Next Article in Journal
Mediating Effect of Psychological Process Variables on the Relationship between Dysfunctional Coping and Psychopathologies: A Comparative Study on Psychopathologies during COVID-19
Previous Article in Journal
Gender Differences in Attachment Anxiety and Avoidance and Their Association with Psychotherapy Use—Examining Students from a German University
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Human-like Behavior of Service Robot Affects Social Distance: A Mediation Model and Cross-Cultural Comparison

1
School of Economics and Management, Chongqing University of Posts and Telecommunications, Chongqing 400065, China
2
Post-Doctoral Station of Business Administration, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, China
3
Institute of Tourism, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai 200234, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2022, 12(7), 205; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12070205
Submission received: 9 May 2022 / Revised: 3 June 2022 / Accepted: 21 June 2022 / Published: 22 June 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Cognition)

Abstract

:
Previous studies on the human likeness of service robots have focused mainly on their human-like appearance and used psychological constructs to measure the outcomes of human likeness. Unlike previous studies, this study focused on the human-like behavior of the service robot and used a sociological construct, social distance, to measure the outcome of human likeness. We constructed a conceptual model, with perceived competence and warmth as mediators, based on social-identity theory. The hypotheses were tested through online experiments with 219 participants from China and 180 participants from the US. Similar results emerged for Chinese and American participants in that the high (vs. low) human-like behavior of the service robot caused the participants to have stronger perceptions of competence and warmth, both of which contributed to a smaller social distance between humans and service robots. Perceptions of competence and warmth completely mediated the positive effect of the human-like behavior of the service robot on social distance. Furthermore, Chinese participants showed higher anthropomorphism (perceived human-like behavior) and a stronger perception of warmth and smaller social distance. The perception of competence did not differ across cultures. This study provides suggestions for the human-likeness design of service robots to promote natural interaction between humans and service robots and increase human acceptance of service robots.

1. Introduction

Robots can be used to perform a series of complex actions [1]. A service robot performs service tasks for humans or devices [2]. It is an autonomous robot capable of interacting with people and completing specific service tasks [1]. The development of artificial-intelligence technology has popularized service robots, such as educational robots, therapeutic robots, and entertainment robots [3]. However, human acceptance of service robots is the main obstacle to popularizing service robots [4]. Since service robots have certain social attributes [5] and human-like characteristics that can encourage humans to treat service robots as social participants, human-like characteristics can influence the service effectiveness of robots [5,6]. The human-like characteristics of robots can effectively influence human attitudes toward robots [6]. Human acceptance of the human-like characteristics of robots promotes human acceptance of service robots [7], whereas human non-acceptance of the human-like characteristics of robots inhibits human acceptance of service robots [6].
However, scholars have different views on human acceptance of human-like robots. Some scholars believe that humans have positive emotions toward human-like robots and are more willing to deal with a robot that has more human-like features [7,8,9], while some other scholars believe that more human-like robots can cause fear and anxiety in people, decreasing their willingness to interact with the robot [6,10]. This paper focused on how the human likeness of a service robot affects human acceptance of it.
Human likeness refers to the degree to which a robot looks and behaves like a human [11]. Human likeness includes two categories: human-like appearance and human-like behavior [12,13]. Appearance describes the static aspects of the robot (look, sound, sense of touch, etc.) [14,15,16], while behavior describes the dynamic aspects of the robot (actions, expressions, emotions, etc.) [11,12]. To enhance the human likeness of the service robot, the designer would endow the service robot with more human characteristics. For example, the designer would make a robot’s face look like a human’s or add more human characteristics to its actions [13]. A few previous empirical studies have explored the human-like behavior of service robots (HLBR) [8]. However, this factor also has an important effect on human–robot interaction [8]. Therefore, this paper focused on the effects of HLBR on human acceptance of a service robot.
In previous studies, scholars used two types of constructs to measure human acceptance of a service robot: (i) psychological constructs, such as trust [16,17,18], likes [11], use intention [19], and satisfaction [20]; and (ii) sociological constructs, such as social distance [21]. Most studies have employed psychological constructs, while few have employed sociological constructs. However, it is important to examine the human acceptance of a service robot from a sociological perspective. The previous literature has shown that the social rules in people-to-people interactions apply to human–robot interaction [22], and robots can be viewed as social actors with specific behavioral patterns [23]. This paper focused on the sociological aspect of human beings’ acceptance of service robots (i.e., social distance). Social distance refers to the closeness of the relationship between the two individuals in people-to-people interactions [24]. The social distance between humans and service robots (SDHR) can measure the closeness of the relationship between humans and service robots [25]. Thus, SDHR can indicate human acceptance of a robot [21].
This paper examined how HLBR affects SDHR. This paper has two contributions. First, this paper extends the outcomes of HLBR. Concerning the effect of HLBR on human acceptance of a robot, previous studies examined only the attitudes [8] and likes [26] but not any other outcomes. This paper examined the effect of HLBR on SDHR. Second, this paper introduced perceived competence and perceived warmth as mediators. The mediating effect of HLBR and SDHR has not been studied in the previous literature. This paper employed the social-identity theory to explain the mediating effect of HLBR and SDHR.
In addition, previous studies have shown that cultural background can affect human responses to robots [27,28,29]. In the US and China, robots are widely used in various fields, including the service industry [30], such as Sony’s entertainment robot AIBO and Takara’s home-care robot TERA [27]. However, the two countries differ in their views on robots. Americans regard robots as assistants, while Chinese tend to regard robots as friends [31]. It is generally believed that the US is an individualist country, and China is a collectivist country [32,33]. Compared to individualism, interpersonal relationships are more intimate in the context of collectivism [34]. Social rules in interpersonal communication can also apply to human–robot interaction [22]. A cross-cultural study of human–robot interaction found that Chinese people have a higher sense of intimacy with robots than Americans [27]. Therefore, we validated our theoretical model with participants from two different cultures (China and the US).

2. Literature Review and Hypothesis Development

2.1. Human Likeness and Social Distance

In the process of human–robot interaction, humans would perceive social distance from the robot [25]. Social distance can be understood as the closeness between two individuals’ relationships [35]. SDHR is the result of the dynamic interaction between human attributes (gender, age, and the experience dealing with the robot) and robot attributes (appearance and interaction cues) [25]. Previous studies have found that humans naturally attribute human characteristics to non-human objects [14]. Consumers would spontaneously give human attributes to, for example, cars [36] or brands [37]. Human-like service robots have some characteristics of humans [4]. The higher the human likeness of the robot, the richer the human characteristics of the robot, and the stronger the human perception of the similarity between robot and human [38]. Perceived similarity can affect an individual’s perceived social distance; the higher the similarity, the smaller the social distance [39,40]. As an aspect of the robot’s human likeness, a higher HLBR can also lead to a smaller SDHR. Based on this, we proposed the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
The higher the HLBR is, the smaller the SDHR.

2.2. Human Likeness as well as Competence and Warmth

Anthropomorphism is the tendency to attribute human-like qualities to non-human objects [41,42]. The robot’s human-like appearance can promote the robot’s anthropomorphism [8]. Anthropomorphism can enhance human emotional attachment to non-human objects in service scenarios [43]. When humans interact with anthropomorphized robots, they may feel an affinity with the robots [44]. Warmth and competence are basic dimensions used to characterize others [45]. Human perception of the robot’s competence is related to the capabilities, intelligence, skills, and other characteristics of the service robot, while the human perception of the warmth of the robot is related to the caring, friendliness, sociability, and other characteristics of the service robot [8,45]. Anthropomorphism affects these two basic judgment dimensions [6,46,47]. Studies have found that if the robot is anthropomorphized by the HLBR, human perception of the robot’s competence may increase [6], and human perception of the warmth of the robot may become more positive [8,46]. Therefore, we can speculate that HLBR may affect human perceptions of the competence and warmth of the service robot. A service robot with a high human-like behavior should be considered more competent and warmer by humans. Based on this, we proposed the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
The higher the HLBR is, the stronger the perceived competence.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
The higher the HLBR is, the stronger the perceived warmth.

2.3. Competence, Warmth, and Social Distance

Social distance reflects the consciousness of kind in human sociological attributes [25,48]. The social-identity theory holds that humans would categorize individuals based on social-categorization cues [45]. Human perceptions of the competence and warmth of robots would serve as social-categorization cues [49,50] and affect the results of categorizing the social groups of robots by humans [43]. The subjective categorization of inter-and intra-social groups affects social distance [39]. When humans regard other individuals as members of the same group, social distance tends to be smaller [40]. Therefore, we can speculate that the stronger the human perception of the competence and warmth of the robot, the smaller the SDHR would be. Based on this, we proposed the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 4 (H4).
The stronger the perceived competence is, the smaller the SDHR.
Hypothesis 5 (H5).
The stronger the perceived warmth is, the smaller the SDHR.

2.4. Mediating Effects of Perceived Competence and Perceived Warmth

Studies suggest that humans tend to be attracted to human-like objects because of their conformity with humans [36,51]. Competence and warmth are the two universal dimensions of human-impression formation [45,49], accounting for almost 80% of human impressions of others [49]. The robot’s human likeness can significantly affect these two basic judgment dimensions [47]. Van Doorn et al. found that perceived competence and perceived warmth mediate the relationship between a robot’s human likeness and the service performance of the robot (such as customer satisfaction and loyalty) [52]. Kim et al. found that the human likeness of the service robot affects consumer attitudes toward the service robot indirectly through competence and warmth [8]. Social distance is a construct close to satisfaction and attitude [25]. Therefore, we speculated that perceived competence and perceived warmth might mediate the relationship between HLBR and SDHR. Based on this, we proposed the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 6 (H6).
Perceived competence mediates the relationship between HLBR and SDHR.
Hypothesis 7 (H7).
Perceived warmth mediates the relationship between HLBR and SDHR.

3. Methods

3.1. Participants

This study recruited participants from different cultural backgrounds. Among them, the 219 Chinese participants were recruited from Credamo (www.credamo.com) [53,54], while the 180 American participants were recruited from MTurk (www.mturk.com) [17,55,56,57]. All the participants from Credamo were Chinese. The participants from MTurk were people from different countries, so we selected the US participants by setting three questions (Where were you born? Where do you live? Have you ever lived in a country other than the United States for more than three months?). The characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1.

3.2. Procedure

The Chongqing University of Posts and Telecommunications Ethics Committee reviewed the experimental procedures (School of Economics and Management, Project-2022-0003). We employed two videos as stimuli, which were used by scholars in previous studies [8] and which present the scenes of humans talking with robots. In the two videos, the robots’ appearances were the same, and the questions humans asked of the robots were also the same, but the robots answered and acted differently. In the low-HLBR setting, the robot took no actions, and its language expression had no emotional characteristics. In the high-HLBR setting, the robot could move its hands, nod its head, and express strong emotions through language. The conversations in the videos were in English. The videos for the Chinese participants contained Chinese subtitles. We employed back-translation techniques. Two research assistants, who were native Chinese but excellent in English, translated the videos. One of them translated the English conversations in the videos into Chinese, while the other subsequently translated the Chinese version back into English. A professor of consumer behavior compared the back-translated English with the English conversations in the videos to ensure language equivalence. See Appendix A for the URL of the full videos.
The procedures for the Chinese and American participants were the same. The introductory language and questionnaire items were in Chinese and English for the Chinese and American participants, respectively. We employed the same back-translation process that we used when translating the video conversations, to ensure language equivalence. The participants were first informed of the purpose of the experiment and the ways in which the data would be used. Subsequently, they were randomly assigned to a setting (high HLBR vs. low HLBR) to watch the corresponding video. On-page time for watching the video was at least the total time of the video (31 s/38 s). Next, the participants answered two questions related to the video contents (What is the robot’s main job in the video? What type of robot is the robot in the video?) to check whether they had truly engaged with the experiment and understood the video correctly. The robots in the videos provided answers to both questions. Finally, the participants responded to the questionnaire (with attention-check questions inserted) based on the video contents. After completing the questionnaire, each Chinese participant was paid RMB 3 (about USD 0.45), while each American participant was paid USD 0.4. We excluded any unqualified participants (participants who wrongly answered any question related to the video contents, wrongly answered any question among the attention check questions, or selected the same answer for all questions, or whose answers showed obvious regularity). The data from the remaining participants were used for analysis.

3.3. Measurement

The questionnaire items were adopted from previous studies, and some of them were adapted to fit the service-robot settings. The measurement items and sources of each construct are shown in Table 2. All items were measured on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). In addition to measuring the three constructs (perceived competence, perceived warmth, and SDHR) included in the hypotheses, we also measured anthropomorphism. This construct (anthropomorphism) reflects the perception of HLBR of the participants, and we used it to test whether our manipulation of HLBR was successful. During subsequent hypothesis testing, we treated the HLBR as a dummy variable (1 = high, 0 = low).

4. Results

4.1. Reliability and Validity

We included the Chinese and American participants in the reliability and validity analysis [56,57], using SPSS 23.0 and LISREL 8.80. As shown in Table 3, the Cronbach’s α value of each construct exceeded the acceptable cut-off point of 0.7. The fit of the CFA model (four factors) was acceptable (χ2 = 274.697, df = 84, χ2/df = 3.270, RMSEA = 0.076, SRMR = 0.052, NNFI = 0.977, CFI = 0.981, IFI = 0.981, RFI = 0.967, GFI = 0.916, and AGFI = 0.880). The standardized factor loadings of each item were between 0.701 and 0.930 and were significant (p < 0.001). The combined reliability (CR) of each construct was greater than 0.7, and the average variance extracted (AVE) was greater than 0.5. The square root of each AVE was greater than the correlation coefficient between the constructs, as seen in Table 4. The above results indicated that the reliability, convergent validity, and differential validity of all the constructs were acceptable [61,62]. In addition, the fit of Harman’s single-factor model (χ2 = 1824.681, df = 90, χ2/df = 20.274, RMSEA = 0.220, SRMR = 0.111, NNFI = 0.803, CFI = 0.831, IFI = 0.832, RFI = 0.795, GFI = 0.621, and AGFI = 0.494) was significantly worse than that of the four-factor model, indicating that one latent variable cannot be used to replace the four factors. Therefore, the effect of common-method bias was not an issue in this study.

4.2. Manipulation Check

The independent samples t-test on the scores of anthropomorphism (the participants’ perception of HLBR) under the two settings of HLBR (high vs. low) was performed using SPSS 23. For the Chinese participants (t = 3.685, p = 0.000, and M high = 5.100 vs. M low =4.384) and the American participants (t = 2.796, p = 0.006, and M high = 4.631 vs M low = 4.027), the between-group difference in HLBR (high vs. low) was significant, indicating that the manipulation was successful.

4.3. Hypothesis Testing

The direct and indirect effects were tested using the PROCESS (Model 4). HLBR (high = 1, low = 0) was the independent variable, SDHR was the dependent variable, and perceived competence and warmth were mediators. Gender (1 = male, 0 = female) and age (natural logarithm) were included in the model as control variables. The bootstrapped sample size was 5000. The results are shown in Table 5, Table 6, Table 7 and Table 8 and in Figure 1.
The results of hypothesis testing was consistent for the Chinese and American participants.
The direct effect of HLBR on SDHR was not significant; therefore, H1 was not supported. HLBR was positively related to perceived competence, supporting H2. HLBR was positively related to perceived warmth, supporting H3. The perceived competence was negatively related to SDHR, supporting H4. The perceived warmth was negatively related to SDHR, supporting H5.
Both confidence intervals corresponding to the two mediating effects (HLBR → perceived competence → SDHR and HLBR → perceived warmth → SDHR) do not contain 0, indicating a significant mediating effect, supporting H6 and H7. Perceived competence and perceived warmth fully mediated the proposed relationship, in the absence of the direct effect of HLBR on SDHR.

4.4. Cross-Cultural Comparison

As an extended analysis, we compared the differences between the Chinese and American participants in their scores on the constructs. The results are shown in Table 9. The Chinese and American participants differed significantly in the scores on anthropomorphism (the participants’ perception), perceived warmth, and SDHR, whereas they did not differ in their scores on perceived competence.

5. Discussion

Our mediation-effect model was supported in both groups (the Chinese and American participants), indicating that the mediating effects of perceived competence and perceived warmth are applicable across cultures (China and the US), to a certain extent. However, the two groups differed in some aspects, stemming from cultural differences.
First, the Chinese participants’ score on anthropomorphism was higher than that of the US participants. Cross-cultural studies have found that compared to Americans, Chinese people more strongly advocate animism, so Chinese people are more inclined to anthropomorphize robots [31]. Therefore, when faced with the same HLBR, the Chinese people more strongly anthropomorphized the service robot in the experiment.
Second, the Chinese participants’ score on perceived warmth was higher than that of the American participants. Since the external characteristics of service robots (such as small size and slow movement speed) are more in line with Eastern cultural preferences [27], people from Eastern cultural backgrounds might have a more positive attitude toward human-like robots and a higher evaluation of the robot’s cuteness and friendliness [27,63]. These characteristics are linked to the human perception of the warmth of the human-like robot [45]. As a result, the Chinese people had a stronger perception of the warmth of the human-like robot.
Finally, the Chinese participants’ score on SDHR was lower than that of the American participants. Previous studies have shown that Americans are high on individualism, while Chinese are high on collectivism [32,33,34]. Compared to individualists, collectivists have a more pronounced in-group preference and maintain a smaller social distance from in-group members [39]. Since the Chinese people tend to view robots as in-group members [64], they keep a smaller social distance from human-like service robots.

5.1. Theoretical Contributions

First, this paper extends the study on the outcomes of HLBR. Few previous empirical studies have examined HLBR, and the existing literature has mainly discussed HLBR related to customer attitude [8] and user preference [29]. This paper linked HLBR with SDHR and examined the outcome of HLBR from a sociological perspective.
Second, this paper extended the study on the antecedents of SDHR. Social distance is an important indicator of human acceptance of robots [21], but only a few empirical studies have explored the antecedents of social distance. The existing literature has studied only the effect of the robot’s language form on social distance [25]. This paper extended the antecedents of social distance to HLBR (including the conversation contents, voice intonation, and actions of the robots when they interact with humans) to investigate the effect of the robot’s behavioral characteristics on SDHR.
Third, this paper demonstrated that perceived competence and warmth mediate the relationship between HLBR and its outcome. Previous studies on the robot’s human likeness and human acceptance of the robot have found that mediators include social-interaction needs [43] and the sense of social presence [65]. The mediators (i.e., perceived competence and perceived warmth) identified in this paper are fundamental dimensions of social cognition [45], and they help us understand the mechanisms by which HLBR produces outcomes, from the perspective of human perception.
Fourth, this cross-cultural study tested the model’s universality, with participants from China and the US. This paper also found differences between the Chinese and American participants in their perception of service robots. These results complement cross-cultural studies on human attitudes toward robots [66,67].

5.2. Practical Implications

First, it is necessary to consider human-like behavior when designing service robots. Robots’ expressions, attitudes, and actions can enhance human acceptance of service robots. This paper responded to the debate on the necessity of the human-like design of robots [7,9,10,19]. The findings of our study suggest that investing resources in the design of HLBR is beneficial.
Second, designing the HLBR is conducive to humans’ acceptance of service robots. The findings of our study showed that the design of HLBR can enhance human in-group identification with service robots (represented by a smaller social distance), making service robots not only accepted by humans but also better integrated with human groups in a sociological sense [25,68].
Finally, the detailed design of HLBR, such as the conversation contents, specific voice intonation, and specific actions, should aim to promote individuals’ positive perceptions of the competence and warmth of service robots as the mediators of the relationship between a robot’s human-like behavior and service satisfaction [43,49,50].

5.3. Limitations and Future Research

First, the model in this paper did not include human characteristics. However, the existing studies showed that human characteristics are also important factors affecting SDHR. Future research may incorporate human characteristics into the model. Second, this paper used video as the stimuli, rather than stimuli generated by real contact between humans and robots, which may have led to different results than would be expected. Future research may conduct field studies to examine real human—robot contact. Third, the Chinese and American participants showed some differences in the perception of service robots, for multiple reasons that we discussed in the previous section. Future research may conduct a more nuanced cross-cultural analysis. Fourth, we selected participants based on their country of origin; therefore, selection bias may have affected the experimental results. Future research could improve the selection method.

6. Conclusions

This paper aimed to explore the effects of HLBR on SDHR. Based on social identity theory, we constructed a conceptual model with perceived competence and perceived warmth as mediating variables. We recruited participants from China (219) and the United States (180) to complete the online experiment. Through empirical testing, we found that high HLBR (vs. low) led to higher perceived competence and perceived warmth, which shortened SDHR.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.L. and Y.L.; methodology, Y.L. and L.L.; software, C.W. and L.L.; validation, Z.S. and L.L.; formal analysis, Y.L. and L.L.; investigation, Z.S. and Y.L.; resources, Y.L. and B.S.; data curation, Y.L. and B.S.; writing—original draft preparation, L.L.; writing—review and editing, L.L. and Y.L.; visualization, L.L.; supervision, Y.L.; project administration, Y.L.; funding acquisition, Y.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The study was funded by the Center for Japanese Studies of Chongqing University of Posts and Telecommunications (grant number K2020-215).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Video link:
Low HLBR (English) https://youtu.be/1fhWUXfzs30 (Accessed on 25 August 2021)
High HLBR (English) https://youtu.be/IqXtITebLWc (Accessed on 25 August 2021)
Low HLBR (Chinese) https://b23.tv/TZkDvu9 (Accessed on 1 September 2021)
High HLBR (Chinese) https://b23.tv/gv8IotN (Accessed on 1 September 2021)

References

  1. Wirtz, J.; Patterson, P.G.; Kunz, W.H. Brave new world: Service robots in the frontline. J. Serv. Manag. 2018, 29, 907–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. IFR. “Service Robots”. Available online: https://www.ifr.org/service-robots/ (accessed on 10 March 2019).
  3. Lee, I. Service Robots: A Systematic Literature Review. Electronics 2021, 10, 2658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Castelo, N.; Schmitt, B.; Sarvary, M. Human or robot? Consumer responses to radical cognitive enhancement products. J. Associat. Consum. Res. 2019, 4, 217–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Choi, S.; Liu, S.Q.; Mattil, A.S. “How may I help you?” Says a robot: Examining language styles in the service encounter. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2019, 82, 32–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Duffy, B.R. Anthropomorphism and the Social Robot. Robot. Auton. Syst. 2003, 42, 177–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Kiesler, S.; Powers, A.; Fussell, S.R.; Torrey, C. Anthropomorphic interactions with a robot and robot–like agent. Soc. Cognit. 2008, 26, 169–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kim, S.Y.; Schmitt, B.H.; Thalmann, N.M. Eliza in the uncanny valley: Anthropomorphizing consumer robots increases their perceived warmth but decreases liking. Mark. Lett. 2019, 30, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hancock, P.A.; Billings, D.R.; Schaefer, K.E.; Chen, J.Y.; De Visser, E.J.; Parasuraman, R. A Meta-Analysis of Factors Affecting Trust in Human-Robot Interaction. Hum. Factors 2011, 53, 517–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Murphy, J.; Gretzel, U.; Pesonen, J. Marketing robot services in hospitality and tourism: The role of anthropomorphism. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 2019, 36, 784–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zitzewitz, J.V.; Boesch, P.M.; Wolf, P.; Riener, R. Quantifying the human likeness of a humanoid robot. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2013, 5, 263–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Minato, T.; Shimada, M.; Itakura, S.; Lee, K.; Ishiguro, H. Evaluating the human likeness of an android by comparing gaze behaviors elicited by the android and a person. Adv. Robot. 2006, 20, 1147–1163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Choi, J.; Kim, M. The usage and evaluation of anthropomorphic form in robot design. In Proceedings of the Design Research Society Conference, Sheffield, UK, 16–19 July 2008. [Google Scholar]
  14. Epley, N.; Waytz, A.; Cacioppo, J.T. On Seeing a Human: A Three-Factor Theory of Anthropomorphism. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 114, 864–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Yao, S.; Luximon, A.; Yan, L. The Effect of Facial Features on Facial Anthropomorphic Trustworthiness in Social. Robots. Appl. Ergon. 2021, 94, 103420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bernotat, J.; Eyssel, F.; Sachse, J. The (Fe)male Robot: How Robot Body Shape Impacts First Impressions and Trust Towards Robots. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2021, 13, 477–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Kim, W.; Kim, N.; Lyons, J.B.; Chang, S.N. Factors affecting trust in high-vulnerability human-robot interaction contexts: A structural equation modelling approach. Appl. Ergon. 2020, 85, 103056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Christoforakos, L.; Gallucci, A.; Surmava-Große, T.; Ullrich, D.; Diefenbach, S. Can Robots Earn Our Trust the Same Way Humans Do? A Systematic Exploration of warmth, competence, and Anthropomorphism as Determinants of Trust Development in HRI. Front. Robot. AI 2021, 8, 640444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pinxteren, M.V.; Wetzels, R.J.; Pluymaekers, M. Trust in humanoid robots: Implications for services marketing. J. Serv. Mark. 2019, 33, 507–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Jia, J.W.; Chung, N.; Hwang, J. Assessing the hotel service robot interaction on tourists’ behaviour: The role of anthropomorphism. Ind. Manag. Data Syst. 2021, 121, 1457–1478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kim, Y.; Mutlu, B. How social distance shapes human–robot interaction. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Stud. 2014, 72, 783–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lee, M.K.; Kiesler, S.; Forlizzi, J.; Srinivasa, S.; Rybski, P. Gracefully mitigating breakdowns in robotic services. In Proceedings of the 5th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human Robot Interaction, Osaka, Japan, 2–5 March 2010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Rahwan, I.; Cebrian, M.; Obradovich, N.; Bongard, J.; Bonnefon, J.F.; Breazeal, C.; Crandall, J.W.; Christakis, N.A.; Couzin, I.D.; Jackson, M.O.; et al. Machine behaviour. Nature 2019, 568, 477–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Liviatan, I.; Trope, Y.; Liberman, N. Interpersonal similarity as a social distance dimension: Implications for perception of others’ actions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1256–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Kim, Y.; Kwak, S.S.; Kim, M. Am I acceptable to you? Effect of a robot’s verbal language forms on people’s social distance from robots. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013, 29, 1091–1101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Walters, M.L.; Syrdal, D.S.; Dautenhahn, K.; Te Boekhorst, R.; Koay, K.L. Avoiding the uncanny valley: Robot appearance, personality and consistency of behavior in an attention-seeking home scenario for a robot companion. Aoton. Robot. 2008, 24, 159–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Li, D.; Rau, P.; Ye, L. A Cross-cultural Study: Effect of Robot Appearance and Task. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2010, 2, 175–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Eresha, G.; Häring, M.; Endrass, B.; André, E.; Obaid, M. Investigating the influence of culture on proxemic behaviors for humanoid robots. In Proceedings of the IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, Gyeongju, Korea, 26–29 August 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ho, Y.; Sato-Shimokawara, E.; Yamaguchi, T.; Tagawa, N. Interaction robot system considering culture differences. In Proceedings of the IEEE Workshop on Advanced Robotics and Its Social Impacts, Tokyo, Japan, 7–9 November 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Cette, G.; Devillard, A.; Spiezia, V. The contribution of robots to productivity growth in 30 OECD countries over 1975–2019. Econ. Lett. 2021, 200, 109762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Evers, V.; Maldonado, H.C.; Brodecki, T.L.; Hinds, P.J. Relational vs. group self-construal: Untangling the role of national culture in HRI. In Proceedings of the 2008 3rd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), New York, NY, USA, 12–15 March 2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Germani, A.; Delvecchio, E.; Nartova-Bochaver, S.K.; Li, J.B.; Lis, A.; Vazsonyi, A.T.; Mazzeschi, C. The link between individualism–collectivism and life satisfaction among emerging adults from four countries. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 2021, 13, 437–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Chang, J.; Panjwani, A.; Perera, S.; Steinberg, H. Information Technology Customer Service, Cultural Differences, & the Big 5 in China and the USA. In Allied Academies International Conference: Proceedings of the Academy of Management Information & Decision Sciences; Jordan Whitney Enterprises, Inc.: Candler, NC, USA, 2016; Volume 20, pp. 1–5. [Google Scholar]
  34. Oyserman, D.; Coon, H.M.; Kemmelmeier, M. Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 2002, 128, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Bogardus, E.S. Measurement of personal-group relations. Sociometry 1947, 10, 306–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Aggarwal, P.; McGill, A.L. Is that car smiling at me? Schema congruity as a basis for evaluating anthropomorphized products. J. Consum. Res. 2007, 34, 468–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Denntt, D.C. Kinds of Minds: Towards an Understanding of Consciousness; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1996; p. 184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Seyama, J.I.; Nagayama, R.S. The uncanny valley: Effect of realism on the impression of artificial human faces. Presence 2007, 16, 337–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bar-Anan, Y.; Liberman, N.; Trope, Y. The association between psychological distance and construal level: Evidence from an implicit association test. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 135, 609–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kruglansik, A.W.; Higgins, E.T. Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 540–561. [Google Scholar]
  41. Guido, G.; Peluso, A.M. Brand anthropomorphism: Conceptualization, measurement, and impact on brand personality and loyalty. J. Brand Manag. 2015, 22, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. DiSalvo, C.; Gemperle, F. From seduction to fulfillment: The use of anthropomorphic form indesign. In Proceedings of the Designing Pleasurable Products and Interfaces Conference, Pittsburgh, PA, USA, 23–26 June 2003; Available online: https://dl.acm.org/doi/10.1145/782896.782913 (accessed on 22 June 2021).
  43. Belanche, D.; Casaló, L.V.; Schepers, J.; Flavián, C. Examining the effects of robots’ physical appearance, warmth, and competence in frontline services: The Humanness-Value-Loyalty model. Psychol. Mark. 2021, 38, 2357–2376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Komatsu, T.; Takahashi, H. How does unintentional eye contact with a robot affect users’ emotional attachment to it? Investigation on the effects of eye contact and joint attention on users’ emotional attachment to a robot. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Universal Access in Human-Computer Interaction: User & Context Diversity, Las Vegas, NV, USA, 21–26 July 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Fiske, S.T.; Cuddy, A.J.; Glick, P. Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Zhu, D.H.; Chang, Y.P. Robot with Humanoid Hands Cooks Food Better? Effect of Robotic Chef Anthropomorphism on Food Quality Prediction. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2020, 32, 1367–1383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Scott, M.L.; Martin, M.; Lisa, E.B. Judging the Book by Its Cover? How Consumers Decode Conspicuous Consumption Cues in Buyer-Seller Relationships. J. Mark. Res. 2013, 50, 334–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Duvall, R. The Conflict Helix. In Understanding Conflict and War; Rummel, R.J., Ed.; Halsted Press: Beverly Hills, CA, USA, 1976; Volume 2, p. 400. [Google Scholar]
  49. Cuddy, A.J.; Glick, P.; Beninger, A. The dynamics of warmth and competence judgments, and their outcomes in organizations. Res. Organ. Behav. 2011, 31, 73–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Rosenthal-von der Püthen, A.M.; Kramer, N.C. How design characteristics of robots determine evaluation and uncanny valley related responses. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014, 36, 422–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Bartneck, C.; Kulic, D.; Croft, E.; Zoghbi, S. Measurement instruments for the anthropomorphism, animacy, likeability, perceived intelligence, and perceived safety of robots. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2009, 1, 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Doorn, J.V.; Mende, M.; Noble, S.M.; Hulland, J.; Ostrom, A.L.; Grewal, D.; Petersen, J.A. Domo arigato mr. roboto: Emergence of automated social presence in organizational frontlines and customers service experiences. J. Serv. Res. 2016, 20, 43–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Gong, S.; Lu, J.G.; Schaubroeck, J.M.; Li, Q.; Qian, X. Polluted psyche: Is the effect of air pollution on unethical behavior more physiological or psychological? Psychol. Sci. 2020, 31, 1040–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Gai, P.J.; Puntoni, S. Language and consumer dishonesty: A self-diagnosticity theory. J. Consum. Res. 2021, 47, 333–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Gursoy, D.; Chi, O.H.; Lu, L.; Nunkoo, R. Consumers acceptance of artificially intelligent device use in service delivery. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 2019, 49, 157–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Lin, H.; Chi, O.H.; Gursoy, D. Antecedents of customers’ acceptance of artificially intelligent robotic device use in hospitality services. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2019, 29, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Chi, O.H.; Gursoy, D.; Chi, C.G. Tourists’ attitudes toward the use of artificially intelligent (AI) devices in tourism service delivery: Moderating role of service value seeking. J. Travel Res. 2022, 61, 170–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Yilmaz, S.; Tasci, A. Circumstantial impact of contact on social distance. J. Tour. Cult. Change 2015, 13, 115–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Joo, D.; Tasci, A.D.; Woosnam, K.M. Residents’ attitude towards domestic tourists explained by contact, emotional solidarity and social distance. Tour. Manag. 2018, 64, 245–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Fiske, S.T.; Cuddy, A.J.C.; Glick, P.; Xu, J. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 82, 878–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 24, 337–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Chin, W.; Marcoulides, G. The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling. Adv. Hosp. Leis. 1998, 8, 295–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Haring, K.S.; Silvera-Tawil, D.; Takahashi, T.; Velonaki, M.; Watanabe, K. Perception of a humanoid robot: A cross-cultural comparison. In Proceedings of the 2015 24th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, Kobe, Japan, 31 August–4 September 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Lin, W.; Hinds, P. When in Rome: The role of culture & context in adherence to robot recommendations. In Proceedings of the 2010 5th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), Osaka, Japan, 2–5 March 2010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kim, K.J.; Park, E.; Sundar, S.S. Caregiving role in human–robot interaction: A study of the mediating effects of perceived benefit and social presence. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013, 29, 1799–1806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Bröhl, N.J.; Brandl, C.; Mertens, A.; Nitsch, V. Human–robot collaboration acceptance model: Development and comparison for Germany, Japan, China and the USA. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 2019, 11, 709–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Appel, M.; Izydorczyk, D.; Weber, S.; Mara, M.; Lischetzke, T. The uncanny of mind in a machine: Humanoid robots as tools, agents, and experiencers. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2020, 102, 274–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Rummel, R.J. Understanding Conflict and War: The Conflict Helix; Sage Publications: Beverly Hills, CA, USA, 1976; p. 400. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Results of mediation analysis. Note: Two coefficients on the path are China/US; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Results of mediation analysis. Note: Two coefficients on the path are China/US; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Behavsci 12 00205 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the participants.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the participants.
CategoryChina (Frequency)America (Frequency)
GenderMale10099
Female11981
Age18–2510021
26–305652
31–405652
41–50236
51–60413
More than 6016
HLBR settingHigh12793
Low9287
Total219180
Table 2. Measurement items of each construct.
Table 2. Measurement items of each construct.
ConstructItemsReferences
AnthropomorphismHow did you perceive the robot? (Machine-like or Human-like)Bartneck et al., 2009 [51]
How did you perceive the robot? (Fake or Natural)
How did you perceive the robot? (Unconscious or Conscious)
SDHRI think I have the potential to become friends with this robot.Yilmaz et al., 2015 [58]; Joo et al., 2018 [59]
I would like to keep in touch with this robot as a friend.
I would like to introduce this robot to others as my new friend.
I think this robot has the potential to be a dear friend to whom I can pour my heart out.
Perceived CompetenceThe robot is capable.Fiske et al., 2002 [60]
The robot is efficient.
The robot is intelligent.
The robot is skillful.
Perceived WarmthThe robot is friendly.Fiske et al., 2002 [60]
The robot is well-intentioned.
The robot is warm.
The robot is sincere.
Table 3. Results of reliability and validity analysis.
Table 3. Results of reliability and validity analysis.
FactorsItemsStandardized Factor Loadings (λ)T-ValueCronbach’sαComposite
Reliability (CR)
Average Variance Extracted (AVE)
AnthropomorphismAT10.81118.6660.8450.8480.650
AT20.83219.373
AT30.77417.481
SDHRSDHR 10.90423.1620.9400.9410.799
SDHR 20.93024.334
SDHR 30.90423.152
SDHR 40.83520.314
Perceived CompetenceComp10.82919.4770.8770.8800.647
Comp20.78718.056
Comp30.76417.292
Comp40.83519.710
Perceived WarmthWarm10.70115.2990.8590.8580.603
Warm20.73516.310
Warm30.86320.560
Warm40.79618.258
Note: N = 399.
Table 4. The mean, standard deviation, correlation coefficient, and AVE’s square root.
Table 4. The mean, standard deviation, correlation coefficient, and AVE’s square root.
VariableMeanSD1234
1. Anthropomorphism4.5911.4820.806−0.664 ***0.603 ***0.556 ***
2. SDHR3.6911.713−0.738 ***0.894−0.533 ***−0.586 ***
3. Perceived Competence5.0561.1470.685 ***−0.583 ***0.8040.494 ***
4. Perceived Warmth5.2271.1500.666 ***−0.681 ***0.552 ***0.777
Note: Correlation coefficients between latent variables (from LISREL) are below the diagonal, diagonal values represent square root of AVE, and Pearson-correlation coefficients (from SPSS) are above the diagonal; *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Model test of mediation analysis (China).
Table 5. Model test of mediation analysis (China).
Dependent VariableVariableβSET95% Confidence IntervalR2F
LLCIULCI
Perceived CompetenceConstant1.1631.0951.062−0.9953.3200.17214.930 ***
HLBR0.818 ***0.1595.1300.5021.132
Gender0.323 *0.1631.9790.0060.645
Age1.003 **0.3362.9830.0831.666
Perceived WarmthConstant2.617 **0.8603.0440.9224.3120.18616.413 ***
HLBR0.751 ***0.1255.9970.5040.998
Gender0.1400.1281.093−0.1130.393
Age0.756 **0.2642.8630.2361.277
SDHRConstant10.368 ***1.0819.5608.23712.4990.45235.069 ***
HLBR−0.0800.170−0.471−0.4150.255
Perceived Competence−0.469 ***0.070−6.665−0.608−0.330
Perceived Warmth−0.454 ***0.090−5.062−0.630−0.277
Gender−0.2990.160−1.872−0.6130.016
Age−0.6000.335−1.792−1.2600.060
Note: N = 219; LLCI = lower-level confidence interval, ULCI = upper-level confidence interval; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 6. Model test of mediation analysis (US).
Table 6. Model test of mediation analysis (US).
Dependent VariableVariableβSET95% Confidence intervalR2F
LLCIULCI
Perceived CompetenceConstant5.768 ***0.9396.1453.9157.6200.0281.715
HLBR0.310 *0.1472.1080.0200.600
Gender−0.0250.151−0.167−0.3240.274
Age−0.2530.259−0.976−0.7640.258
Perceived WarmthConstant5.891 ***1.0695.5103.7818.0010.0875.583**
HLBR0.610 ***0.1673.6460.2800.940
Gender0.1150.1720.664−0.2260.455
Age−0.4240.295−1.439−1.0060.158
SDHRConstant9.643 ***1.5386.2716.60812.6780.40323.524***
HLBR0.0360.2240.160−0.4060.478
Perceived Competence−0.554 ***0.140−3.971−0.829−0.279
Perceived Warmth−0.563 ***0.123−4.594−0.804−0.321
Gender−0.726 **0.223−3.254−1.1660.286
Age0.1100.383−0.287−0.6460.865
Note: N = 180; LLCI = lower-level confidence interval, ULCI = upper-level confidence interval; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 7. Mediating-effect test (China).
Table 7. Mediating-effect test (China).
EffectBoot SE95% Confidence Interval
LLCIULCI
Direct effect −0.0800.170−0.4150.255
Indirect effectTotal−0.7240.146−1.032−0.457
HLBR → PC → SDHR−0.3840.106−0.605−0.197
HLBR → PW → SDHR−0.3410.100−0.554−0.169
Note: N = 219; LLCI = lower-level confidence interval, ULCI = upper-level confidence interval; PC: perceived competence, PM: perceived warmth.
Table 8. Mediating-effect test (US).
Table 8. Mediating-effect test (US).
EffectBoot SE95% Confidence Interval
LLCIULCI
Direct effect 0.0360.224−0.4060.478
Indirect effectTotal−0.5150.173−0.865−0.190
HLBR → PC → SDHR−0.1720.089−0.357−0.006
HLBR → PW → SDHR−0.3430.130−0.637−0.128
Note: N = 180; LLCI = lower-level confidence interval, ULCI = upper-level confidence interval; PC: perceived competence, PW: perceived warmth.
Table 9. Comparison of variable means for Chinese and American participants.
Table 9. Comparison of variable means for Chinese and American participants.
VariablesChinaUST
Anthropomorphism4.7994.3393.119 **
SDHR3.2814.190−5.364 ***
Perceived Competence5.0885.0180.620
Perceived Warmth5.6074.7657.803 ***
Note: ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, L.; Li, Y.; Song, B.; Shi, Z.; Wang, C. How Human-like Behavior of Service Robot Affects Social Distance: A Mediation Model and Cross-Cultural Comparison. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 205. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12070205

AMA Style

Li L, Li Y, Song B, Shi Z, Wang C. How Human-like Behavior of Service Robot Affects Social Distance: A Mediation Model and Cross-Cultural Comparison. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(7):205. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12070205

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Linyao, Yi Li, Bo Song, Zhaomin Shi, and Chongli Wang. 2022. "How Human-like Behavior of Service Robot Affects Social Distance: A Mediation Model and Cross-Cultural Comparison" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 7: 205. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12070205

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop