Next Article in Journal
Kantian Ethics and the Animal Turn. On the Contemporary Defence of Kant’s Indirect Duty View
Next Article in Special Issue
Freedom and Animal Welfare
Previous Article in Journal
Immunocastration in Gilts: A Preliminary Study of the Effect of the Second Dose Administration Time on Growth, Reproductive Tract Development, and Carcass and Meat Quality
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Click Production of Captive Yangtze Finless Porpoises (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaorientalis) Is Influenced by Social and Environmental Factors

1
Institute of Hydrobiology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Wuhan 430000, China
2
University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100864, China
3
School of Naval Architecture and Ocean Engineering, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan 430000, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2021, 11(2), 511; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11020511
Submission received: 12 January 2021 / Revised: 6 February 2021 / Accepted: 9 February 2021 / Published: 16 February 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Zoo and Wild Animals Welfare Assessments)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Yangtze finless porpoises’ high-frequency clicks have often been studied and used for wild population surveys. However, the influence of captive environmental and social variables on Yangtze finless porpoises’ production of such signals has never been investigated. In the present study, the click production of a group of captive Yangtze finless porpoises was analyzed across various contexts. This click production was significantly impacted by temporal factors (season), social factors (social separation), and environmental factors (training sessions, presence of enrichment, noise, presence of visitors). The patterns found in this study may be useful for further monitoring of the welfare of captive groups of Yangtze finless porpoises (e.g., welfare assessments) as well as for improving wild surveys (e.g., more accurate interpretation of click density).

Abstract

Yangtze finless porpoises use high-frequency clicks to navigate, forage, and communicate. The way in which click production may vary depending on social or environmental context has never been investigated. A group of five captive Yangtze finless porpoises was monitored for one year, and 107 h of audio recordings was collected under different conditions. Using a MATLAB-generated interface, we extracted click density (i.e., number of clicks per minute) from these recordings and analyzed its variation depending on the context. As expected, click density increased as the number of animals present increased. The click density did not exhibit diurnal variations but did have seasonal variations, with click density being highest in summer and fall. Yangtze finless porpoises produced more clicks when socially separated than when not (136% more), during training/feeding sessions than outside of such sessions (312% more), when enrichment was provided (265% more on average), and when noisy events occurred rather than when no unusual event occurred (22% more). The click density decreased when many visitors were present in the facility (up to 35% less). These results show that Yangtze finless porpoises modulate their click production depending on the context and suggest that their echolocation activity and their emotional state may be linked to these changes. Such context-dependent variations also indicate the potential usefulness of monitoring acoustical activity as part of a welfare assessment tool in this species. Additionally, the click density variation found in captivity could be useful for understanding click rate variations of wild populations that are hardly visible.

1. Introduction

Odontocetes produce acoustic signals, including whistles, clicks, and burst-pulsed calls [1]. Whistles are frequency-modulated, long-duration tonal calls, which often have harmonic components [2]. Clicks are directional signals, normally of high frequency [3], and burst-pulsed signals are click trains emitted with very short inter-click intervals [4]. Odontocetes are known to produce clicks more than they produce other types of sounds, probably due to the higher energetic costs of whistles in comparison to clicks [5]. Echolocation is a sensory modality used by odontocetes to obtain an assessment of their environment, consisting of emitting clicks and receiving the echoes from the clicks reflecting off objects. Odontocetes use echolocation clicks to explore their environment (including locating objects and prey) and as navigation cues [3,6]. Even though these clicks are assumed to be mainly produced for echolocation purposes, they have also been suggested to be used for social communication by some species [7,8,9].
Researchers may be able to obtain information about marine mammals’ behavior or their environment based on the specific types of sounds they emit and the variations in call rates [10,11,12]. Some odontocete species, including porpoises (Phocoenidae family [13,14,15,16,17,18,19]), Commerson’s dolphins (Cephalorhynchus commersonii [20,21,22,23,24,25]), Hourglass dolphins (Lagenorhynchus cruciger [21,22]), and pygmy sperm whales (Kogia breviceps, [8]), are thought to only produce narrowband and high-frequency clicks, but not whistles. Click emission rate variations would therefore represent the only way for these animals to communicate [7,9,26]. The variability of recorded click rates can reflect periods of activity and rest [27,28,29,30]. Understanding the context of click production might be useful when studying wild animals’ movement patterns; for example, a lack of acoustic detections may not reflect the absence of animals, but rather, result from the presence of non-vocal animals [31]. Lima et al. [32] suggested that more studies on the context of acoustic signal production were needed to understand free-ranging odontocete behavior when animals are not visible. Assessments of odontocetes’ responses to stimuli that are potential disturbances usually focus on whether animals leave exposed areas [33,34]. However, animals that do not leave can exhibit changes in activity budgets [35,36]. Monitoring such changes has often been suggested to be partly possible through the analysis of these animals’ echolocation activity [37,38]. Knowledge of condition-dependent acoustic production rates in small odontocetes is still limited [39]. Authors have suggested that more contexts should be investigated to study the presence of referential content in the acoustic production of odontocetes [40,41,42,43].
Vocal activity has been suggested to be a valid non-invasive tool for monitoring the influence of captive environments and daily routines on farm and zoo animals (e.g., [44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51]). However, the potential use of this method as a tool to assess captive odontocetes’ welfare has rarely been studied [42,43,52]. Monitoring the acoustic activity of these animals might provide valuable information about their daily rhythm, health, and welfare [42,43,52,53]. Understanding captive animals’ vocal activity patterns could aid in improving their management under human care [32]. Using acoustic activity to monitor odontocetes is convenient and less time-consuming than behavioral coding. Analyzing click rates can be automated and therefore achieved relatively quickly. In addition, placing sound-recording devices in the water is non-invasive for animals, who can habituate to these objects. Such passive acoustical monitoring would allow caretakers to notice changes in baseline patterns, supporting a closer examination of the animals.
Yangtze finless porpoises (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaeorientlis) rely on clicks for navigation while traveling and foraging, and they have been shown to produce click trains every 5.1 s on average [6], with a click train rate varying from 0 to 290 click trains per 10 min [39]. Yangtze finless porpoise clicks are similar to those of other members of the Phocoenidae family or of Cephalorhynchus spp. within delphinids: they are typical high-frequency narrow-band ultrasonic pulses with peak frequencies ranging between 87 and 145 kHz [54]. These freshwater animals suffer from greater threats than other species, including being trapped in shallow areas, intense anthropogenic disturbances increasing risks of entanglement with underwater debris or fishing nets, or collision with boats. The environment they live in (i.e., turbid water) does not enable them to efficiently use their vision, making echolocation even more crucial for these animals’ survival. Because of the difficulty of observing Yangtze finless porpoises in their natural habitat, population density is often assessed using passive acoustic monitoring that relies on counting the number of click trains [55,56,57]. Because of anthropogenic threats, Yangtze finless porpoises are now critically endangered [58,59], and in order to add to the in situ conservation efforts, porpoises are being held in captivity or semi-natural environments [59]. The acoustical adaptability of these animals to different environments has rarely been studied. The only work that investigated such abilities involved three groups of Yangtze finless porpoises living under different conditions (i.e., captivity, reserve, wild) and revealed differences in their click production, suggesting that they would adapt their echolocation signals depending on their living conditions [60]. This result has shown that animals modulate their acoustical activity depending on the environment they live in, but within each environment, the context-induced variations in porpoise click production have never been studied. In the present study, we recorded the acoustic activity of a group of five captive Yangtze finless porpoises and analyzed their click production variations depending on the context. The aim of this study was to describe click rate patterns in this group to investigate whether Yangtze finless porpoises modulate their click production depending on the context. The usefulness of this easy-to-assess parameter as a tool to monitor captive porpoise welfare was then considered.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Animals and Captive Management

Data were collected from October 2017 to November 2018. The study subjects were five Yangtze finless porpoises housed in Baiji Dolphinarium, Institute of Hydrobiology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (Table 1). Porpoises were usually housed in a two-pool complex including a kidney-shaped pool, a connected round pool, and a non-connected round pool (Figure 1). The social grouping changed several times during data collection. When social separation occurred, a gate between the two connected pools was closed, still allowing visual and acoustical contact. The three females were pregnant during the data collection, and gave birth during summer 2018. A third pool was used to house the female F7 and the male Taotao from February 2017, until F7 gave birth (2 June 2017). When the female F7 gave birth, she remained alone in this pool with her calf and Taotao was moved back into the two-pool complex. Two calves were present from their birth to their death (two weeks after their birth), and the third one (F7′s calf) was present from its birth until the end of data collection.
Porpoises were fed between 3 and 3.5 kg of thawed Basilewsky fish (Siniperca chuatsi) and/or live fish per day during four to six training/feeding sessions. The total amount of fish provided per day to each animal varied with age, sex, season, and reproductive status, and was designed to maintain a healthy body weight and optimum condition. Occasional visitors were allowed to watch animals both from the surface and from underwater windows. Visitors were groups of between two and thirty persons (4.57 ± 5.99 persons on average), and visits were planned randomly (usually not more than one visit per week). Enrichment was provided (toys, live fish, or free interaction with caretakers). Pools were cleaned by divers and/or caretakers scrubbing the upper part of the pools’ walls approximately once per month.

2.2. Data Collection

A Soundtrap 300 HF (Ocean Instruments Ltd., Whangateau, New Zealand) was used to record underwater sound. The sampling rate was set at 576 kHz, the number of analog-to-digital bits was set at 16 bit, and high-pass filtering was turned off. Animals had been in contact with the Soundtrap from September 2017, and were therefore habituated to it and rarely explored it or interacted with it. The Soundtrap was tied to a rope and placed in the corridor between the two connected pools or on a side of the pool in the non-connected pool, at approximately 2 m depth. Data were collected for a minimum of two days a week, and the Soundtrap was usually placed in the pool in the early morning and removed in the late afternoon in order not to create a change in the porpoises’ environment right before recording. When a group was housed in the non-connected pool, and one in the two-pool complex, the Soundtrap was randomly placed in one of the two pools for the entire day, or was placed in one pool in the morning and in the other at noon. Each recording day, a minimum of three 10–20 min samples were extracted from the Soundtrap: usually one in the morning, one at noon, and one in the afternoon. The precise sampling time depended on the day’s schedule, which was never exactly the same.
For each of the 10–20 min samples, the associated conditions were noted, including: season, time of day, time relative to training/feeding sessions, social grouping, presence of enrichment, unusual events, and presence of visitors (Table 2).

2.3. Analysis

2.3.1. Echolocation Signal Analysis

Data were split into one-minute recordings. MATLAB R2017a was used to analyze each one-minute sample. Here, following previous studies, we defined porpoise echolocation signals as clicks [3] ranging between 87 and 128 kHz [54]. Because of the number of individuals present and the potential reflection of the signals they produced by the water surface and pool walls, records often contained a large number of clicks. These clicks were usually produced in trains defined as a minimum of five consecutive clicks [57,61], with a maximum of 200 ms between clicks (inter-click interval, [6,62]). The resulting large number of click trains recorded were often overlapping or very close to each other. Using MATLAB to analyze click trains was therefore a difficult task; it was determined that only clicks themselves but not click trains would be analyzed to keep the analysis as simple as possible so that captive facilities could easily reproduce it and use it to monitor their animals. The MATLAB algorithm was based on a support vector machine (SVM) model, one of the binary classification machine-learning methods, to distinguish clicks. A first-order finite impulse response (FIR) high-pass filter was used to filter the signal, with a Kaiser window function (beta = 0.5) and an 80 kHz cut-off frequency. After this first step, a short-time Fourier transform (window = 512, noverlap = 256, nfft = 512) was applied to obtain the time-frequency distribution of the signal. Then a dataset consisting of 2000 spectra of representative clicks and background noise was created. Random noise was also added within 3dB to ensure the generalization performance of the dataset. The SVM model was performed after data enhancement, and an accuracy rate of 98.7% through 5-fold cross-validation was eventually achieved. The goal here was not to obtain the exact number of clicks produced by the animals or their characteristics, but to analyze variations in their production. Even if the number of clicks obtained through the analysis was overestimated because of the reflections, these reflections were assumed to always be the same (same pool); therefore, variations in the number of clicks recorded reflected the actual variation in Yangtze finless porpoise click production. To ensure the accuracy of the MATLAB analysis, a visual/manual analysis was additionally conducted on 10% of the data. Number of clicks per minute will hereafter be referred to as “click density”.

2.3.2. Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using R 4.0.3. The effect of the number of individuals present in the pool, and of the environmental and social variables on the click density was analyzed by fitting a generalized linear mixed-effects model (GLMM) for Poisson distributed data using the “glmer” function from the “lme4” package [63]. A bound optimization by quadratic approximation (“bobyqa”) optimizer that performs derivative-free bound-constrained optimization using an iteratively constructed quadratic approximation for the objective function was added. For this model, the click density was the response variable, the number of individuals present in the pool was included as an offset to standardize the density, and the date and observation session ID were included as random factors. The predictors included number of individuals present, time of day, time relative to training, social grouping, presence of enrichment, unusual events, and presence of visitors. A model diagnosis was conducted, including a test for overdispersion and a test for collinearity. Collinearity was tested by looking at variables’ variance inflation factor (VIF). Because the enrichment variable caused collinearity issues (VIF > 3), this variable was split into four variables (presence of toys, presence of humans, presence of toys and humans together, presence of new objects). The live fish enrichment was excluded from the analysis because of a too small sample size. An additional case level random factor was added to correct over-dispersion. The model with the lowest Akaike information criterion (AIC) was selected [64]. A Wald chi-squared test was conducted to extract p-values from the model. Pairwise tests were conducted by running the selected model with an appropriate sub-setting (i.e., same model, with only two levels of the targeted predictor included) and a sequential Bonferroni correction.

3. Results

A total of 6442 min (approximately 107 h) of recording was analyzed. The click density ranged between 0 and 9576 clicks per minute (3864.33 ± 2189.60 clicks on average). The click density (i.e., number of clicks per minute) was negatively linked to the number of porpoises present in the pool (Figure 2a, Table 3). The more porpoises in the pool, the less clicks were produced. The click density was significantly affected by the season: it was significantly higher in fall than in spring (Figure 2b, Table 3). The click density did not significantly vary depending on the time of day, but it was significantly higher during training/feeding sessions than outside of such sessions (Figure 2c, Table 3). Clicks were significantly more frequent when porpoises were separated by a gate than when not (Figure 2d, Table 3). Clicks were significantly more frequent with the presence of toys, humans and toys together, and new objects, but less frequent when humans were present alone than when not present (Figure 2e, Table 3). Clicks were significantly more frequent during noisy events rather than when no unusual event occurred (Figure 2f, Table 3). The presence of visitors significantly impacted the click density, with less clicks produced when many visitors were present rather than when few or none were (Figure 2g, Table 3).

4. Discussion

First, the standardized number of clicks decreased with increasing number of animals present in the pool. Clicks are thought to serve both as an echolocation and a communication function in odontocetes such as Yangtze finless porpoises that do not whistle [9,65,66,67]. With more animals present, each animal’s clicks are added to the others’, and we expected a higher occurrence of clicks. A low number of animals in a pool always followed a separation and transport process during which porpoises were lifted out of the water and moved to a new pool. During this process, they were separated from some of their congeners. They might have needed to (1) explore this new environment, and (2) locate the congeners that stayed in the initial pool, resulting in more clicks. This hypothesis requires further investigation.
Unlike previous findings suggesting that clicks, which are used mainly to navigate, feed, and communicate, were unlikely to exhibit any seasonal cycle (e.g., [68]), a seasonal pattern was found in the Yangtze finless porpoises’ click production. Yangtze finless porpoises are seasonal breeders, and their socio-sexual activities increase in summer and fall [69]; such periods may require more communication as well as more echolocation to locate potential mates. Conversely, the click density did not exhibit any diurnal pattern. Some studies have shown that wild Yangtze finless porpoises produced more clicks at night than during the day, suggesting that these animals were feeding at night [70], while other studies found no day/night differences [39]. Data were not collected at night in this study, and only differences between morning, noon, and afternoon were analyzed. Such analysis could bring additional information about the diurnal rhythm of Yangtze finless porpoises’ acoustic production.
Social changes are likely to impact odontocete sound production [43]. Captive bottlenose dolphins have been shown to whistle 7.8 times more in isolation than when they were not separated from their congeners [71], and social separation has been reported to affect some whistle types but not others in this species [43]. Yangtze finless porpoises produced more clicks when they were separated in subgroups. Being physically separated into two pools with a gate and yet remaining in acoustic contact, Yangtze finless porpoises might have used clicks in order to communicate and maintain cohesion between pools, or to try to locate other individuals through the gate.
Yangtze finless porpoises produced more clicks during training/feeding sessions than outside of such sessions. This pattern has already been reported in other captive odontocetes with an increase in all types of underwater sounds during training/feeding/public presentations (bottlenose dolphins [42,72,73,74,75]; belugas, Delphinapterus leucas [76]; Pacific white-sided dolphins, Lagenorhynchus obliquidens [77]). Such routine events might have caused an increased level of excitement or anticipation [72,73]. In addition, studies have shown that odontocete click rate increases when foraging because they produce buzzes that are click trains with short inter-click intervals [8,78,79,80]. Yangtze finless porpoises have been shown to emit such buzzes when feeding, especially when an individual catches its prey [69]. During training/feeding sessions, the presence of prey (both dead and live), and the animals’ eating behavior may have elicited a production of buzzes which resulted in an increased click density. Porpoises could also have simply increased their echolocation activity and produced more clicks when prey were present. The fact that the Yangtze finless porpoises’ click density was the highest when live fish were present (compared with other types of enrichment) is congruent with this hypothesis.
Clicks were also more frequent when toys, humans and toys, or new objects were present together rather than when not. The presence of humans has been shown to be linked to an increased sound production in captive odontocetes [43,74,77], which could explain why the presence of humans and toys together resulted in a higher click density than when toys were provided alone. However, the presence of humans alone resulted in a lower click density. When humans were present together with toys, these humans were always familiar and they always tried to interact with the porpoises (e.g., showing them familiar or new toys, throwing them in the pool), and porpoises often participated in the interaction (e.g., approaching, observing, touching, bringing objects back). In addition, these familiar humans were usually caretakers, and porpoises might have associated their presence with events such as training sessions and feeding; an increase in clicks production may therefore have been linked to an anticipation of such events when caretakers were interacting with them. When caretakers were present without toys, porpoises were often coming to observe them and leaving as soon as they realized no fish or toys were provided. This reaction could be an explanation for the lower click density associated with human enrichment, but more work is needed to validate this hypothesis. The presence of many visitors was also linked to a lower production of clicks. Visitors were not allowed to approach the water surface, and porpoises interacted much less with them. Such infrequent and often noisy stimulus might have been stressful for the animals, and producing less clicks may be a response to this stressful stimulus. Regarding the presence of new objects in the pool, the higher density of clicks may be linked to an increased exploration behavior.
Yangtze finless porpoises’ click production was higher during noisy events (e.g., construction work, unusual noisy meetings) than when no unusual event occurred. When discussing the effects of boats on wild odontocetes, it was suggested that a boat’s physical presence, its noise, and its behavior together might impact the animals’ perception of risk to determine their response [81,82]. Because it is often hypothesized that odontocetes are sensitive to noise [83,84], noisy events have the potential to affect their behavior, including vocalizations. The porpoises in this study may have used clicks to search for and investigate the source of the noise. Social events and pool cleaning did not significantly influence Yangtze finless porpoise click production. The few studies that investigated the impact of unusual events that occur in captive facilities on the acoustic activity of odontocetes reported a decreased acoustic production in beluga whales after transportation to a new facility and after the introduction of four harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) into their pool [52], and a decrease in acoustic productions during pool cleaning for bottlenose dolphins [43]. Here, social events were group separations and reunions, and it seems that Yangtze finless porpoises did not change their click production much following these events. Pool cleaning, which elicits changes in behavior in this group of Yangtze finless porpoises [85,86,87], did not significantly influence their click production either. Noise and pool cleaning can represent stressful stimuli, noise being an acoustic one, while pool cleaning involves the presence of moving objects/humans in the water. The fact that Yangtze finless porpoises modified their click production during noisy events but not during pool cleaning suggests that different types of potentially stressful stimuli elicit different responses, and that behavior should be monitored together with acoustical activity to get a full picture of the animals’ reaction.
Although studies investigating the link between vocalizations and welfare in odontocetes are scarce, previous studies have suggested that variations in odontocete signals might be related to emotional states and specific events [88,89,90]. Most studies focused on the link between behavior/context (e.g., excitement, bow riding, feeding, socializing) and wild animal whistle production (Hawaiian spinner dolphins, Stenella longirostris [27]; common dolphins, Delphinus delphis [91]; pilot whales, Globicephala sp. [92]; bottlenose dolphins [93,94]). For porpoises that are thought to only produce one type of vocalization (high-frequency clicks), it has been suggested that differences in click train characteristics (e.g., inter-click interval) are dependent on behavioral context [9,68,95]. Here, because of the reflection phenomenon, we could not analyze click trains and their parameters.
However, despite these reflections, the variations we observed in click density seemed to be reliable and to indicate which variables influenced Yangtze finless porpoises’ click production and in what ways. The variations observed depending on the number of animals present and on the presence of prey allow us to validate the patterns we found for the other variables. Among these other variables, social separation, presence of enrichment, presence of visitors, and potentially stressful events probably resulted in changes in the animals’ emotional state, and the changes in click production we observed might be indirectly reflecting these emotional state changes. The patterns we found might help the care team to understand their animals’ acoustical activity and to adapt their management to this activity. An abrupt change in click density after a management decision such as group separation or the occurrence of any kind of unusual event should be alerting and trigger a closer monitoring of the animals to ensure they are adapting adequately to the situation for instance. Also, starting a training session when porpoises’ click production is relatively high might not be very efficient as the animals might not be fully attentive, but this hypothesis needs further work to be validated. It seems that the relationship between click production and emotional state can be ambiguous, and hard to interpret on its own. For instance, click density is particularly high when porpoises are feeding, which can be seen as a positive situation, but it is also very high when animals are socially separated, which is probably experienced as a negative situation by these porpoises [85,86,87]. Noting the context when collecting data is crucial to avoid misinterpretations. Additionally, because some variables may influence acoustic production while others may influence behavior, and because behavioral data may help the interpretation of acoustical data, monitoring captive animals’ click production should be combined with behavioral observations.
In addition to these potential applications in captive settings, our study also provides information that can be used when monitoring wild populations. Such information on the contexts in which captive Yangtze finless porpoises increase or reduce their click production could aid in understanding wild animal responses to anthropogenic disturbances. A low click density could reflect not only the absence or low number of porpoises in a zone, but also their low click production in response to a noisy environment. In addition, the important click density difference when animals are eating/hunting versus when they are not should be considered when using clicks as an indicator of Yangtze finless porpoise density. Foraging animals and resting/traveling/socializing animals do not produce the same number of clicks, and the number of Yangtze finless porpoises present may be over- or underestimated. However, the click production of wild and captive individuals may differ, and studying the differences in click rates between wild and captive porpoises as well as the variation of click rates depending on potential wild disturbances is needed to determine if results obtained in captivity can be applied in the wild. Considering the different environments in which wild and captive porpoises live, their click rates may not vary depending on similar factors or following similar patterns for instance. We therefore suggest that more research should be conducted to investigate the acoustical response of Yangtze finless porpoises to various stimuli, in both wild and captive settings, and that other kinds of signals should be analyzed (e.g., burst-pulsed signals).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.S.; methodology, A.S.; software, C.X.; statistical analysis, A.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.S.; writing—review and editing, A.S.; supervision, Y.H. and D.W.; funding acquisition, A.S. and Y.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Ocean Park Conservation Foundation Hong Kong, grant number AW01-1819, and the Science and Technology Department of Hubei Province, grant number 2018BEC477.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the caretakers of the Baiji Dolphinarium who allowed us to conduct our audio recordings. We also thank Alain Serres for his help in data handling. A financial contribution for students was supplied by the CAS-TWAS President’s fellowship.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Tyack, P.L.; Clark, C.W. Communication and acoustic behavior of dolphins and whales. In Hearing by Whales and Dolphins; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 156–224. [Google Scholar]
  2. Richardson, W.J.; Greene, C.R., Jr.; Malme, C.I.; Thomson, D.H. Marine Mammals and Noise; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Au, W.W. Characteristics of dolphin sonar signals. In The Sonar of Dolphins; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2003; pp. 115–139. [Google Scholar]
  4. Lammers, M.O.; Au, W.W.; Herzing, D.L. The broadband social acoustic signaling behavior of spinner and spotted dolphins. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 114, 1629–1639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Jensen, F.H.; Beedholm, K.; Wahlberg, M.; Bejder, L.; Madsen, P.T. Estimated communication range and energetic cost of bottlenose dolphin whistles in a tropical habitat. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012, 131, 582–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Akamatsu, T.; Wang, D.; Wang, K.; Naito, Y. Biosonar behaviour of free-ranging porpoises. Proc. R. Soc. B: Boil. Sci. 2005, 272, 797–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Dawson, S.M. Clicks and communication: The behavioural and social contexts of Hector’s dolphin vocalizations. Ethology 1991, 88, 265–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Madsen, P.T.; Carder, D.A.; Bedholm, K.; Ridgway, S.H. Porpoise clicks from a sperm whale nose—Convergent evolution of 130 kHz pulses in toothed whale sonars? Bioacoustics 2005, 15, 195–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Clausen, K.T.; Wahlberg, M.; Beedholm, K.; Deruiter, S.; Madsen, P.T. Click communication in harbour porpoises Phocoena phocoena. Bioacoustics 2011, 20, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Esch, H.C.; Sayigh, L.S.; Blum, J.E.; Wells, R.S. Whistles as potential indicators of stress in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J. Mammal. 2009, 90, 638–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Hawkins, E.R.; Gartside, D.F. Whistle emissions of Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) differ with group composition and surface behaviors. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010, 127, 2652–2663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Platto, S.; Wang, D.; Wang, K. Variation in the emission rate of sounds in a captive group of false killer whales Pseudorca crassidens during feedings: Possible food anticipatory vocal activity? Chin. J. Oceanol. Limnol. 2015, 34, 1218–1237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hatakeyama, Y.; Soeda, H. Studies on echolocation of porpoises taken in salmon gillnet fisheries. In Sensory Abilities of Cetaceans; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1990; pp. 269–281. [Google Scholar]
  14. Silber, G. Acoustic signal of the vaquita (Phocoena sinus). Aquat. Mamm. 1991, 17, 130–133. [Google Scholar]
  15. Au, W.W.L.; Kastelein, R.A.; Rippe, T.; Schooneman, N.M. Transmission beam pattern and echolocation signals of a harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1999, 106, 3699–3705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Li, S.; Wang, D.; Wang, K.; Akamatsu, T.; Ma, Z.; Han, J. Echolocation click sounds from wild inshore finless porpoise (Neophocaena phocaenoides sunameri) with comparisons to the sonar of riverine N. p. asiaeorientalis. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007, 121, 3938–3946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Villadsgaard, A.; Wahlberg, M.; Tougaard, J. Echolocation signals of wild harbour porpoises, Phocoena phocoena. J. Exp. Biol. 2007, 210, 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Jefferson, T.A.; Webber, M.A.; Pitman, R.L. Marine Mammals of the World: A Comprehensive Guide to Their Identification; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  19. Bassett, H.R.; Baumann, S.; Campbell, G.S.; Wiggins, S.M.; Hildebrand, J.A. Dall’s porpoise (Phocoenoides dalli) echolocation click spectral structure. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 125, 2677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dawson, S.M. The high frequency sounds of free-ranging Hector s dolphins, Cephalorhynchus hectori. Rep. Int. Whal. Comm. Spec. Issue 1988, 9, 339–344. [Google Scholar]
  21. Kyhn, L.A.; Tougaard, J.; Jensen, F.; Wahlberg, M.; Stone, G.; Yoshinaga, A.; Beedholm, K.; Madsen, P.T. Feeding at a high pitch: Source parameters of narrow band, high-frequency clicks from echolocating off-shore hourglass dolphins and coastal Hector’s dolphins. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 125, 1783–1791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Kyhn, L.A.; Jensen, F.H.; Beedholm, K.; Tougaard, J.; Hansen, M.; Madsen, P.T. Echolocation in sympatric Peale’s dolphins (Lagenorhynchus australis) and Commerson’s dolphins (Cephalorhynchus commersonii) producing narrow-band high-frequency clicks. J. Exp. Biol. 2010, 213, 1940–1949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Götz, T.; Antunes, R.; Heinrich, S. Echolocation clicks of free-ranging Chilean dolphins (Cephalorhynchus eutropia). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010, 128, 563–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Morisaka, T.; Karczmarski, L.; Akamatsu, T.; Sakai, M.; Dawson, S.; Thornton, M. Echolocation signals of Heaviside’s dolphins (Cephalorhynchus heavisidii). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 129, 449–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Reyes Reyes, M.V.; Iñíguez, M.A.; Hevia, M.; Hildebrand, J.A.; Melcón, M.L. Description and clustering of echolocation signals of Commerson’s dolphins (Cephalorhynchus commersonii) in Bahía San Julián, Argentina. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2015, 138, 2046–2053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Yoshida, Y.M.; Morisaka, T.; Sakai, M.; Iwasaki, M.; Wakabayashi, I.; Seko, A.; Kasamatsu, M.; Akamatsu, T.; Kohshima, S. Sound variation and function in captive Commerson’s dolphins (Cephalorhynchus commersonii). Behav. Process. 2014, 108, 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Norris, K.S.; Wursig, B.; Wells, R.S.; Wursig, M. The Hawaiian spinner dolphin. University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1994; 436p. [Google Scholar]
  28. Barrett-Lennard, L.G.; Ford, J.K.; Heise, K.A. The mixed blessing of echolocation: Differences in sonar use by fish-eating and mammal-eating killer whales. Anim. Behav. 1996, 51, 553–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Van Parijs, S.M.; Corkeron, P.J. Vocalizations and behaviour of Pacific humpback dolphins Sousa chinensis. Ethology 2001, 107, 701–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Nowacek, D.P. Acoustic ecology of foraging bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), habitat-specific use of three sound types. Mar. Mammal Sci. 2005, 21, 587–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. MacKenzie, D.I.; Nichols, J.D.; Royle, J.A.; Pollock, K.H.; Bailey, L.; Hines, J.E. Occupancy Estimation and Modeling: Inferring Patterns and Dynamics of Species Occurrence; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  32. Lima, A.; Lemasson, A.; Boye, M.; Hausberger, M. Vocal activities reflect the temporal distribution of bottlenose dolphin social and non-social activity in a zoological park. Zoo Biol. 2017, 36, 351–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Nowacek, D.P.; Thorne, L.H.; Johnston, D.W.; Tyack, P.L. Responses of cetaceans to anthropogenic noise. Mammal Rev. 2007, 37, 81–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Thompson, P.M.; Brookes, K.L.; Graham, I.M.; Barton, T.R.; Needham, K.; Bradbury, G.; Merchant, N.D. Short-term disturbance by a commercial two-dimensional seismic survey does not lead to long-term displacement of harbour porpoises. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2013, 280, 20132001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Lusseau, D. Effects of tour boats on the behavior of bottlenose dolphins: Using Markov chains to model anthropogenic impacts. Conserv. Biol. 2003, 17, 1785–1793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Pirotta, E.; Brookes, K.L.; Graham, I.M.; Thompson, P.M. Variation in harbour porpoise activity in response to seismic survey noise. Biol. Lett. 2014, 10, 20131090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Van Parijs, S.M.; Clark, C.W.; Sousa-Lima, R.S.; Parks, S.E.; Rankin, S.; Risch, D.; Van Opzeeland, I.C. Management and research applications of real-time and archival passive acoustic sensors over varying temporal and spatial scales. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2009, 395, 21–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Marques, T.A.; Thomas, L.; Martin, S.W.; Mellinger, D.K.; Ward, J.A.; Moretti, D.J.; Harris, D.V.; Tyack, P.L. Estimating animal population density using passive acoustics. Biol. Rev. 2012, 88, 287–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Kimura, S.; Akamatsu, T.; Wang, D.; Li, S.; Wang, K.; Yoda, K. Variation in the production rate of biosonar signals in freshwater porpoises. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2013, 133, 3128–3134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Ralston, J.V.; Herman, L.M. Dolphin auditory perception. In The Comparative Psychology of Audition: Perceiving Complex Sounds; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1989; pp. 295–328. [Google Scholar]
  41. Herzing, D.L. Acoustics and social behavior of wild dolphins: Implications for a sound society. In Hearing by Whales and Dolphins; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 225–272. [Google Scholar]
  42. Akiyama, J.; Ohta, M. Increased number of whistles of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, arising from interaction with people. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 2007, 69, 165–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Therrien, S.C.; Thomas, J.A.; Therrien, R.E.; Stacey, R. Time of Day and Social Change Affect Underwater Sound Production by Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) at the Brookfield Zoo. Aquat. Mamm. 2012, 38, 65–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Boinski, S.; Gross, T.S.; Davis, J.K. Terrestrial predator alarm vocalizations are a valid monitor of stress in captive brown capuchins (Cebus apella). Zoo Biol. 1999, 18, 295–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mansour, A.A.; Zakaria, A.H.; Fraser, A.F. Effect of enclosure quality on reactivity and welfare of captive Soemmerring’s gazelle (Gazella soemmerringii). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2000, 3, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Taylor, A.A.; Weary, D.M. Vocal responses of piglets to castration: Identifying procedural sources of pain. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000, 69, 255–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Zimmerman, P.H.; Koene, P.; Van Hooff, J.A. Thwarting of behaviour in different contexts and the gakel-call in the laying hen. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000, 69, 255–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Zimmerman, P.H.; Lundberg, A.; Keeling, L.J.; Koene, P. The effect of an audience on the gakel-call and other frustration behaviours in the laying hen (Gallus gallus domesticus). Anim. Welf. Potters Bar Wheathampstead 2003, 12, 315–326. [Google Scholar]
  49. Schon, P.C.; Puppe, B.; Manteuffel, G. Automated recording of stress vocalisations as a tool to document impaired welfare in pigs. Anim. Welf. Potters Bar Wheathampstead 2004, 13, 105–110. [Google Scholar]
  50. McCowan, B.; Rommeck, I.; Anderson, K.; Heagerty, A.; Cameron, A. Bioacoustic monitoring of aggression in group-housed rhesus macaques. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2006, 9, 261–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Golosova, O.S.; Volodin, I.A.; Isaeva, I.L.; Volodina, E.V. Effects of free-ranging, semi-captive and captive management on the acoustics of male rutting calls in Siberian wapiti Cervus elaphus sibiricus. Mammal Res. 2017, 62, 387–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Castellote, M.; Fossa, F. Measuring acoustic activity as a method to evaluate welfare in captive beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Aquat. Mamm. 2006, 32, 325–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Clegg, I.; Borger-Turner, J.; Eskelinen, H. C-Well: The development of a welfare assessment index for captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Anim. Welf. 2015, 24, 267–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Li, S.; Wang, K.; Wang, D.; Akamatsu, T. Echolocation signals of the free-ranging Yangtze finless porpoise (Neophocaena phocaenoides asiaeorientialis). J. Acoust. Soc.Am. 2005, 117, 3288–3296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Wang, K.; Wang, D.; Akamatsu, T.; Li, S.; Xiao, J. A passive acoustic monitoring method applied to observation and group size estimation of finless porpoises. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2005, 118, 1180–1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Kimura, S.; Akamatsu, T.; Li, S.; Dong, S.; Dong, L.; Wang, K.; Wang, D.; Arai, N. Density estimation of Yangtze finless porpoises using passive acoustic sensors and automated click train detection. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010, 128, 1435–1545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Wang, Z.-T.; Akamatsu, T.; Mei, Z.; Dong, L.; Imaizumi, T.; Wang, K.; Wang, D. Frequent and prolonged nocturnal occupation of port areas by Yangtze finless porpoises (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis): Forced choice for feeding? Integr. Zool. 2015, 10, 122–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Mei, Z.; Huang, S.-L.; Hao, Y.; Turvey, S.T.; Gong, W.; Wang, D. Accelerating population decline of Yangtze finless porpoise (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaeorientalis). Biol. Conserv. 2012, 153, 192–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Wang, D. Population status, threats and conservation of the Yangtze finless porpoise. Chin. Sci. Bull. 2009, 54, 3473–3484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Fang, L.; Wang, D.; Li, Y.; Cheng, Z.; Pine, M.K.; Wang, K.; Li, S. The source parameters of echolocation clicks from captive and free-ranging yangtze finless porpoises (neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaeorientalis). PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0129143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Dong, L.; Wang, D.; Wang, K.; Li, S.; Dong, S.; Zhao, X.; Akamatsu, T.; Kimura, S. Passive acoustic survey of Yangtze finless porpoises using a cargo ship as a moving platform. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 130, 2285–2292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Akamatsu, T.; Wang, D.; Nakamura, K.; Wang, K. Echolocation range of captive and free- ranging baiji (Lipotes vexillifer), finless porpoise (Neophocaena phocaenoides), and bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1998, 104, 2511–2516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2014, 67, 7–48. [Google Scholar]
  64. Akaike, H. Maximum likelihood identification of Gaussian autoregressive moving average models. Biometrika 1973, 60, 255–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Verboom, W.C.; Kastelein, R.A. Structure of Harbor Porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) Acoustic Signals with High Repetition Rates. In Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins; Thomas, J.A., Moss, C., Vater, M., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2003; pp. 40–42. [Google Scholar]
  66. Koschinski, S.; Diederichs, A.; Amundin, M. Click train patterns of free-ranging harbour porpoises acquired using T-PODs may be useful as indicators of their behaviour. J. Cetacean Res. Manag. 2008, 10, 147–155. [Google Scholar]
  67. Verfuss, U.K.; Miller, L.A.; Pilz, P.K.D.; Schnitzler, H.-U.; Verfuß, U.K. Echolocation by two foraging harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). J. Exp. Biol. 2009, 212, 823–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Soldevilla, M.S.; Wiggins, S.M.; Hildebrand, J.A. Spatial and temporal patterns of Risso’s dolphin echolocation in the Southern California Bight. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010, 127, 124–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Wei, Z.; Wang, D.; Zhang, X.; Wang, K.; Chen, D.; Zhao, Q.; Kuang, X.; Gong, W.; Wang, X. Observation on some sexual behavior of the Yangtze finless porpoise (Neophocaena phocaenoides asiaerientals) in captivity. Acta Theriol. Sin. 2004, 24, 98–102. [Google Scholar]
  70. Wang, Z.; Akamatsu, T.; Wang, K.; Wang, D. The diel rhythms of biosonar behavior in the Yangtze finless porpoise (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaeorientalis) in the port of the Yangtze River: The correlation between prey availability and boat traffic. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e97907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Rachinas-Lopes, P.; Luís, A.R.; Borges, A.S.; Neto, M.; Dos Santos, M.E. Whistle Stability and Variation in Captive Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) Recorded in Isolation and Social Contexts. Aquat. Mamm. 2017, 43, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Tyack, P. Whistle repertoires of two bottlenosed dolphins, Tursiops truncatus: Mimicry of signature whistles? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1986, 18, 251–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Janik, V.M.; Todt, D.; Dehnhardt, G. Signature whistle variations in a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1994, 35, 243–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Sekiguchi, Y.; Kohshima, S. Resting behaviors of captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Physiol. Behav. 2003, 79, 643–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Lopez Marulanda, J.; Adam, O.; Delfour, F. Modulation of whistle production related to training sessions in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) under human care. Zoo Biol. 2016, 35, 495–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Fish, M.P.; Mowbray, W.H. Production of Underwater Sound by the White Whale or Beluga, Delphinapterus Leucas (Pallus) (No. TR17R62 5); Rhode Island University Kingston Narragansett Marine Lab: Narragansett, RI, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
  77. Brickman, J.J. Factors Influencing Whistle Usage of the Pacific White-Sided Dolphin, Lagenorhynchus Obliquidens, at the John G. Shedd Aquarium. Ph.D. Thesis, Western Illinois University, Macomb, IL, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  78. Miller, P.J.O.; Johnson, M.P.; Tyack, P.L. Sperm whale behaviour indicates the use of echolocation click buzzes ‘creaks’ in prey capture. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2004, 271, 2239–2247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Wahlberg, M.; Jensen, F.H.; Aguilar Soto, N.; Beedholm, K.; Bejder, L.; Oliveira, C.; Rasmussen, M.; Simon, M.; Villadsgaard, A.; Madsen, P.T. Source parameters of echolocation clicks from wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus and Tursiops truncatus). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 130, 2263–2274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  80. Pirotta, E.; Thompson, P.M.; Miller, P.I.; Brookes, K.L.; Cheney, B.; Barton, T.R.; Graham, I.M.; Lusseau, D. Scale-dependent foraging ecology of a marine top predator modelled using passive acoustic data. Funct. Ecol. 2014, 28, 206–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Ellison, W.; Southall, B.; Clark, C.; Frankel, A. A new context-based approach to assess marine mammal behavioral responses to anthropogenic sounds. Conserv. Biol. 2011, 26, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Pirotta, E.; Merchant, N.D.; Thompson, P.M.; Barton, T.R.; Lusseau, D. Quantifying the effect of boat disturbance on bottlenose dolphin foraging activity. Biol. Conserv. 2015, 181, 82–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Buckstaff, K.C. Effects of watercraft noise on the acoustic behavior of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, in Sarasota Bay, Florida. Mar. Mammal Sci. 2004, 20, 709–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. David, J. Likely sensitivity of bottlenose dolphins to pile-driving noise. Water Environ. J. 2006, 20, 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Serres, A. The frequency of solitary behaviours in captive odontocetes is modulated by environmental and social factors. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2019, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Serres, A.; Hao, Y.; Wang, D. Swimming features in captive odontocetes: Indicative of animals’ emotional state? Behav. Process. 2020, 170, 103998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Serres, A.; Hao, Y.; Wang, D. Body Contacts and Social Interactions in Captive Odontocetes Are Influenced by the Context: An Implication for Welfare Assessment. Animals 2020, 10, 924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Tavolga, W.N. Theoretical principles for the study of communication in cetaceans. Mammalia 1983, 47, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Johnson, C. Animal communication via coordinated cognitive systems. Perspect. Ethol. 1993, 10, 187–207. [Google Scholar]
  90. Herzing, D.L. Vocalizations and associated underwater behavior of free-ranging Atlantic spotted dolphins, Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Aquat. Mamm. 1996, 22, 61–80. [Google Scholar]
  91. Busnel, R.G.; Dziedzic, A. Acoustic signals of the pilot whale Globicephala melaena and of the porpoises Delphinus delphis and Phocoena phocoena. In Whales, Dolphins and Porpoises; University of California Press: Oakland, CA, USA, 1966; pp. 607–646. [Google Scholar]
  92. Dreher, J.J.; Evans, W.E. Cetacean communication. Mar. Bioacoust. 1964, 1, 373–393. [Google Scholar]
  93. Acevedo-Gutiérrez, A.; Stienessen, S.C. Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) increase number of whistles when feeding. Aquat. Mamm. 2004, 30, 357–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Dos Santos, M.E.D.; Louro, S.; Couchinho, M.N.; Brito, C.M. Whistles of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Sado Estuary, Portugal: Characteristics, production rates, and long-term contour stability. Aquat. Mamm. 2005, 31, 453–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Akamatsu, T.; Hatakeyama, Y.; Kojima, T.; Soeda, H. Echolocation rates of two harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). Mar. Mammal Sci. 1994, 10, 401–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Design of the housing pools and position of the Soundtrap in both pools.
Figure 1. Design of the housing pools and position of the Soundtrap in both pools.
Animals 11 00511 g001
Figure 2. Click density average and 95% confidence intervals depending on the number of porpoises present in the pool (a) the season (b) the social grouping (c), the presence of enrichment (d) the time relative to training (e) the occurrence of unusual events (f) and the presence of visitors (g) *: p < 0.05, ***: p < 0.001; when pairwise tests were conducted within a context variable, click densities that share the same letter (a or b) do not differ significantly, whereas click densities that have no letter in common differ significantly (Wald chi-squared test with a sequential Bonferroni correction). N: number of one-minute samples. The presented data are not raw, but have been extracted from the generalized linear mixed-effects model.
Figure 2. Click density average and 95% confidence intervals depending on the number of porpoises present in the pool (a) the season (b) the social grouping (c), the presence of enrichment (d) the time relative to training (e) the occurrence of unusual events (f) and the presence of visitors (g) *: p < 0.05, ***: p < 0.001; when pairwise tests were conducted within a context variable, click densities that share the same letter (a or b) do not differ significantly, whereas click densities that have no letter in common differ significantly (Wald chi-squared test with a sequential Bonferroni correction). N: number of one-minute samples. The presented data are not raw, but have been extracted from the generalized linear mixed-effects model.
Animals 11 00511 g002
Table 1. Catalog of studied individuals’ features.
Table 1. Catalog of studied individuals’ features.
NameSexAge (year)Length (m/h)
DuoduoM8157
F7 *F8145
F9 *F8145
TaotaoM14156
Yangyang *F11147
* pregnant females that gave birth during the data collection.
Table 2. Context variables.
Table 2. Context variables.
SeasonTime of DayTime Relative to Training *Social GroupingEnrichmentUnusual EventsVisitors
Winter (December–February)Morning (8 am–11 pm)Outside of trainingTogetherNoneNoneNone
Spring (March–May)Noon (11 pm–2 pm)During trainingSeparatedToys aPool cleaning dFew h
Summer (June–August)Afternoon (2 pm–5 pm) Humans bNoise eMany i
Fall (September–November) Humans and toysSocial event f
Live fishOther event g
New object c
* Training: training or feeding session. a Balls. b Caretakers or other humans interacting with porpoises from the surface or from underwater windows. c Stretcher, material used for new experiments, new toys. d Diver and/or caretaker scrubbing the surface using long-handle brushes. e Construction work or people having meetings next to the pool to prepare events such as transport of animals. f Right after separation or reunion of groups (or separation attempts for BDs). g Shoal of small fish in the pool, unusual water level. h <5 persons next to the pool: employees or visitors. i >5 persons next to the pool: visitors.
Table 3. Model characteristics (a) and statistical outputs (b) from the generalized linear mixed-effects model.
Table 3. Model characteristics (a) and statistical outputs (b) from the generalized linear mixed-effects model.
(a)Model ComponentsModel CharacteristicsScaled Residuals
Response VariableOffsetRandomPredictorsAICBIClogLikDeviancedf.residMin1QMedian3QMax
factors
Selected modelClick densityNumber of individualsDate,Number of individuals,107,583.40107,717.10−53,771.7107,543.405878−4.1621−0.00650.00190.00931.0357
Observation session IDSeason,
Time relative to training,
Social grouping,
Presence of toys,
Presence of humans,
Presence of humans and toys,
Presence of new objects,
Unusual events,
Presence of visitors
(b)Number of individualsSeasonSocial groupingPresence of toysPresence of humansPresence of humans and toysPresence of new objectsTime relative to trainingUnusual eventsPresence of public
Selected modelχ2 = 48.687, df = 1, p < 0.0001χ2 = 17.076, df = 3, p = 0.0007χ2 = 18.709, df = 1, p < 0.0001χ2 = 5.256, df = 1, p = 0.0218χ2 = 6.580, df = 1, p = 0.0103χ2 = 13.205, df = 1, p = 0.0003χ2 = 30.067, df = 1, p < 0.0001χ2 = 617.822, df = 1, p < 0.0001χ2 = 8.915, df = 4, p = 0.0634χ2 = 40.891, df = 2, p < 0.0001
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Serres, A.; Xu, C.; Hao, Y.; Wang, D. The Click Production of Captive Yangtze Finless Porpoises (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaorientalis) Is Influenced by Social and Environmental Factors. Animals 2021, 11, 511. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11020511

AMA Style

Serres A, Xu C, Hao Y, Wang D. The Click Production of Captive Yangtze Finless Porpoises (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaorientalis) Is Influenced by Social and Environmental Factors. Animals. 2021; 11(2):511. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11020511

Chicago/Turabian Style

Serres, Agathe, Chen Xu, Yujiang Hao, and Ding Wang. 2021. "The Click Production of Captive Yangtze Finless Porpoises (Neophocaena asiaeorientalis asiaorientalis) Is Influenced by Social and Environmental Factors" Animals 11, no. 2: 511. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11020511

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop