Next Article in Journal
Animal Welfare Considerations and Ethical Dilemmas Inherent in the Euthanasia of Blind Canine Patients
Next Article in Special Issue
Multiobject Tracking of Wildlife in Videos Using Few-Shot Learning
Previous Article in Journal
Molecular Survey of Dirofilaria and Leishmania Species in Dogs from Central Balkan
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Use of Selfie Camera Traps to Estimate Home Range and Movement Patterns of Small Mammals in a Fragmented Landscape

Centre for Sustainable Ecosystem Solutions, School of Earth, Atmospheric and Life Sciences, Faculty of Science, Medicine and Health, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, NSW 2522, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2022, 12(7), 912; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12070912
Submission received: 8 March 2022 / Revised: 22 March 2022 / Accepted: 25 March 2022 / Published: 2 April 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Use of Camera Trap for a Better Wildlife Monitoring and Conservation)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Camera trapping allows scientists to study a range of species, across large areas for long periods of time, with little impact on animals. It has been readily used to study movements and territories of large mammals but not for smaller species. This is because they often appear too small on camera to be able to identify distinct individuals. The selfie trap is a method that allows for up close and detailed images of small mammals, and our study aimed to test the ability of the method to track the movement and social behaviour of sugar gliders. We found that the selfie trap is an efficient camera trapping method for estimating home ranges and movements due to its ability to obtain high recapture rates for multiple small mammal species and individuals. In our study landscape, linear strips of habitat were readily utilised by all small mammals, highlighting their importance as wildlife corridors in a fragmented landscape.

Abstract

The use of camera traps to track individual mammals to estimate home range and movement patterns, has not been previously applied to small mammal species. Our aim was to evaluate the use of camera trapping, using the selfie trap method, to record movements of small mammals within and between fragments of habitat. In a fragmented landscape, 164 cameras were set up across four survey areas, with cameras left to record continuously for 28 nights. Live trapping was performed prior to ear mark animals to facilitate individual identification on camera. Four small mammal species (sugar glider; Petaurus breviceps; brown antechinus; Antechinus stuartii, bush rat; Rattus fuscipes, and brown rat; Rattus norvigecus) were recorded on camera (N = 284 individuals). The maximum distance travelled by an individual sugar glider was 14.66 km, antechinus 4.24 km; bush rat 1.90 km and brown rat 1.28 km. Movements of both female and male sugar gliders in linear fragments were recorded at much higher rates than in larger patches of forest sampled in grids. Short term core homes ranges (50% KDE) of 34 sugar gliders ranged from 0.3 ha to 4.2 ha. Sugar glider core home ranges were on average 1.2 ha (±0.17) for females and 2.4 ha (±0.28) for males. The selfie trap is an efficient camera trapping method for estimating home ranges and movements due to its ability to obtain high recapture rates for multiple species and individuals. In our study landscape, linear strips of habitat were readily utilised by all small mammals, highlighting their importance as wildlife corridors in a fragmented landscape.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

The study of animal movements allows ecologists to understand species’ behaviours related to: habitat selection, territoriality, foraging, mating, and dispersal, as well as investigate responses to anthropogenic changes in the environment [1,2,3]. Barriers to animal movement such as habitat fragmentation and roads, have received significant attention because of the conservation concerns around population viability from restricted movement across a landscape [4,5,6,7].
Most noticeable is the impact of fragmentation on arboreal species as they rely on continuous canopy, otherwise populations can decline due to the isolation of available habitat patches [8,9,10,11]. Fragmentation can result in changes in home range behaviour, such as where individual animals will have smaller home ranges and restrict their movements [12]. When in fragmented landscapes, Langurs (Semnopithecus spp.), a mostly arboreal primate, were found to adopt an energy conservation strategy were individuals spent more time feeding than moving, resulting in a smaller home range and diets shifting to consume different plant species [12]. However, other species may not adopt similar energy conservation strategies due to the disjointed distribution of resources within the fragmented landscape. Male brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) were found to travel further than females in linear strips of roadside habitat [13]. It was found that both sexes had greater levels of activity in roadside habitat compared to other forest fragments, and this indicates that linear shaped strips of habitat could be considered as energetically challenging as ranging behaviours must increase to access resources [13]. Therefore, the movement behaviour of arboreal mammals within a fragmented landscape can change adaptively [12] or remain energetically costly as it will depend on species resource requirements (e.g. males seeking mates or dispersing) and the spatial availability of these resources within the landscape [13]. In the case of arboreal marsupials, recent observations of species normally perceived as strictly arboreal, have been documented moving on the ground [14,15]. Whether these observations reflect the potential for sensitive arboreal species to disperse through the matrix (rather than be solely reliant on tree cover), is yet to be determined.
To study movements and home ranges a variety of methods to track individual animals are available to ecologists. Many home range studies utilise tracking collars [16,17,18] or directly resight tagged individuals over a study period [19,20,21]. However, these methods can be costly, difficult or have impacts on animal welfare. Since the advent of camera trapping methods, that allow for highly effective and efficient surveys of wildlife, the method has also had successful application in estimating home ranges of many mammal species [22,23,24,25,26]. However, to date, all home range studies using motion sensor cameras, have been focused on large charismatic mammals, particularly felines with unique fur patterns [27,28,29].
The use of camera traps to estimate home range and track movements of small mammal species has not been previously undertaken. In the context of small mammals, cameras have limited focus ranges resulting in low resolution images that can then lead to issues in correct species identification. Some researchers have modified camera placement, for example mounting cameras to face down, parallel with the ground, to obtain images in higher clarity [30,31,32] or placed in protective housing for intertidal areas [33]. More recently, purpose-designed camera traps have been made to obtain facial images of small mammal species, a method aptly named the selfie trap [34]. Through several modifications the selfie trap is able to record small mammals in high resolution for accurate species and individual identification [34]. Many small mammal movement studies use intensive repeated live trapping surveys for mark-recapture (e.g. 12,171 live trapping nights [35]) and radio-tracking very small animals is highly difficult due to size limitations and battery life [36,37,38]. Thus, replacing live trapping mark-recapture with camera trapping would be more efficient, as recapture rates are higher for camera traps as they are considered an ‘open’ trap [39]. One study compared the use of live trapping grids to estimate sugar glider (Petaurus breviceps) home range, and when calibrated with radio-telemetry data, found the use of recaptures through live trapping to be a valid method for home range estimation [40]. As live trapping is an intensive process, the use of camera trapping for small mammals would be more effective for estimating movements and home ranges.
The focus of our study was to understand if selfie traps, a modified camera trapping method that targets small mammals, could be used to track their movements, and calculate home range. The target species of this study was the sugar glider (P. breviceps), with opportunistic data collected on other small mammals (brown antechinus; Antechinus stuartii, bush rat; Rattus fuscipes, and brown rat; Rattus norvigecus). We aimed to compare short term movements of small mammals between linear patches of habitat compared with larger patches of habitat. We aimed to estimate short term core home ranges of sugar gliders and make observations on their spatial and social organisation in a fragmented landscape.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Area

This study was part of a broader project investigating arboreal marsupial abundance and distribution within a fragmented landscape around the township of Berry, located approximately 140 km south of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia (Figure 1). The study design focused on four major areas of interest as part of research into the Berry wildlife corridor [41,42]. Vegetation towards the coast (16 m a.s.l.) was a combination of littoral rainforest, and blackbutt-dominated (Eucalyptus pilularis) forest [14]. East of the coast, majority of the fragments of available habitat was either blackbutt-dominated or turpentine-dominated (Syncarpia glomulifera) forests. The creek lines in the study area mostly contained Casuarina sp. Sites furthest to the north on the Illawarra Escarpment (410 m a.s.l.), contained sub-tropical rainforest. The fragments south of the escarpment were surrounded by agricultural land, majority of which were pastures of grass for cattle and other livestock. At the nearest weather station, Kiama Bombo Headland, annual rainfall was 646.0 mm and mean monthly temperatures ranged between 25.4 °C (February) and 10.1 °C (July) [43].

2.2. Live Trapping

The study area was divided into four survey areas that targeted key areas of the Berry corridor with 164 trap sites in total (Figure 1). Each area survey was performed for four weeks (28 camera trap nights), with area one live trapping starting on the 13 August 2019, before sequentially moving onto the next area. Each area was surveyed in distinct linear transects, or grids, which was dependent on the type of fragment of habitat being surveyed (Figure 1). The focus species of the study was the sugar glider and traps were set 100 m apart to study sugar glider ecology [40]. Traps were spaced 100 m apart in grids [40], and an average of 250m along linear transects (dependent on private property limitations). This spacing of traps did not appropriately target the study of movements of other species, such as brown antechinus and bush rats, thus the data collected on these species is opportunistic (for example a spacing of 20 m between traps is more appropriate for antechinus species [35]).
Elliott A traps [44] were placed up trees, secured onto platforms, at heights of 2 m above ground to target arboreal sugar gliders. Trap entrances faced the tree and peanut butter, honey and oats were used as bait [45,46]. A honey water mixture was sprayed up and down the tree that the trap was secured to and surrounding foliage [47]. Traps were checked for seven mornings before they were packed down. Animals caught had a 2 mm clipping taken on the ear margin for genetic sampling [48,49]. Each individual animal was given a unique code based on the position of the clipping (Figure 2). This allowed for recaptures of individuals to be recorded through both live trapping, and on camera footage.

2.3. Camera Trapping

Following the last morning of live trapping at each area, a selfie trap (Figure 3) was placed on the same platform as the Elliott trap and left to record for four weeks (N = 164 selfie traps). The bait holders inside the selfie traps limited direct access for small mammals. The bait used was a mixture of peanut butter, honey, and oats. The bait was positioned within the modified focal range of a browning BTC-7-4K camera [34]. Selfie traps were rebaited and resprayed with honey water halfway into the 28-night survey sessions.
Camera trap footage collected was sorted for each site to species, and individual where possible. Individuals were identified from either: (1) ear notches made when animals were caught from live trapping; (2) unique distinguishable natural scars on ears; or (3) a combination of facial markings, tail tip colour (in the case of sugar gliders) or other unique feature (e.g., presence of tick, or tail injury). Profiles were created for each individual animal to aid in identification, and where a video could not be confidently assigned to an individual it was assigned as an ‘unknown individual’ and not used.
The sex of each individual was recorded where possible. For sugar gliders, males were readily identified by the presence of a bald patch on their foreheads, as well as scent glands on their chest. Their testicles were also often easily observed on camera as well. Female gliders were identified only if the presence of a pouch was observed, as the absence of a bald patch could indicate a sub-adult male. Male antechinus were identified by the presence of testicles and female antechinus by the presence of a pouch or pouch young. Rattus species were identified as male if testicles were observed, and females were identified by a clear lack of testicles as adults.

2.4. Analysis

Recapture rates were firstly calculated at a nightly interval (i.e., independence criterion of 24 hrs between an observation at a site). Recapture rates were summarised per species, per area, and observations included sightings of individuals across multiple sites in a single night. However, the final dataset used observations separated by ten minutes (lowest common interval across all areas), as some individuals were observed moving between adjacent cameras as quickly as four minutes apart. From this final dataset, total movements of individual small mammals were calculated using the Tracking Analyst tool in ArcGIS 10.7.1 (Esri, Redlands, CA, USA) [50].
Minimum convex polygons (MCP) are a simple and frequently used method for estimating home range size however it is highly sensitive to sample size, outliers and assumes an even distribution, and can often lead to over-estimation of home range [51]. In this study, most individual animals were only recorded at less than three locations. Therefore, MCPs were performed and masked by available habitat to visualise areas of habitat being used by individuals but were not used in formal analyses. Masking was performed by using the Extract by Mask tool in ArcGIS [50] to highlight the useable habitat occurring within each MCP and remove unusable habitat (cleared areas like pastures or built environments) from the MCP.
Due to the non-normal nature of the recapture data, non-parametric Mann-Whitney U-test were used to compare distances moved by male and female antechinus and sugar gliders, in linear strips and in fragments of habitat.
For the target species, kernel density estimates (KDE) of 95% and 50% home ranges, were performed for sugar gliders that were observed at more than four sites (n = 34), using the R package ‘LatticeDensity’ [52] in R Studio (RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA, USA) [53]. ‘LatticeDensity’ was used to estimate KDE as it allows for home range analyses to account for irregular boundaries of habitat, and gaps in habitat, which is a critical consideration for the study landscape where the available habitat is fragmented at fine scales. As recapture rates (per 24 h) ranged between 6 and 25 per individual, to determine the impact of sample sizes on KDE home ranges, regression analysis were performed using JMP Pro 15 (SAS Institute Inc.: Cary, NC, USA) [54]. As there was no significant relationship between recapture rates and all three home range estimates (95% KDE, 50% KDE and MCP), then all 34 individual sugar gliders were included in subsequent KDE home range analyses. Due the shorter time frames of the surveys conducted, only core home ranges of 50% were used to compare differences between sex and fragmentation classes. Differences in core home range sizes between male and female gliders, and gliders within larger fragments and linear fragments, were compared using t-tests (assuming unequal variances) on log-transformed data (where tests for normality were not met).

3. Results

3.1. Recaptures

Across all four areas, selfie traps recorded 28,566 videos of animals, across 4592 camera trap nights. Of the videos, 53.3% of videos were assigned to an individual and the remainder as unknown individuals. A total of 284 small mammals were individually identified. Approximately, 56% of these individuals had movements recorded between sites (i.e., observed at two or more sites; see Table S1 in Supplementary Materials). As selfie traps were placed on planks positioned up trees, the species that was most recorded on camera was the arboreal sugar glider and semi-arboreal brown antechinus (Table 1 and Table S1). Across all 284 small mammal individuals, 92% were able to be sexed (females: 52%, males: 40%).
Capture success was high for sugar gliders and antechinus in most areas surveyed (Table 1). Area four had the least recapture rate for gliders, and area two had the lowest rate of recaptures for antechinus. Both Rattus species had low capture success across all areas (Table 1).
At the individual level, female sugar gliders were captured on camera on an average of 11.2 (±0.6) nights (maximum of 20) and male sugar gliders were captured 9.0 (±0.6) times (maximum of 23). In comparison, the highest recapture rate for an individual glider during one week of live trapping in this study was three. Female antechinus were caught on cameras an average of 14.9 (±1.5) times (maximum of 26), and male antechinus were captured at an average of 6.4 (±1.2) times (maximum of 20). The highest live capture rate for an individual antechinus was four. Only a small number of individual rats were recaptured on camera, and these recaptures often occurred only at one site (Table 2 and Table S1).

3.2. Movement: Straight Line Distances

Within a four-week survey period, the maximum distance travelled by an individual glider as detected by camera traps, was 14,661 m (Table 3). The maximum distance observed for an individual brown antechinus was 4237 metres (Table 3). Distances moved by sugar gliders within a month ranged between 0 to 14,661 m, with averages between 1013 m for females and 1167 m for males (Table 3). Distances moved by antechinus ranged from 0 m to 4237 m, with averages between 243 m for females and 861 m for males (Table 3). Most movements recorded were within each sampling unit, either within a grid or within a linear transect (Figure 4 and Figure 5). For both rat species, there were insufficient observations made due to the spacing and positioning of the selfie traps up trees. However, one male bush rat travelled 1902 m, and a male brown rat was observed travelling 1283 m (Table 3).
Within a single night, the most distance moved for a female sugar glider was 1411 m and for a male glider, it was 1760 m. One female antechinus was observed moving 880 m in one night, and a male antechinus moved 1080 m.

3.3. Movements within and between Fragments of Habitat

Across 121 sugar gliders, the amount of useable habitat within MCPs for an individual ranged from 3.1 ha to 5.2 ha (Figure 4). Gliders were detected in all sampled fragments and areas, except for one isolated patch surrounded by cleared land (see north-east of Coomonderry Swamp; Figure 4). Across 20 antechinus, the amount of useable habitat within a MCP for an individual ranged from 1.3 ha to 3.1 ha (Figure 5). Antechinus were detected in all areas, however the wide spacing of trap sites meant that most movement observations were restricted to less than two sites and only 29% (21/72 individuals) were recorded at two or more sites (Table S1).
Nine between fragment movements (movements made between distinct grids and linear transects, or between large gaps in forest cover) were recorded for three individuals. Though actual movement pathways remain unknown, it is likely that animals would have to have travelled through cleared areas as there was little or no connectivity between patches (Figure 4 and Figure 5). Two sugar gliders were recorded moving between large gaps in canopy, one crossed this gap (43m) on six occasions, and the other crossed another gap (39m) once (Figure 4). One antechinus was recorded moving between the swamp edge and an isolated habitat patch, across cleared empty paddocks, with straight line distances of 1359m (Figure 5).
For female sugar gliders, mean distances moved between recaptures in patches and linear strips, were not significantly different (Mann Whitney U = 1062.50, P = 0.304; Figure 6a). For male sugar gliders, mean distances moved were significantly greatest in linear strips compared to forest patches (U = 952.00, P < 0.05; Figure 6a).
Distances moved by female antechinus in forest patches and linear strips, were not significantly different (U = 289.00, P = 0.87; Figure 6b). For male antechinus, mean distances moved were greatest in linear strips compared to forest patches, though differences were not significant (U = 59.00, P = 0.10; Figure 6b).

3.4. Home ranges & Social Organisation of Sugar Gliders

Short term core (50% KDE) homes ranges of 34 sugar gliders (detected at four or more sites) ranged from 0.4 ha to 4.3 ha (see Table S2 in Supplementary Materials for all estimates). Sugar glider core home ranges were on average 1.2 ha (±0.2) for all females and 2.4 ha (±0.3) for all males. In fragments of habitat measured in grids, core home ranges were 1.1 ha females and 1.4 ha for males (Table 4). In linear fragments of habitat, core home ranges 1.2 ha for females and 2.7 ha for males (Table 4). The size of core home ranges did not significantly differ between female and male sugar gliders (t = 1.37, df = 28.42, P = 0.09), nor between gliders within larger fragments sampled as grids, and linear forms of habitat (t = 1.27, df = 23.96, P = 0.21).
Of the core home ranges calculated, there were 21 instances of home range overlap. Of these overlapping home ranges, 67% were between males and females, 19% occurring between females, and 14% between males. The average percentage overlap for sugar gliders of male-female was 14% (±4%), male-male 20% (±9%) and for female-female 20% (±8%). The maximum overlap between two females was 38% (1.7 ha); two males was 25% (0.5 ha), and between a female and a male was 55% (0.5 ha).
There were 313 events where two or more gliders were observed in the selfie trap together, where all individuals were identifiable in the video. Of these co-occurring individuals, 50% (155) of observations were two females, 42% (133) were a female and male pair, 6% (20) were two females and one male, and the remaining 2% (5) were a pair of males.

4. Discussion

4.1. Recapture Rates

The rates of recapture for individual sugar gliders obtained in this study can be considered high. Studies capturing sugar gliders using live traps have had varied success rates, ranging from 5.1% [55], 8.2% [56], 9.7% [47] and 27.7% [45]. Through camera trapping our study achieved high rates of capture (up to 70%) as selfie traps act as an ‘open’ trap which allows multiple individuals of different species to be detected each night, across sites [39]. In addition to this, the use of bait attracts animals to return, however the bait was enclosed with limited access to ensure continued visitation during the four-week surveys. Though bait impacts on animal behaviour, an individual must first be ranging in the area to locate the selfie trap. In our study, when baits began to run low, many visits were quick. Gliders spent only a few seconds inside the selfie trap to smell the bait holder, before immediately leaving (there was an average decline of 30.5% in the number of videos recorded in the last three days of a session, when compared to the first three days since baited). Thus, for long term home range studies using selfie traps we recommend that baiting occurs irregularly and with randomisation using olfactory lures (e.g., cotton wool soaked with vanilla essence [42]). This is so that animals do not learn to respond to a consistently scheduled bait source but rather that selfie traps detect their within-area movements.
The recapture rates for individual antechinus can also be considered high, when compared to published rates of recapture for similar species. For example, one study obtained an average recapture rate of 4.3 individual brown antechinus (males and females) across 16,630 live trap nights [57]. Comparatively, our study was able to obtain high rates of recaptures per unit effort when using selfie traps (average nightly recapture rates for antechinus was 11.1, across 28 trap nights). Thus, the selfie trap method is useful for mark-resight methods for small mammals due to the high rates of recapture that can be obtained.

4.2. Movement: Straight Line Distances

The maximum distances moved as observed on selfie traps, were high for both sugar gliders and antechinus. Few studies report total or nightly movements made by individual gliders, and this is likely due to observations being made via radio-tracking and not through GPS-collars. The nightly movements as observed through selfie traps, made by both antechinus and sugar gliders, indicates high mobility and an ability to use linear forms of habitat despite significant edge effects. For Antechinus, the most amount of movement we observed for an individual for one night was 880 m for a female, and 1080 m for a male. Comparatively, a live trapping study by Marchesan and Carthew (2004) detected one male traversing 540 m in a single night (Antechinus flavipes).
For sugar gliders, the most distance we observed in a single night was 1411 m (female) and 1760 m (male). Comparatively in one other study, a male sugar glider was reported to move 360 m in one night [58]. Other congenera species have detected large nightly movements of 1561 m in male Petaurus norflocensis [59], and 3430 m of movement in male Petaurus gracilis [60]. For female P. norflocensis 1909 m of movement in a single night has been previously recorded [59]. Our study was able to observe large movements of gliders, both at the nightly scale and across the whole study duration. This indicates that the selfie trap method could be readily used in movement ecology study for a range of glider species, such as to look at the impacts of various disturbances (e.g., roads, agriculture, urbanisation) on glider movements.

4.3. Movements within and between Fragments of Habitat

Our study identified native small mammal persistence in very small patches of habitat and along linear roadsides, surrounded by highly disturbed agricultural lands. Though most movement observations were detected within fragments, sugar gliders were detected travelling across canopy gaps (presumably by gliding). Sugar gliders can persist in highly fragmented landscapes and move through the matrix where structures are available as steppingstones to permit gliding [58,61,62]. In comparison, the ability of one antechinus to move through a large open paddock indicates the species can traverse through the matrix. Similar observations were made for A. flavipes where 17 (4% of all movements) between fragment movements were recorded [63]. Our findings contribute to the idea that both brown antechinus and sugar gliders have some matrix tolerance for dispersal, which is likely an important behavioural advantage to their persistence in fragmented landscapes [9,58,64,65,66].
In another study, foraging distances of the mahogany glider per night ranged from 590–3430 m [60]. Foraging distances varied throughout the year and this was related to the flowering index in the study landscape, indicating changes in behaviour in response to dynamic food resources in the area [60]. In our fragmented areas, nearly all habitat was predominantly Eucalyptus forest, though creek lines and swamps were dominated by Casuarina glauca. Interestingly all gliders in our study that were detected in Coomonderry swamp, were not detected in the nearby forest. Gliders when released were observed gliding west, away from the closest Eucalyptus forest. Furthermore, on one occasion a glider when released from live trapping was observed gliding onto a dead C. glauca and entering a hollow. These observations could indicate glider residency within a swamp. The canopy edge was incredibly narrow (five metres wide), dominated by Casuarina trees, with only a few Banksia serrata present (not in flower during the survey period) and most available vegetation was at a height of less than two metres (thickets of Melaleuca scrub). The residency of sugar gliders within the swamp may be explained by possible access to other areas, perhaps at different times of year to access associated changes in flowering plants. However, our live trapping and camera trapping does data suggests significant use of a swamp by sugar gliders. Other studies have documented the use of low strata vegetation by sugar gliders, this being in banksia heaths, with patches of woodland nearby [67,68].
The use of space and home range will vary between individuals of species, because of the distribution of resources (food, shelter, mates) within an area [69,70,71]. Where habitat patch size is small, behavioural adaptions to obtain resources can occur, such as brushtail possums will range further in linear strips of habitat [13]. In our study, within larger fragments of habitat, less movements were made by individual antechinus and sugar gliders, compared to conspecifics found within linear forms of habitat (creek lines and roadsides). However, sample sizes were small and variability high, thus differences were not significant between females in patches of habitat and linear habitats. Male movements were higher in linear habitat forms, and this could be partly attributed to the dispersal of male antechinus and sugar gliders during the breeding season as they search for mates. Other studies identified changes in home range size and shape due to linearity of habitat available [72,73,74]. One resource such as hollows (den sites) may be a limiting factor in the study landscape. As part of a separate study, surveys into hollow availability were conducted in various locations in this study’s landscape. The surveys found the availability of hollows were limited in various fragments and linear strips of habitat.
Overall, the limitation of the movement data from this study was that survey periods were relatively short, and that whole patches of habitat could not always be surveyed entirely (limited by access to private property), thus many gliders that were observed at one or two sites may only reflect the edge of their home range.

4.4. Home Ranges & Social Organisation of Sugar Gliders

Core home ranges of sugar gliders in our fragmented landscape were similar to that of other studies, despite potential methodological differences [40,56,58]. Our study found core home ranges (50% KDE) ranged from 0.35 ha to 4.26 ha, with an average of 1.55 ha (across both females and males). In a continuous forest, Quin [56] found sugar glider short-term home ranges ranged from 0.79 to 1.75 ha, with an average of 1.27 ha (added squares method), with no differences found between seasons and sex. In another continuous forest, sugar glider home ranges were on average 5–6 ha (radio tracking) [40]. However in a fragmented system of forest, home ranges were smaller with an average of 0.6 ha (manually mapped) [58]. Compared to these studies, our home range estimates may reflect an overall response to the level of fragmentation within the study landscape. Species can either restrict their home ranges, or be forced to range further, depending on the distribution of resources in a fragmented or disturbed system [75]. At a fine scale, our study found no differences in core home range sizes between fragment type. However, long-term study would be needed to directly compare large continuous forests (i.e., Illawarra Escarpment to the North and Seven Mile Beach National Park to the South), with the fragmented areas located centrally in the study area, to determine if there is a fragmentation effect on home range size.
In our study, 95% KDE and MCP home ranges, reached estimates of 16.44 ha and 5.2 ha respectively. Though these are overestimates of home range, it allows us to identify that some individuals were exhibiting large ranging behaviours (see Figure 4). These observations could be explained by male gliders dispersing [9,58,76] or ranging behaviour in response to food resources changing with season and time (e.g., Savannah glider, Petaurus ariel; [60,69]). Studies into gliding species have found that home ranges increase in size in fragmented or urban areas, which has been attributed to the stretched and disjointed distribution of resources in the landscape [77,78]. In our study, some of the wide-ranging movements observed may be further explained by the limited amount of habitat and its connectivity. For the savannah glider, it was found that when resources were limiting, home-range size can far exceed what is predicted by body mass and diet [69]. However, Jackson [60] found that the mahogany glider (Petaurus gracilis) had smaller home ranges in fragmented habitat (6–11 ha) than in continuous habitat (19–20 ha), and the reasons for this were unclear though the authors suggested that home ranges in fragmented landscapes are unstable.
The overlap in core home ranges in our study are reflective of the species social organisation, where groups are made of one or two codominant males and the remaining family group consists of females [56,58]. Family groups have one or two dominant males that passively mark territory with their scent glands [79], and will also scent mark individuals in their group [80]. Groups are territorial and do not socialise [80], thus the number of different individuals overlapping at sites is likely to indicate the different groups occurring at that site. In our study gliders were recorded on camera in pairs, and on multiple occasions groups of three and four gliders were seen on camera. The proportions of co-occurring individuals on camera confirm observations as seen by other authors, where female-female and female-male co-occurrence are more common than male-male co-occurrence [58,76]. We observed on several occasions male-male glider antagonistic behaviour, where a male approaching another male glider already inside the selfie trap would be met with aggressive snarls and grunts, and in some cases, chased away. Scent marking behaviour was also observed where male gliders rubbed their chests along the entrance of selfie traps. Gliders could be observed often sniffing the air upon approaching the selfie trap, and on some occasions, this was followed by males immediately leaving on their first visit.
Sugar gliders form colonies that can range up to 12 individuals [58]. In our study, there was one occurrence where 12 individuals were detected at a single selfie trap, indicating the potential presence of a large family group in our study landscape, occurring in a linear strip of habitat. However, there is the possibility that some of these individuals may have been transient or at the edge of their home range given that four of the males were only observed on one to three independent nights.

5. Conclusions

The selfie trap is proven to be an effective camera trapping method for detecting movements and behaviour of small mammals. Due to the high rates of recapture the method can be readily applied to long-term studies that estimate home ranges. We found that linear forms of habitat and larger fragments throughout the study landscape provided both habitat and pathways for movement. These linear strips along roadsides and creek lines provide some of the last remaining habitat corridors for both arboreal and ground dwelling small mammals. Efforts must be made to conserve and rehabilitate these areas through additional plantings (especially feed trees) and tree hollow conservation and supplementation (nest box or artificial cavity installation).

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani12070912/s1, Table S1: Number of individual small mammals detected at various spatial samples on selfie traps positioned in the four areas within the study landscape; Table S2: Short term home ranges (50% and 95%) using Kernel Density Estimate (KDE), of sugar gliders (Petaurus breviceps) detected at four or more sites (n = 34). Female n = 15, male n = 19. Home ranges estimates from Minimum Convex Polygons (MCP) was calculated by calculating the amount of available habitat within the polygon.

Author Contributions

A.G. and K.M.M. conceived the ideas and designed methodology; A.G. collected the data; A.G. analysed the data; A.G. and K.M.M. led the writing of the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by a SMAH Partnership grant with National Parks Association and Berry Wildlife Corridor (NSW Environment Trust).

Institutional Review Board Statement

This work was conducted under NSW DPIE Scientific Licence 101968 and UOW Animal Ethics protocol AE1902.

Informed Consent Statement

No applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the Dharawal people, the traditional custodians of the land, and pay our respects the elders past, present and emerging. A massive thank you to all the volunteers who assisted with fieldwork, and to the private landholders for allowing us to do research on their properties. Special thanks to Monica Knipler for assisting with the many rounds of live trapping.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Demšar, U.; Buchin, K.; Cagnacci, F.; Safi, K.; Speckmann, B.; Van de Weghe, N.; Weiskopf, D.; Weibel, R. Analysis and visualisation of movement: An interdisciplinary review. Mov. Ecol. 2015, 3, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Nathan, R.; Giuggioli, L. A milestone for movement ecology research. Mov. Ecol. 2013, 1, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Fraser, K.C.; Davies, K.T.A.; Davy, C.M.; Ford, A.T.; Flockhart, D.T.T.; Martins, E.G. Tracking the Conservation Promise of Movement Ecology. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2018, 6, 150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. McCall, S.C.; Cesarini, S.; van der Ree, R.; Soanes, K.; Harper, M.J.; McCarthy, M.A. Evidence that a Highway Reduces Apparent Survival Rates of Squirrel Gliders. Ecol. Soc. 2017, 15, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Ng, S.J.; Dole, J.W.; Sauvajot, R.M.; Riley, S.P.D.; Valone, T.J. Use of highway undercrossings by wildlife in southern California. Biol. Conserv. 2004, 115, 499–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wilson, R.F.; Marsh, H.; Winter, J. Importance of canopy connectivity for home range and movements of the rainforest arboreal ringtail possum (Hemibelideus lemuroides). Wildl. Res. 2007, 34, 177–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Van Dyck, H.; Baguette, M. Dispersal behaviour in fragmented landscapes: Routine or special movements? Basic Appl. Ecol. 2005, 6, 535–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Vasudev, D.; Fletcher, R.J. Incorporating movement behavior into conservation prioritization in fragmented landscapes: An example of western hoolock gibbons in Garo Hills, India. Biol. Conserv. 2015, 181, 124–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Malekian, M.; Cooper, S.J.B.; Saint, K.M.; Lancaster, M.L.; Taylor, A.C.; Carthew, S.M. Effects of landscape matrix on population connectivity of an arboreal mammal, Petaurus breviceps. Ecol. Evol. 2015, 5, 3939–3953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Van Der Ree, R.; Bennett, A.F.; Gilmore, D.C. Gap-crossing by gliding marsupials: Thresholds for use of isolated woodland patches in an agricultural landscape. Biol. Conserv. 2004, 115, 241–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Lindenmayer, D.B.; Cunningham, R.B.; Matthew, L.P.; Donnelly, C. The response of arboreal marsupials to landscape context over time: A large-scale fragmentation study revisited. J. Biogeogr. 2013, 40, 2082–2093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Huang, Z.; Yuan, P.; Huang, H.; Tang, X.; Xu, W.; Huang, C.; Zhou, Q. Effect of habitat fragmentation on ranging behavior of white-headed langurs in limestone forests in Southwest China. Primates 2017, 58, 423–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Allan, B.M.; Nimmo, D.G.; Arnould, J.P.Y.; Martin, J.K.; Ritchie, E.G. The secret life of possums: Data loggers reveal the movement ecology of an arboreal mammal. J. Mammal. 2019, 100, 158–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Gracanin, A.; Cappelletti, C.; Knipler, M.; Dallas, R.K.K.; Mikac, K.M. Exploring new grounds: Arboreal sugar gliders frequently observed spending time on the ground as seen on camera traps. Aust. Mammal. 2019, 42, 10–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Vanhelden, B.E.; Close, P.G.; Stewart, B.A.; Speldewinde, P.C.; Comer, S.J. Going to ground: Implications of ground use for the conservation of an arboreal marsupial. Aust. Mammal. 2019, 42, 106–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Lindenmayer, D.B.; Pope, M.L.; Cunningham, R.B. Patch use by the greater glider (Petauroides volans) in a fragmented forest ecosystem. II. Characteristics of den trees and preliminary data on den-use patterns. Wildl. Res. 2004, 31, 569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Braden, A.W.; Lopez, R.R.; Roberts, C.W.; Silvy, N.J.; Owen, C.B.; Frank, P.A. Florida Key deer Odocoileus virginianus clavium underpass use and movements along a highway corridor. Wildlife Biol. 2008, 14, 155–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Smith, G.C.; Means, K.; Churchill, S. Aspects of the ecology of the Atherton antechinus (Antechinus godmani) living in sympatry with the rusty antechinus (A. adustus) in the Wet Tropics, Queensland—A trapping and radio-tracking study. Aust. Mammal. 2018, 40, 16–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Henry, S.R. Social organisation of the greater glider (Petauroides volans) in Victoria. In Possums and Gliders; Smith, A.P., Hume, I.D., Eds.; Surrey Beatty and Sons: Chipping Norton, UK, 1984; pp. 221–228. [Google Scholar]
  20. Goldingay, R.L.; Kavanagh, R.P. Home-range estimates and habitat of the yellow-bellied glider (Petaurus australis) at Waratah Creek, New South Wales. Wildl. Res. 1993, 20, 387–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rathbun, G.B.; Dumbacher, J.P. Home range and use of diurnal shelters by the Etendeka round-eared sengi, a newly discovered Namibian endemic desert mammal. PeerJ 2015, 3, e1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Trolle, M.; Noss, A.J.; De Lima, E.S.; Dalponte, J.C. Camera-trap studies of maned wolf density in the Cerrado and the Pantanal of Brazil. In Vertebrate Conservation and Biodiversity; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 371–378. [Google Scholar]
  23. Ridout, M.S.; Linkie, M. Estimating overlap of daily activity patterns from camera trap data. J. Agric. Biol. Environ. Stat. 2009, 14, 322–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Popescu, V.D.; de Valpine, P.; Sweitzer, R.A. Testing the consistency of wildlife data types before combining them: The case of camera traps and telemetry. Ecol. Evol. 2014, 4, 933–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Srivathsa, A.; Kumar, N.; Karanth, K.U. Home range size of the dhole estimated from cameratrap surveys. Canid Biol. Conserv. 2017, 20, 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  26. Pallemaerts, L.; Adul, I.P.; Jeffers, K.A.; Macdonald, D.W.; Cheyne, S.M. Bornean clouded leopard minimum home range analysis. Indonesian Borneo. Cat News 2019, 70, 32–35. [Google Scholar]
  27. Soisalo, M.K.; Cavalcanti, S.M.C. Estimating the density of a jaguar population in the Brazilian Pantanal using camera-traps and capture–recapture sampling in combination with GPS radio-telemetry. Biol. Conserv. 2006, 129, 487–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Pérez, J.; Valenzuela, G.; Torralba, B.; Martín, J.M.; Rodríguez-Siles, J.; Garrote, G.; Simón-Mata, M.Á.; Bueno, J.; Moral, M.; Gil-Sánchez, J.M.; et al. The use of camera trapping for estimating Iberian lynx (Lynx pardinus) home ranges. Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 2011, 57, 1203–1211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kumbhojkar, S.; Yosef, R.; Mehta, A.; Rakholia, S. A camera-trap home-range analysis of the indian leopard (Panthera pardus fusca) in Jaipur, India. Animals 2020, 10, 1600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gray, E.L.; Dennis, T.E.; Baker, A.M. Can remote infrared cameras be used to differentiate small, sympatric mammal species? A case study of the black-tailed dusky antechinus, Antechinus arktos and co-occurring small mammals in Southeast Queensland, Australia. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0181592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. De Bondi, N.; White, J.G.; Stevens, M.; Cooke, R. A comparison of the effectiveness of camera trapping and live trapping for sampling terrestrial small-mammal communities. Wildl. Res. 2010, 37, 456–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Dundas, S.J.; Ruthrof, K.X.; Hardy, G.E.S.J.; Fleming, P.A. Pits or pictures: A comparative study of camera traps and pitfall trapping to survey small mammals and reptiles. Wildl. Res. 2019, 46, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. McCleery, R.A.; Zweig, C.L.; Desa, M.A.; Hunt, R.; Kitchens, W.M.; Percival, H.F. A novel method for camera-trapping small mammals. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 2014, 38, 887–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gracanin, A.; Gracanin, V.; Mikac, K.M. The selfie trap: A novel camera trap design for accurate small mammal identification. Ecol. Manag. Restor. 2018, 20, 156–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Marchesan, D.; Carthew, S.M. Autecology of the yellow-footed antechinus (Antechinus flavipes) in a fragmented landscape in southern Australia. Wildl. Res. 2004, 31, 273–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kirk, J.; Smith, G.C.; Agnew, G. Trial radio-tracking of feathertail gliders Acrobates pygmaeus. Aust. Mammal. 2000, 22, 129–131. [Google Scholar]
  37. Millspaugh, J.; Marzluff, J.M. Radio Tracking and Animal Populations; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001; ISBN 0080540228. [Google Scholar]
  38. Soanes, K.; Lobo, M.C.; Van Der Ree, R. Radio-collared squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) struck by vehicle and transported 500km along freeway. Aust. Mammal. 2016, 38, 127–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gracanin, A.; Minchinton, T.E.; Mikac, K.M.; University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia. Estimating the density of small mammals using the selfie trap is an effective camera trapping method. 2022; manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
  40. Quin, D.G.; Smith, A.P.; Green, S.W.; Hines, H.B. Estimating the home ranges of sugar gliders (Petaurus breviceps) (Marsupialia: Petauridae), from grid-trapping and radiotelemetry. Wildl. Res. 1992, 19, 471–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. GER The Great Eastern Ranges. Available online: https://ger.org.au/ (accessed on 23 March 2021).
  42. Gracanin, A.; Mikac, K.M. Camera traps reveal overlap and seasonal variation in the diel activity of arboreal and semi-arboreal mammals. Mamm. Biol. 2022, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Australian Bureau of Meteorology Climate Data Online. Available online: http://www.bom.gov.au/climate/data/ (accessed on 2 December 2021).
  44. Tasker, E.M.; Dickman, C.R. A review of elliott trapping methods for small mammals in Australia. Aust. Mammal. 2001, 23, 77–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Suckling, G.C.; Macfarlane, M.A. Introduction of the sugar glider, Petaurus breviceps, into re-established forest of the Tower Hill State Game Reserve, Vic. Wildl. Res. 1983, 10, 249–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Campbell, C.D.; Sarre, S.D.; Stojanovic, D.; Gruber, B.; Medlock, K.; Harris, S.; MacDonald, A.J.; Holleley, C.E. When is a native species invasive? Incursion of a novel predatory marsupial detected using molecular and historical data. Divers. Distrib. 2018, 24, 831–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Jackson, M.S. Population dynamics and life history of the mahogany glider, Petaurus gracilis, and the sugar glider, Petaurus breviceps, in north Queensland. Wildl. Res. 2000, 27, 21–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Nowack, J.; Rojas, A.D.; Körtner, G.; Geiser, F. Snoozing through the storm: Torpor use during a natural disaster. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 11243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Knipler, M.L.; Dowton, M.; Mikac, K.M. Genome-wide SNPs detect hybridisation of marsupial gliders (Petaurus breviceps breviceps × Petaurus norfolcensis) in the wild. Genes 2021, 12, 1327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. ESRI. ArcGIS Desktop: Release 10.7.1; Environmental Systems Research Institute: Redlands, CA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  51. Nilsen, E.B.; Pedersen, S.; Linnell, J.D.C. Can minimum convex polygon home ranges be used to draw biologically meaningful conclusions? Ecol. Res. 2008, 23, 635–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Barry, R.P.; McIntyre, J. Estimating animal densities and home range in regions with irregular boundaries and holes: A lattice-based alternative to the kernel density estimator. Ecol. Modell. 2011, 222, 1666–1672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  54. JMP®, Version Pro 15; SAS Institute Inc.: Cary, NC, USA, 2021.
  55. Smith, A.P.; Phillips, K. A systematic technique for census of sugar gliders and other small arboreal mammals. Wildl. Res. 1984, 11, 83–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Quin, D.G. Population ecology of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) and the sugar glider (P. breviceps) (Maruspialia: Petauridae) at Limeburners Creek, on the central north coast of New South Wales. Wildl. Res. 1995, 22, 471–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Gray, E.L.; Baker, A.M.; Firn, J. Autecology of a new species of carnivorous marsupial, the endangered black-tailed dusky antechinus (Antechinus arktos), compared to a sympatric congener, the brown antechinus (Antechinus stuartii). Mammal Res. 2017, 62, 47–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Suckling, G.C. Population ecology of the sugar glider, petaurus breviceps, in a system of fragmented habitats. Wildl. Res. 1984, 11, 49–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sharpe, D.J.; Goldingay, R.L. Home range of the Australian squirrel glider, Petaurus norfolcensis (Diprotodontia). J. Mammal. 2007, 88, 1515–1522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Jackson, S.M. Home-range and den use of the mahogany glider, Petaurus gracilis. Wildl. Res. 2000, 27, 49–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Caryl, F.M.; Thomson, K.; Van Der Ree, R. Permeability of the urban matrix to arboreal gliding mammals: Sugar gliders in Melbourne, Australia. Austral Ecol. 2013, 38, 609–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Goldingay, R.L.; Taylor, B.D.; Parkyn, J.L. Use of tall wooden poles by four species of gliding mammal provides further proof of concept for habitat restoration. Aust. Mammal. 2018, 41, 255–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Marchesan, D.; Carthew, S.M. Use of space by the yellow-footed antechinus, Antechinus flavipes, in a fragmented landscape in South Australia. Landsc. Ecol. 2008, 23, 741–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Law, B.S.; Chidel, M.; Turner, G. The use by wildlife of paddock trees in farmland. Pacific Conserv. Biol. 2000, 6, 130–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Knight, E.H.; Fox, B.J. Does habitat structure mediate the effects of forest fragmentation and human-induced disturbance on the abundance of Antechinus stuartii? Aust. J. Zool. 2000, 48, 577–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Lindenmayer, D.B.; Cunningham, R.B.; Pope, M.L. A large-scale “experiment” to examine the effects of landscape context and habitat fragmentation on mammals. Biol. Conserv. 1999, 88, 387–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Carthew, S.M. Foraging Behaviour of Marsupial Pollinators in a Population of Banksia spinulosa. Oikos 1994, 69, 133–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Howard, J. Diet of Petaurus breviceps (Marsupialia: Petauridae) in a mosaic of coastal woodland and heath. Aust. Mammal. 1989, 12, 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Stobo-Wilson, A.M.; Cremona, T.; Murphy, B.P.; Carthew, S.M. Resource availability drives variation in a marsupial glider’s home-range size. J. Zool. 2021, 315, 199–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zabel, C.J.; McKelvey, K.; Ward, J.P., Jr. Influence of primary prey on home-range size and habitat-use patterns of northern spotted owls (Strix occidentalis caurina). Can. J. Zool. 1995, 73, 433–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Herfindal, I.; Linnell, J.D.C.; Odden, J.; Nilsen, E.B.; Andersen, R. Prey density, environmental productivity and home-range size in the Eurasian lynx (Lynx lynx). J. Zool. 2005, 265, 63–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Lindenmayer, D. Gliders of Australia: A Natural History; UNSW Press: Kensington, Australia, 2002; ISBN 086840523X. [Google Scholar]
  73. Van Der Ree, R.; Bennett, A.F. Home range of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) in a network of remnant linear habitats. J. Zool. 2003, 259, 327–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Pope, M.L.; Lindenmayer, D.B.; Cunningham, R.B. Patch use by the greater glider (Petauroides volans) in a fragmented forest ecosystem. I. Home range size and movements. Wildl. Res. 2004, 31, 559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. O’Donnell, K.; Delbarco-Trillo, J. Changes in the home range sizes of terrestrial vertebrates in response to urban disturbance: A meta-analysis. J. Urban Ecol. 2020, 6, juaa014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Sadler, L.M.; Ward, S.J. Coalitions in male sugar gliders: Are they natural? J. Zool. 1999, 248, 91–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Goldingay, R.L.; Sharpe, D.J.; Dobson, M.D.J. Variation in the home-range size of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis). Aust. Mammal. 2010, 32, 183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Jacques, C.N.; Zweep, J.S.; Jenkins, S.E.; Klaver, R.W. Home range use and survival of southern flying squirrels in fragmented forest landscapes. J. Mammal. 2017, 98, 1479–1488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Schultze-Westrum, T.G. Social communication by chemical signals in flying phalangers (Petaurus breviceps papuanus). Olfaction Tast. 1969, 3, 268–277. [Google Scholar]
  80. Nowak, R.M. Walker’s Mammals of the World: Monotremes, Marsupials, Afrotherians, Xenarthrans, and Sundatherians; JHU Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2018; ISBN 1421424673. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Site locations in the landscape near Berry, NSW (n = 164). Each site had a live trap (Elliott A trap) for seven nights and this was immediately replaced with a selfie trap for 28 nights. Areas one (orange), two (white), three (green) and four (blue) were surveyed immediately after each other sequentially (August to November 2019). The number of sites in each area was: Area one = 44, Area two = 45, Area three = 40, and Area four = 40.
Figure 1. Site locations in the landscape near Berry, NSW (n = 164). Each site had a live trap (Elliott A trap) for seven nights and this was immediately replaced with a selfie trap for 28 nights. Areas one (orange), two (white), three (green) and four (blue) were surveyed immediately after each other sequentially (August to November 2019). The number of sites in each area was: Area one = 44, Area two = 45, Area three = 40, and Area four = 40.
Animals 12 00912 g001
Figure 2. For small mammals captured, a unique code that corresponded with ear notching, was assigned (a). The appearance of these unique ear codes on selfie trap footage is shown in two examples: (b) a female sugar glider (Petaurus breviceps) is shown with the code ‘03’ applied (ear notch 1 and 2); and (c) a female antechinus (Antechinus stuartii) is shown with code ‘40’ applied (ear notch 40).
Figure 2. For small mammals captured, a unique code that corresponded with ear notching, was assigned (a). The appearance of these unique ear codes on selfie trap footage is shown in two examples: (b) a female sugar glider (Petaurus breviceps) is shown with the code ‘03’ applied (ear notch 1 and 2); and (c) a female antechinus (Antechinus stuartii) is shown with code ‘40’ applied (ear notch 40).
Animals 12 00912 g002
Figure 3. Example of a selfie trap positioned 2m off the ground to target arboreal species. Inside is a camera with an altered focal distance for close recording of small mammal species.
Figure 3. Example of a selfie trap positioned 2m off the ground to target arboreal species. Inside is a camera with an altered focal distance for close recording of small mammal species.
Animals 12 00912 g003
Figure 4. Polygons formed for 121 sugar gliders (Petaurus breviceps) where individuals (females = 63, males = 58) were observed on at least two selfie trap sites. Useable habitat within polygons (masked habitat) is highlighted in green.
Figure 4. Polygons formed for 121 sugar gliders (Petaurus breviceps) where individuals (females = 63, males = 58) were observed on at least two selfie trap sites. Useable habitat within polygons (masked habitat) is highlighted in green.
Animals 12 00912 g004
Figure 5. Polygons formed by connected sites for 20 brown antechinus (Antechinus stuartii) where individuals (females = 9, males = 11) were observed on at least two selfie trap sites. Useable habitat within polygons (masked habitat) is highlighted in green.
Figure 5. Polygons formed by connected sites for 20 brown antechinus (Antechinus stuartii) where individuals (females = 9, males = 11) were observed on at least two selfie trap sites. Useable habitat within polygons (masked habitat) is highlighted in green.
Animals 12 00912 g005
Figure 6. Mean distances moved (±standard error) between recaptures within patches of habitat (sampled in grids) and in linear strips (sampled as transects) for female and male (a) sugar gliders (Petaurus breviceps); and (b) brown antechinus (Antechinus stuartii). Individuals used in analysis were detected at two or more sites. Numbers below bars represent N sample size. Credit for illustration on left: Rocco Russo; and on right: Tracy MacVean.
Figure 6. Mean distances moved (±standard error) between recaptures within patches of habitat (sampled in grids) and in linear strips (sampled as transects) for female and male (a) sugar gliders (Petaurus breviceps); and (b) brown antechinus (Antechinus stuartii). Individuals used in analysis were detected at two or more sites. Numbers below bars represent N sample size. Credit for illustration on left: Rocco Russo; and on right: Tracy MacVean.
Animals 12 00912 g006
Table 1. Total number of independent observations (separated by 24 h) made for each small mammal. Observations include where individuals were recorded at different sites during a single night. The current study performed 4592 camera trap nights in total, with each area (thus each individual) camera trapped for 28 nights. Capture success was calculated by diving the total number of independent observations made per species, in each area, by camera trap nights. The number of sites in each area was: Area one = 44, Area two = 45, Area three = 40, and Area four = 40.
Table 1. Total number of independent observations (separated by 24 h) made for each small mammal. Observations include where individuals were recorded at different sites during a single night. The current study performed 4592 camera trap nights in total, with each area (thus each individual) camera trapped for 28 nights. Capture success was calculated by diving the total number of independent observations made per species, in each area, by camera trap nights. The number of sites in each area was: Area one = 44, Area two = 45, Area three = 40, and Area four = 40.
FemaleMaleUnknown SexTotalCapture Success
Area 1
Petaurus breviceps2652511553143%
Antechinus stuartii40115916413%
Rattus fuscipes02020%
Rattus norvegicus4221272%
Area 2
Petaurus breviceps529351588570%
Antechinus stuartii24814152772%
Rattus fuscipes18481671%
Rattus norvegicus60061%
Area 3
Petaurus breviceps1721084132129%
Antechinus stuartii3400343%
Rattus norvegicus15168393%
Area 4
Petaurus breviceps1110121%
Antechinus stuartii3060030627%
Rattus fuscipes3500353%
Rattus norvegicus20130333%
Table 2. Average rates of selfie trap recaptures for each individual (for those confidently sexed). Recaptures refer to the number of independent nightly observations (i.e. observations separated by 24 h), and includes observations where individuals were recorded at different sites during a single night. The current study performed 4592 camera trap nights in total, with each area (thus each individual) camera trapped for 28 nights.
Table 2. Average rates of selfie trap recaptures for each individual (for those confidently sexed). Recaptures refer to the number of independent nightly observations (i.e. observations separated by 24 h), and includes observations where individuals were recorded at different sites during a single night. The current study performed 4592 camera trap nights in total, with each area (thus each individual) camera trapped for 28 nights.
NMean Recapture Rate S.E.
Petaurus breviceps
Female8711.20.6
Male779.00.6
Antechinus stuartii
Female4914.91.5
Male216.41.2
Rattus fuscipes
Female414.85.0
Male78.02.5
Rattus norvegicus
Female96.31.4
Male87.01.4
Table 3. Movement distances observed on selfie traps for individual small mammals (of known sex) over 28-trap night periods, between August and November 2019. Included are the mean and maximum of direct straight-line distances moved by an individual small mammal.
Table 3. Movement distances observed on selfie traps for individual small mammals (of known sex) over 28-trap night periods, between August and November 2019. Included are the mean and maximum of direct straight-line distances moved by an individual small mammal.
NMean Number of Sites Detected atS.E.Mean Distance Travelled (m)S.E.Maximum Distance Observed (m)
Petaurus breviceps
Female872.410.13101320014,661
Male772.680.17116720512,015
Antechinus stuartii
Female491.270.09243912555
Male212.190.328612924237
Rattus fuscipes
Female41.000.00000
Male71.570.203942641902
Rattus norvegicus
Female91.000.00000
Male81.380.183441901283
Table 4. Short term home ranges (50% and 95%) using Kernel Density Estimate (KDE), of sugar gliders (Petaurus breviceps) detected at four or more sites (n = 34). Recapture rates were calculated as the number of distinct nights and site, that an individual was observed at. Minimum convex polygon (MCP) home range estimates were calculated by masking available habitat. Standard Error is show in brackets.
Table 4. Short term home ranges (50% and 95%) using Kernel Density Estimate (KDE), of sugar gliders (Petaurus breviceps) detected at four or more sites (n = 34). Recapture rates were calculated as the number of distinct nights and site, that an individual was observed at. Minimum convex polygon (MCP) home range estimates were calculated by masking available habitat. Standard Error is show in brackets.
NNumber of Sites Detected atRecapture RateDistance Moved (m)KDE Home Range 95% (ha)KDE Home Range 50% (ha)MCP Home Range (ha)
Grid
Female94.3 (0.2)15.0 (1.8)1697 (224)4.6 (0.4)1.1 (0.1)1.3 (0.1)
Male84.6 (0.3)11.3 (1.7)1503 (328)6.9 (0.9)1.4 (0.3)2.0 (0.4)
Linear
Female 64.7 (0.3)18.2 (2.8)5027 (2208)5.6 (1.4)1.5 (0.4)1.2 (0.2)
Male114.8 (0.4)11.8 (1.7)4000 (954)7.5 (1.2)2.1 (0.4)2.7 (0.5)
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gracanin, A.; Mikac, K.M. The Use of Selfie Camera Traps to Estimate Home Range and Movement Patterns of Small Mammals in a Fragmented Landscape. Animals 2022, 12, 912. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12070912

AMA Style

Gracanin A, Mikac KM. The Use of Selfie Camera Traps to Estimate Home Range and Movement Patterns of Small Mammals in a Fragmented Landscape. Animals. 2022; 12(7):912. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12070912

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gracanin, Ana, and Katarina M. Mikac. 2022. "The Use of Selfie Camera Traps to Estimate Home Range and Movement Patterns of Small Mammals in a Fragmented Landscape" Animals 12, no. 7: 912. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12070912

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop