptions of Vulnerability and Mortality. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 162. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13020162

AMA Style

Negri A, Conte F, Caldiroli CL, Neimeyer RA, Castiglioni M. Psychological Factors Explaining the COVID-19 Pandemic Impact on Mental Health: The Role of Meaning, Beliefs, and Perceptions of Vulnerability and Mortality. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(2):162. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13020162

Chicago/Turabian Style

Negri, Attà, Federica Conte, Cristina L. Caldiroli, Robert A. Neimeyer, and Marco Castiglioni. 2023. "Psychological Factors Explaining the COVID-19 Pandemic Impact on Mental Health: The Role of Meaning, Beliefs, and Perceptions of Vulnerability and Mortality" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 2: 162. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13020162

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop