Next Article in Journal
Three-Dimensional Models of Liver Vessels for Navigation during Laparotomic Attenuation of Intrahepatic Portosystemic Shunt in Dogs
Next Article in Special Issue
Evolutionary Insights into the Relationship of Frogs, Salamanders, and Caecilians and Their Adaptive Traits, with an Emphasis on Salamander Regeneration and Longevity
Previous Article in Journal
Deletion of the CD2 Gene in the Virulent ASFV Congo Strain Affects Viremia in Domestic Swine, but Not the Virulence
Previous Article in Special Issue
An Isolated and Deeply Divergent Hynobius Species from Fujian, China
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Disentangling Exploitative and Interference Competition on Forest Dwelling Salamanders

Department of Earth, Environment and Life Sciences (DISTAV), University of Genova, Corso Europa 26, 16126 Genova, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(12), 2003; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13122003
Submission received: 12 April 2023 / Revised: 12 June 2023 / Accepted: 13 June 2023 / Published: 15 June 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Salamanders: Distribution, Diversity, and Conservation)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Exploitative competition and interference competition differ in the way access to resources is modulated by a competitor. Exploitative competition implies resource depletion and usually produces spatial segregation, while interference competition is independent from resource availability and can result in temporal niche partitioning. Here, we inferred the presence of these two patterns of competition on a two-salamander system in Northern Italy. We found evidence supporting interference competition and temporal niche partitioning.

Abstract

Exploitative competition and interference competition differ in the way access to resources is modulated by a competitor. Exploitative competition implies resource depletion and usually produces spatial segregation, while interference competition is independent from resource availability and can result in temporal niche partitioning. Our aim is to infer the presence of spatial or temporal niche partitioning on a two-species system of terrestrial salamanders in Northern Italy: Speleomantes strinatii and Salamandrina perspicillata. We conducted 3 repeated surveys on 26 plots in spring 2018, on a sampling site where both species are present. We modelled count data with N-mixture models accounting for directional interactions on both abundance and detection process. In this way we were able to disentangle the effect of competitive interaction on the spatial scale, i.e., local abundance, and from the temporal scale, i.e., surface activity. We found strong evidence supporting the presence of temporal niche partitioning, consistent with interference competition. At the same time, no evidence of spatial segregation has been observed.

1. Introduction

Identifying which ecological processes influence the occurrence and local abundance of a species is of fundamental interest among ecologists and conservationists. Local abundance for a given species is expected to reach its maximum when environmental features correspond to the optimum of its ecological requirements [1,2]. However, the local rise in abundance or density of a species as resources or suitability of a given area increase is counterbalanced and limited by biotic interactions with other organisms that share the same ecological niche, usually through density-dependent competitive interactions [3,4].
Even though the study of competition laid its root early in the field of ecology [5], nowadays, the topic is still alive and debated, and the consequences of competitive interactions are still attracting interest among the scientific community. This happens at the theoretical [6,7,8] and practical level [9,10]. Competitive interactions can occur at the inter- or intra-specific level and can involve various niche axes and mechanisms. For instance, organisms compete directly for mates (e.g., [11,12]) and physical space, such as habitat or shelter use [13], and indirectly for food resources [14] or other niches, such as call space [15]. Regardless of the mechanism or origin of competitive interactions, they usually occur when one species prevents the access of its competitors to a shared resource. This exclusion from use of a resource usually takes place in two different ways: exploitative competition or interference competition [13,16,17]. These two types of competition differ substantially in the way they affect the access of one species to a given resource. In the case of exploitative competition, some organisms make better use of the same limiting resource, directly reducing the availability of the shared resource for other individuals [18]. By contrast, interference competition occurs when one organism actively prevents another organism from accessing a resource, independently from its availability, for instance by means of territorial or aggressive behaviors [13,16].
Both types of competition are assumed to have significant effects on population dynamics, local abundance, community assembly, and species coexistence [5,19,20]. Indeed, exploitative competition can either lead to the exclusion of a species from a given habitat [18], resulting in a density reduction of one competitor, or can trigger resource partitioning among different niche axes [21,22,23]. Interference competition can trigger competitive exclusion [3], causing behavioral changes, for instance by shifting species activity patterns [13,17]. One example of interference competition is the shift to crepuscular activity of a species that avoids the presence of a more nocturnal competitor [17].
Likewise, different kinds of competitive interactions can result in different layouts of niche and resource partitioning, minimizing niche overlap and competitive exclusion [13]. For instance, when two species or organisms segregate over different microhabitats, we observe spatial niche partitioning; conversely, when organisms exploit the same set of resources but in different time frames, we observe a partitioning along the temporal niche [2,24]. In this respect, exploitative and interference competition are also hypothesized to have different outcomes for what concern spatial or temporal segregation/partitioning. Exploitative competition has been found to have an effect on the local abundance or spatial displacement of one or both competitors (e.g., [21]). This is because of a density-dependent effect on limited resources, in which the under-performing competitor must rely on the leftover resource by the best-performing competitor [25,26]. Conversely, interference competition usually produces niche shift patterns that are consistent with temporal niche partitioning, i.e., when species access the same resource in different timeframes in order to reduce their interactions with the competitor (e.g., [17,27,28]).
In the present study, our aim was to test the occurrence of exploitative or interference competition in a two-species system of forest dwelling salamanders, the Strinati’s cave salamander (Speleomantes strinatii) and the northern spectacled salamander (Salamandrina perspicillata). These two salamanders are found syntopically in NW Italy, where they rely on the same set of trophic resources, particularly soil-dwelling invertebrates [29,30]. We used binomial N-mixture models that account for directional biotic interaction on both the local abundance and the timing of surface activity to alternatively test the occurrence of spatial niche partitioning or temporal niche partitioning, after modelling abundance and activity on environmental covariates. We expected that (i) in the case of exploitative competition, we should observe a negative interaction of one species on the local abundance of the other, this being a negative density-dependent effect; or (ii) in the case of interference competition we should observe a negative interaction of one species on the surface activity of the other, this outcome representing niche partitioning on the temporal axis.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Framework

To test for the presence of exploitative or interference competition, we randomly selected 26 permanent plots under a meta-population design [31,32], in one site in Northern Italy where both salamanders are present. We counted salamanders in all plots during three repeated surveys over a short time period. We then used count data to model abundance and activity interaction occurring between the species, using a two-species N-mixture model [31], while incorporating environmental covariates [33,34,35,36,37,38].

2.2. Study Species

The first focal species is the Strinati’s cave salamander Speleomantes strinatii, a plethodontid salamander with a maximum total length of about 128 mm. The second focal species is the northern spectacled salamander Salamandrina perspicillata, that belongs to family Salamandridae; this salamander is smaller, not exceeding 100 mm in total length. Strinati’s cave salamanders possess direct development and are fully terrestrial, while northern spectacled salamanders have a biphasic life cycle, lay eggs in the water, and become fully terrestrial after metamorphosis, with only reproductive females entering the water for egg deposition [39,40]. Adults of both species are lungless (S. strinatii) or have vestigial lungs (S. perspicillata), and gas exchange occurs mainly through their skin. Both species are usually found in the talus and the leaf litter of mixed broadleaved woodlands, and their surface activity is mainly regulated by the amount of rain or soil moisture [29,37,41]. In forest environments, the diet of S. strinatii and S. perspicillata has been extensively studied, by means of stomach flushing: both species feed on a large number of invertebrate taxa and share many trophic resources [29,30]. However, at the population level S. strinatii has been described as a generalist predator [29], while S. perspicillata is a specialized predator on springtails and mites [30].

2.3. Study Site

The study area is located in North-Western Italy, on the Apennine Mountain range of the Piemonte region, where the two studied species occurs sympatrically and are often found syntopically. The sampling area is located at an altitude of 600 m a.s.l. in the municipality of Mongiardino Ligure (44°38′24″ N; 9°03′00″ E), Alessandria province. The site is crossed by a first order Apennine stream and is characterized by a Supra–Mediterranean mixed deciduous forest, dominated by European Oak (Quercus pubescens Willd., 1805; [42]). We selected, individually marked, and GPS-positioned 26 square plots in the proximity of the stream banks, each one measuring 30 m2 (5.5 m side). The minimum distance among plots was about 20 m.

2.4. Salamanders’ Sampling

In spring 2018 (16 April–25 May), the same observer visited all sites three times during daytime (8–11 a.m.) and in favorable weather conditions for salamander’s activity (e.g., during or immediately after light rain). During each survey the observer searched for salamanders within each plot for four minutes [37,43], checking the leaf litter, inspecting rock crevices with a flashlight, and lifting temporary shelters such as superficial rocks and dead wood in search of salamanders. Since both species are known to be active during or immediately after rain and considering that inactive individuals retreat underground and are usually unavailable for sampling, we considered both individuals encountered on the forest floor and those found under temporary shelters as active on the surface [37,38,41]. We assigned each individual to one of the two study species and their abundance in each plot recorded.

2.5. Environmental Covariates

Within each marked plot at a depth of 20 cm, we obtained five measurements of soil relative humidity using a digital moisture meter (Extech MO750). All measurements were obtained on the same day (20 April 2018), 4 days following a 50 mm precipitation event. The average of these five measurements was considered as a plot-specific proxy, describing the soil moisture retention potential (MOIST). From a digital elevation model (DEM; 20 m mesh size) of the study area, we calculated two covariates: the duration of direct insolation (INSOL), expressed in hours, and the topographic position index (TPI). This latter index expresses the topographic position of each cell within the landscape, assuming positive values for cells located on ridges or hilltops and negative values for cells located in depressions [44]. Moreover, for each sampling session, we recorded the day of the year (DAY; i.e., the continuous count of the number of days beginning each year from 1 January). We obtained the temperature of the survey (TEMP) and the accumulated rain in the 72 h prior to sampling (RAIN) from local weather stations. We conducted terrain analyses with software SAGA 7 [45].

2.6. Data Analysis

We analyzed our repeated count data of S. strinatii and S. perspicillata using a co-abundance formulation of the static binomial N-mixture model of Royle [31], accounting for directional biotic interactions [32], both on the abundance of salamanders and on their activity pattern. Binomial N-mixture models estimate the latent abundance state (N) at site i (Ni), assuming Ni~Poisson(λ), where λ is the expected abundance over all sites, by using repeated counts C at site i during survey j (Cij) to estimate individual detection probability p, assuming Cij|Ni~Binomial(Ni,p). Both parameters can be modelled as a function of environmental covariates trough a log or logit link, respectively. In order to model the possible effect of exploitative competition on local abundance, we stacked two N-mixture models, using the latent abundance of the larger species (i.e., S. strinatii) as a covariate in the abundance model of S. perspicillata [34,35,36]. Therefore, we added a co-abundance interaction effect term (γ) to the model [33] to estimate the overall effect of the abundance of Strinati’s cave salamanders on the abundance of the northern spectacled salamander [32]. Likewise, to model the possible presence of interference competition resulting from reduced activity of the smaller species when the larger one is active, we considered the detection probability p of both species to be a rough proxy of species’ surface activity. This approach is widely shared and often successfully used in ecological studies assessing species interactions [37,38,46]. Therefore, we added another interaction term (ε) on the detection process, using the detection probability pij of S. strinatii as a covariate on the detection of S. perpicillata. Prior to building our model, we standardized covariates and checked them for collinearity, considering a cut-off for inclusion of Pearson r < 0.7 [47,48]. We modelled the detection process of two species as follows:
[1; S. strinatii] logit(pijSs) = α0 + α1*DAYij + α2*TEMPij + α3*RAINij + δij
[2; S. perspicillata] logit(pijSp) = α0 + α1*DAYij + α2*TEMPij + α3*RAINij + ε*pijSs + δij
where the subscripts Ss and Sp stand for S. strinatii and S. perspicillata, respectively, α0 is the intercept, α1–α3 are covariate effects, ε is the directional interaction term on the activity between the two species, and δ is a normally distributed random effect that accounts for possible over dispersion in the detection process [49]. Similarly, we modelled the abundances of the two species as:
[3; S. strinatii] log (NiSs) = β0 + β1*MOISTi + β2*INSOLi + β3*TPIi + ηi
[4; S. perspicillata] log (NiSp) = β0 + β1*MOISTi + β2*INSOLi + β3*TPIi + γ*NiSs + ηi
where the subscripts Ss and Sp stand for S. strinatii and S. perspicillata, respectively, β0 is the intercept, β1–β3 are covariate effects, Ni is the latent abundance of adults at site i, γ is the co-abundance effect of adults on juveniles, and η is a normally distributed site-level random effect [49]. We estimated model parameters using a Bayesian approach with Markov chain Monte–Carlo methods, using uninformative priors. We ran three chains, each one with an adaptive phase of 10,000 iterations, followed by 350,000 iterations, discarding the first 50,000 as a burn-in and thinning by 100. We considered that chains reached convergence when the Gelman–Rubin statistic (R-hat) was <1.1 [50]. We used the 90% highest density interval of the posterior distribution as a credible interval (CRI), and considered covariates on both abundance, detection, and biotic interaction to have a significant effect when CRI did not cross the zero. In order to assess model fit [51,52,53], we employed posterior predictive checks based on χ2 statistics as a measure of the discrepancy between observed and simulated data and calculated a Bayesian p-value accordingly [49]. Analyses were conducted calling program JAGS (V4.3.0; [54]) from the R environment with package “JagsUI” (V1.5.1; [55]).

3. Results

During our sampling, we counted 96 salamanders: 61 S. strinatii and 35 S. perspicillata. For both species, the co-abundance N-mixture model was in a good fit (posterior predictive checks: Bayesian p-value for S. strinatii = 0.46, and for S. perspicillata = 0.51). For all monitored parameters, convergence resulted successful (maximum R-hat = 1.059). The complete list of parameters’ estimates, along with their respective 90% CRI, for both species, is reported in Table 1.
The two species showed different detection probabilities, with S. strinatii, the larger species, being more active on the forest floor (p = 0.43; CRI = 0.21–0.66) when compared to S. perspicillata (p = 0.32; CRI = 0.08–0.62). None of the survey covariates had a clear effect on p (CRI crossed zero, in all cases), although there was an 89.7% probability that TEMP had a negative effect on the detection of S. perspicillata (Pd, posterior probability of direction; Table 1). The two species also showed different per-site abundances, with S. strinatii (λ = 0.70; CRI = 0.25–1.15) being less abundant than S. perspicillata (λ = 2.10; CRI = 0.48–4.02).
Overall, the two species showed differential responses to the same set of environmental predictors, both for the ecological process and their detection/activity. Indeed, the abundance of S. strinatii was positively affected by MOIST (Figure 1) and negatively affected by INSOL (Figure 2), while abundance of S. perspicillata was only negatively affected by INSOL (Figure 2).
Finally, for what concern the two interaction terms, the one on the surface activity of S. perspicillata was negative and significant (ε = −3.76; CRI = −7.66–−0.09), highlighting that the surface activity of S. strinatii has a strong negative effect on the activity of S. perspicillata (Figure 3). At the same time, the interaction term on local abundance was also negative (γ = −0.18), but its CRI largely crossed the zero (CRI = −0.56–0.20; pd = 79.9%; Figure 3).

4. Discussion

Identifying the ecological processes that impact the presence and population size of a species holds significant importance for ecologists and conservationists, since the maximum local abundance of a species is observed when environmental characteristics align with its ecological requirements. On the other hand, the increase in local abundance or density of a species due to improved resources or suitability of a particular area is constrained by biotic interactions with other organisms that partially overlap the same ecological niche. These interactions typically occur as density-dependent competitive interactions, which can balance and limit local abundance. These competitive interactions can arise within or between species, encompassing a range of niche axes and mechanisms. For instance, organisms engage in direct competition for mates and physical space, such as habitats or shelters, as well as indirect competition for food resources or other niches, such as call space. Irrespective of the mechanism or origin of these competitive interactions, they typically emerge when one species hinders its competitors’ access to a shared resource. This exclusion from resource utilization generally occurs through two distinct pathways: exploitative competition or interference competition. These two forms of competition differ significantly in their impact on a species’ access to a particular resource. Exploitative competition arises when certain organisms make more efficient use of the same limited resource, thus directly reducing its availability for other individuals. Conversely, interference competition takes place when one organism actively obstructs another organism’s access to a resource, regardless of its availability.
In this study, we found evidence that interference competition, rather than exploitative competition, is shaping the co-occurrence of Speleomantes strinatii (Strinati’s cave salamander) and S. perspicillata (northern spectacled salamander), after accounting for differential habitat requirements. That is, after modelling ecological requirements of the two species by means N-mixture models using appropriate covariates on both the abundance and detection processes, we still detected some significant interaction between the two species, which is attributable to a biological interaction [32,33,34,35,36]. Specifically, after accounting for environmental covariates, we found that the activity of S. strinatii negatively affected the surface activity of S. perspicillata, indicating that the observed difference in surface activity between the two species can be explained by temporal niche partitioning. This suggests that the two species avoided direct interactions on the forest floor by being active at different times. Conversely, we did not find any effect of Strinati’s cave salamander’s abundance on northern spectacled salamander’s abundance at the scale at which the study has been conducted. This indicates that resource depletion (i.e., exploitative competition) probably does not limit the local abundance of either species in the study area. The overall findings of this study align with previous research on interference competition, which has been observed to produce niche shift patterns consistent with temporal niche partitioning [17,27,28].
In contrast, our results did not provide support for the hypothesis of exploitative competition, which influences local abundance or spatial displacement of one or both competitors due to density-dependent effects on limited resources [21,25,26]. The lack of evidence for exploitative competition in this two-species system may imply that resource availability for these salamander species is not a major ecological limiting factor, or that they have developed mechanisms partitioning resources in a way that avoids direct competition [13]. Indeed, S. perspicillata and S. strinatii share some of the same trophic resources, but (i) display different trophic strategies [29,30], (ii) forage successfully in slightly different weather conditions [56], and (iii) when they both are active on the forest floor, S. perspicillata reduces the foraging intensity on the core prey items composing its diet, suggesting some kind of interference with S. strinatii [57].
Such niche differentiation likely relaxes competition for food, explaining why we did not detect exploitative effects. Temporal niche partitioning due to interference competition has been observed in several taxa, including amphibians [58], birds [59], and mammals [60]. For other forest salamanders, Jaeger and colleagues [58,61,62] found evidence of temporal niche partitioning at the intraspecific level in the eastern red-backed salamander (Plethodon cinereus), which was driven by interference competition for food resources and territories during the breeding season. However, interference competition has also been found to produce negative effects on the fitness and survival of competitors. Hairston et al. [63] found that interference competition between P. cinereus and P. richmondi resulted in reduced growth rates and survival of P. richmondi due to aggressive interactions with P. cinereus. Similarly, Petranka and Smith [64] found that interference competition between P. cinereus and P. glutinosus resulted in decreased survival and growth rates of both species.
Salamandrina perspicillata in the study site, despite being the outperformed species (i.e., its overall activity is reduced by the surface activity of S. strinatii), is roughly three times more abundant than S. strinatii. This is further evidence for the lack of a density-related exploitative competition on this two-species system, otherwise S. perspicillata abundance should be reduced. This also suggests the presence of behavioral strategies to minimize the overlap in their realized temporal niches. This may have been facilitated by their ability to successfully exploit similar prey resources at different times, thus minimizing the intensity of their competitive trophic interactions [2,24].

5. Conclusions

Our results support the hypothesis that interference competition promotes temporal niche partitioning in terrestrial salamanders. By shifting their surface activity, S. strinatii and S. perspicillata can co-occur at high densities while minimizing competitive interactions. On the other hand, trophic niche differentiation, divergences in metabolism, prey selection or behavioral traits, together with a high prey abundance may help to prevent the emergence of competition for food resources and may promote species coexistence [65]. Nevertheless, further studies in different ecological situations and adopting different sampling frameworks specifically designed to model surface activity should be conducted to help clarify the role of temporal or spatial niche partitioning in shaping the coexistence of these salamander species.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.C., G.R. and S.S.; methodology, A.C., G.R. and S.S; data analysis, A.C.; writing—original draft preparation, A.C. and S.S.; writing—review and editing, A.C., G.R. and S.S.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

A.C. is funded by the Italian National Operative Programme “Research and Innovation” (PON—Ricerca e Innovazione, tematica GREEN; CUP N. D31B21008270007); G.R. is fulfilling his PhD at the University of Genoa and S.S. is funded by the University of Genoa (FRA-2018).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Capture permits for the present study were issued by the Italian Ministry of Environment (Protocol N#8453/T-A31) and no animals were harmed during the fieldwork.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Fretwell, S.D.; Lucas, H.L. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheor. 1970, 19, 16–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Chesson, P. Mechanisms of maintenance of species diversity. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 2000, 31, 343–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Connell, J.H. On the prevalence and relative importance of interspecific competition: Evidence from field experiments. Am. Nat. 1983, 122, 661–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Houston, A.I.; McNamara, J.M. The ideal free distribution when competitive abilities differ: An approach based on statistical mechanics. Anim. Behav. 1988, 36, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Krebs, C.J. Ecology: The Experimental Analysis of Distribution and Abundance, 5th ed.; Benjamin Cummings: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  6. Périquet, S.; Fritz, H.; Revilla, E. The Lion King and the Hyaena Queen: Large carnivore interactions and coexistence. Biol. Rev. 2015, 90, 1197–1214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Grether, G.F.; Okamoto, K.W. Eco-evolutionary dynamics of interference competition. Ecol. Lett. 2022, 25, 2167–2176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Grether, G.F.; Peiman, K.S.; Tobias, A.; Robinson, B.W. Causes and consequences of behavioral interference between species. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2017, 32, 760–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Smallegange, I.M.; Van Der Meer, J.; Kurvers, R.H. Disentangling interference competition from exploitative competition in a crab–bivalve system using a novel experimental approach. Oikos 2006, 113, 157–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lang, B.; Rall, B.C.; Brose, U. Warming effects on consumption and intraspecific interference competition depend on predator metabolism. J. Anim. Ecol. 2012, 81, 516–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Daly, M. The cost of mating. Am. Nat. 1978, 112, 771–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Parker, G.A. Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating strategies. In Sperm Competition and the Evolution of Animal Mating Systems; Elsevier Academic Press: Cambdridge, MA, USA, 1984; pp. 1–60. [Google Scholar]
  13. Schoener, T.W. Resource Partitioning in Ecological Communities: Research on how similar species divide resources helps reveal the natural regulation of species diversity. Science 1974, 185, 27–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. MacArthur, R.; Levins, R. The limiting similarity, convergence, and divergence of coexisting species. Am. Nat. 1967, 101, 377–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Gerhardt, H.C.; Huber, F. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans: Common Problems and Diverse Solutions; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  16. Case, T.J.; Gilpin, M.E. Interference competition and niche theory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1974, 71, 3073–3077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Carothers, J.H.; Jaksić, F.M. Time as a niche difference: The role of interference competition. Oikos 1984, 42, 403–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hardin, G. The Competitive Exclusion Principle: An idea that took a century to be born has implications in ecology, economics, and genetics. Science 1960, 131, 1292–1297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Adler, F.R.; Mosquera, J. Is space necessary? Interference competition and limits to biodiversity. Ecology 2000, 81, 3226–3232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Amarasekare, P. Interference competition and species coexistence. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 2002, 269, 2541–2550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Petren, K.; Case, T.J. An experimental demonstration of exploitation competition in an ongoing invasion. Ecology 1996, 77, 118–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Schoener, T.W. Competition and the Niche. In Biology of the Reptilia. Ecology; Gans, A.C., Tinkle, D.W., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977; Volume 7, pp. 35–136. [Google Scholar]
  23. Schoener, T.W. Field experiments on interspecific competition. Am. Nat. 1983, 122, 240–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Albrecht, M.; Gotelli, N.J. Spatial and temporal niche partitioning in grassland ants. Oecologia 2001, 126, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Andrewartha, H.G.; Birch, L.C. The Distribution and Abundance of Animals; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1954. [Google Scholar]
  26. Sinclair, A.R.E. Population regulation in animals. In Ecological Concept; Cherret, J.M., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  27. Valeix, M.; Chamaillé-Jammes, S.; Fritz, H. Interference competition and temporal niche shifts: Elephants and herbivore communities at waterholes. Oecologia 2007, 153, 739–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Olea, P.P.; Iglesias, N.; Mateo-Tomás, P. Temporal resource partitioning mediates vertebrate coexistence at carcasses: The role of competitive and facilitative interactions. Basic Appl. Ecol. 2022, 60, 63–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Salvidio, S.; Romano, A.; Oneto, F.; Ottonello, D.; Michelon, R. Different season, different strategies: Feeding ecology of two syntopic forest-dwelling salamanders. Acta Oecol. 2012, 43, 42–50. [Google Scholar]
  30. Costa, A.; Salvidio, S.; Posillico, M.; Matteucci, G.; De Cinti, B.; Romano, A. Generalisation within specialization: Inter-individual diet variation in the only specialized salamander in the world. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 13260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Royle, J.A. N-mixture models for estimating Population size from spatially replicated counts. Biometrics 2004, 60, 108–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kéry, M.; Royle, J.A. Applied Hierarchical Modeling in Ecology: Analysis of Distribution, Abundance and Species Richness in R and BUGS: Volume 2: Dynamic and Advanced Models; Academic Press: London, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  33. Waddle, J.; Dorazio, R.M.; Walls, S.; Rice, K.; Beauchamp, J.; Schuman, M. A new parameterization for estimating co-occurrence of interacting species. Ecol. Appl. 2010, 20, 1467–1475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Clare, J.D.; Linden, D.W.; Anderson, E.M.; MacFarland, D.M. Do the antipredator strategies of shared prey mediate intraguild predation and mesopredator suppression? Ecol. Evol. 2016, 6, 3884–3897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Roth, T.; Allan, E.; Pearman, P.B.; Amrhein, V. Functional ecology and imperfect detection of species. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2018, 9, 917–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Brodie, J.F.; Helmy, O.E.; Mohd-Azlan, J.; Granados, A.; Bernard, H.; Giordano, A.J.; Zipkin, E. Models for assessing local-scale co-abundance of animal species while accounting for differential detectability and varied responses to the environment. Biotropica 2018, 50, 5–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Rosa, G.; Salvidio, S.; Costa, A. European plethodontid salamanders on the forest floor: Testing for age-class segregation and habitat selection. J. Herpetol. 2022, 56, 27–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Romano, A.; Rosa, G.; Salvidio, S.; Novaga, R.; Costa, A. How landscape and biotic interactions shape a Mediterranean reptile community. Landsc. Ecol. 2022, 37, 2915–2927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Angelini, C.; Vanni, S.; Vignoli, L. Salamandrina terdigitata (Bonnaterre, 1789) Salamandrina perspicillata (Savi, 1821). In Fauna d’Italia. 42. Amphibia; Lanza, B., Ed.; Edizioni Calderini: Bologna, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  40. Lanza, B. Speleomantes strinatii (Aellen, 1958). In Fauna d’Italia. 42. Amphibia; Lanza, B., Andreone, F., Bologna, M., Corti, C., Razzetti, E., Eds.; Edizioni Calderini: Bologna, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  41. Costa, A.; Crovetto, F.; Salvidio, S. European plethodontid salamanders on the forest floor: Local abundance is related to fine-scale environmental factors. Herpetol. Conserv. Biol. 2016, 11, 344–349. [Google Scholar]
  42. Blondel, J.; Aronson, J. Biology and Wildlife of the Mediterranean Region; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  43. Romano, A.; Costa, A.; Basile, M.; Raimondi, R.; Posillico, M.; Scinti Roger, D.; De Cinti, B. Conservation of salamanders in managed forests: Methods and costs of monitoring abundance and habitat selection. For. Ecol. Manag. 2017, 400, 12–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Guisan, A.; Weiss, S.B.; Weiss, A.D. GLM versus CCA spatial modelling of plant species distribution. Plant Ecol. 1999, 143, 107–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Conrad, O.; Bechtel, B.; Bock, M.; Dietrich, H.; Fischer, E.; Gerlitz, L.; Böhner, J. System for automated geoscientific analyses (SAGA) v. 2.1.4. Geosci. Model Dev. 2015, 8, 1991–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Amir, Z.; Sovie, A.; Luskin, M.S. Inferring predator–prey interactions from camera traps: A Bayesian co-abundance modeling approach. Ecol. Evol. 2022, 12, e9627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Mac Nally, R. Multiple regression and inference in ecology and conservation biology: Further comments on identifying important predictor variables. Biodiv. Conserv. 2002, 11, 1397–1401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Dormann, C.F.; Elith, J.; Bacher, S.; Buchmann, C.; Carl, G.; Carré, G.; García Marquéz, R. Collinearity: A review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography 2013, 36, 27–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kéry, M.; Schaub, M. Bayesian Population Analysis Using WinBUGS: A Hierarchical Perspective; Academic Press: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  50. Gelman, A.; Rubin, D.B. Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat. Sci. 1992, 7, 457–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Duarte, A.; Adams, M.J.; Peterson, J.T. Fitting N-mixture models to count data with unmodeled heterogeneity: Bias, diagnostics, and alternative approaches. Ecol. Model. 2018, 374, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Knape, J.; Arlt, D.; Barraquand, F.; Berg, Å.; Chevalier, M.; Pärt, T.; Żmihorski, M. Sensitivity of binomial N-mixture models to overdispersion: The importance of assessing model fit. Met. Ecol. Evol. 2018, 9, 2102–2114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Costa, A.; Salvidio, S.; Penner, J.; Basile, M. Time-for-space substitution in N-mixture models for estimating population trends: A simulation-based evaluation. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 4581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Plummer, M. JAGS: A program for analysis of Bayesian graphical models using Gibbs sampling. In Proceedings of the 3rd International Workshop on Distributed Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, 20–22 March 2003; Volume 124, pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kellner, K. jagsUI: A Wrapper around Rjags to Streamline JAGSanalyses. R Package Version 1.5.1. Available online: https://github.com/kenkellner/jagsUI (accessed on 1 March 2023).
  56. Rosa, G.; Bosio, M.; Salvidio, S.; Costa, A. Foraging success is differently affected by local climate in two syntopic forest-dwelling salamanders. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 2022, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Costa, A.; Rosa, G.; Salvidio, S. Size-Mediated Trophic Interactions in two Syntopic Forest Salamanders. Animals 2023, 13, 1281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Jaeger, R.G.; Gollmann, B.; Anthony, C.D.; Gabor, C.R.; Kohn, N.R. Behavioral Ecology of the Eastern Red-Backed Salamander: 50 Years of Research; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  59. Alatalo, R.V.; Gustafsson, L.; Linden, M.; Lundberg, A. Interspecific competition and niche shifts in tits and the goldcrest: An experiment. J. Anim. Ecol. 1985, 54, 977–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kronfeld-Schor, N.; Dayan, T. Partitioning of time as an ecological resource. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2003, 34, 153–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Jaeger, R.G.; Kalvarsky, D.; Shimizu, N. Territorial behaviour of the red-backed salamander: Expulsion of intruders. Anim. Behav. 1983, 31, 490–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Jaeger, R.G.; Wicknick, J.A.; Griffis, M.R.; Anthony, C.D. Socioecology of a terrestrial salamander: Juveniles enter adult territories during stressful foraging periods. Ecology 1995, 76, 533–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hairston, N.G.; Smith, F.E.; Slobodkin, L.B. Community structure, population control, and competition. Am. Nat. 1995, 146, 796–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Petranka, J.W.; Smith, C.K. Interspecific competition between two terrestrial salamanders: Effects on individual growth and survivorship. Oecologia 1987, 73, 507–511. [Google Scholar]
  65. Lunghi, E.; Corti, C.; Biaggini, M.; Zhao, Y.; Cianferroni, F. The trophic niche of two sympatric species of salamanders (Plethodontidae and Salamandridae) from Italy. Animals 2022, 12, 2221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Plots showing the effect of the soil moisture retention potential (MOIST) on salamander abundance: Speleomantes strinatii on the left and Salamandrina perspicillata on the right. Bold lines represent the posterior mean, while the thin lines represent 500 random draws from the posterior distribution.
Figure 1. Plots showing the effect of the soil moisture retention potential (MOIST) on salamander abundance: Speleomantes strinatii on the left and Salamandrina perspicillata on the right. Bold lines represent the posterior mean, while the thin lines represent 500 random draws from the posterior distribution.
Animals 13 02003 g001
Figure 2. Plots showing the effect of duration of direct insolation (INSOL) on salamander abundance: Speleomantes strinatii on the left and Salamandrina perspicillata on the right. Bold lines represent the posterior mean, while the thin lines represent 500 random draws from the posterior distribution.
Figure 2. Plots showing the effect of duration of direct insolation (INSOL) on salamander abundance: Speleomantes strinatii on the left and Salamandrina perspicillata on the right. Bold lines represent the posterior mean, while the thin lines represent 500 random draws from the posterior distribution.
Animals 13 02003 g002
Figure 3. Plots showing the effect of surface activity (left panel; interaction terms ε) and abundance of S. strinatii (right panel; interaction term γ) on the surface activity and abundance of S. perspicillata. Bold lines represent the posterior mean, while the thin lines represent 500 random draws from the posterior distribution.
Figure 3. Plots showing the effect of surface activity (left panel; interaction terms ε) and abundance of S. strinatii (right panel; interaction term γ) on the surface activity and abundance of S. perspicillata. Bold lines represent the posterior mean, while the thin lines represent 500 random draws from the posterior distribution.
Animals 13 02003 g003
Table 1. Complete list of the parameters estimated for the detection process, abundance, and interactions for both S. strinatii and S. perspicillata. Mean = mean obtained from the posterior distribution; 90% CRI = 90% highest density region of the posterior distribution; pd = probability of direction.
Table 1. Complete list of the parameters estimated for the detection process, abundance, and interactions for both S. strinatii and S. perspicillata. Mean = mean obtained from the posterior distribution; 90% CRI = 90% highest density region of the posterior distribution; pd = probability of direction.
Speleomantes strinatiiSalamandrina perspicillata
ParameterMean90% CRIpdParameterMean90% CRIpd
Detection Detection
Mean p0.430.21–0.66-Mean p0.320.08–0.62
DAY0.04−3.9–−4.350.7DAY−0.43−4.85–3.6157.0
TEMP1.32−1.7–4.476.6TEMP−2.59−5.88–0.9789.7
RAIN1.33−1.56–4.3277.0RAIN−1.81−5.2–1.3782.0
Abundance Abundance
Mean λ0.700.25–1.15-Mean λ2.100.48–4.02-
MOIST0.880.25–1.5798.7MOIST−0.48−1.30–0.2985.0
INSOL−0.76−1.31–−0.2099.2INSOL−1.41−2.34–−0.3799.5
TPI−1.11−0.48–0.2470.0TPI−0.33−0.99–0.3679.6
Interactions
ε−3.76−7.66–−0.0994.8
γ−0.18−0.56–0.2079.9
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Rosa, G.; Salvidio, S.; Costa, A. Disentangling Exploitative and Interference Competition on Forest Dwelling Salamanders. Animals 2023, 13, 2003. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13122003

AMA Style

Rosa G, Salvidio S, Costa A. Disentangling Exploitative and Interference Competition on Forest Dwelling Salamanders. Animals. 2023; 13(12):2003. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13122003

Chicago/Turabian Style

Rosa, Giacomo, Sebastiano Salvidio, and Andrea Costa. 2023. "Disentangling Exploitative and Interference Competition on Forest Dwelling Salamanders" Animals 13, no. 12: 2003. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13122003

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop