Next Article in Journal
A New Look at Cultivar Preference in Hoplocampa testudinea (Hymenoptera: Tenthredinidae) on Apple in the Annapolis Valley of Nova Scotia, Canada
Next Article in Special Issue
Synaptic Interactions in Scorpion Peg Sensilla Appear to Maintain Chemosensory Neurons within Dynamic Firing Range
Previous Article in Journal
MtDNA Analysis Indicates Human-Induced Temporal Changes of Serbian Honey Bees Diversity
Previous Article in Special Issue
Transcriptome Profiling of Starvation in the Peripheral Chemosensory Organs of the Crop Pest Spodoptera littoralis Caterpillars
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Comparing the Appetitive Learning Performance of Six European Honeybee Subspecies in a Common Apiary

1
Behavioral Physiology & Sociobiology, Biocenter, University of Würzburg, Am Hubland, 97074 Würzburg, Germany
2
Laboratory of Insect Biosystematics, Department of Agricultural Biotechnology, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Korea
3
Landesbetrieb Landwirtschaft Hessen, Bee Institute Kirchhain, Erlenstraße 9, 35274 Kirchhain, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Insects 2021, 12(9), 768; https://doi.org/10.3390/insects12090768
Submission received: 16 July 2021 / Revised: 15 August 2021 / Accepted: 18 August 2021 / Published: 27 August 2021
(This article belongs to the Collection Insect Senses: From Perception to Cognition)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

This study is the first to compare the associative learning performance of six honeybee subspecies from different European regions in a common apiary. We quantified sucrose responsiveness prior to appetitive olfactory proboscis extension learning to dissociate effects of motivation and cognition. Our results show that Apis mellifera iberiensis displayed a significantly poorer learning performance compared to other Apis subspecies from across Europe, which did not differ from each other. Possible causes are discussed.

Abstract

The Western honeybee (Apis mellifera L.) is one of the most widespread insects with numerous subspecies in its native range. How far adaptation to local habitats has affected the cognitive skills of the different subspecies is an intriguing question that we investigate in this study. Naturally mated queens of the following five subspecies from different parts of Europe were transferred to Southern Germany: A. m. iberiensis from Portugal, A. m. mellifera from Belgium, A. m. macedonica from Greece, A. m. ligustica from Italy, and A. m. ruttneri from Malta. We also included the local subspecies A. m. carnica in our study. New colonies were built up in a common apiary where the respective queens were introduced. Worker offspring from the different subspecies were compared in classical olfactory learning performance using the proboscis extension response. Prior to conditioning, we measured individual sucrose responsiveness to investigate whether possible differences in learning performances were due to differential responsiveness to the sugar water reward. Most subspecies did not differ in their appetitive learning performance. However, foragers of the Iberian honeybee, A. m. iberiensis, performed significantly more poorly, despite having a similar sucrose responsiveness. We discuss possible causes for the poor performance of the Iberian honeybees, which may have been shaped by adaptation to the local habitat.

1. Introduction

The natural range of the Western honeybee (A. mellifera) expands throughout Africa, Europe, Western and Central Asia to Western China in the East [1,2,3,4,5,6,7]. The intraspecific diversity of Apis mellifera is remarkable, with currently about 30 subspecies described [1,7,8,9,10]. The different subspecies display diverse adaptations to a wide variety of geographic areas and environmental factors. They can be grouped into five evolutionary lineages (A, M, C, O, and Y), based on morphometric and molecular studies [7,8,9,11]. Whereas the subspecies of lineage A are spread across Africa, those belonging to lineage Y originate in North-Eastern Africa. Subspecies of lineage M are distributed widely in Western and Northern Europe, while lineage C comprises subspecies originating from South-Eastern Europe. Lineage O stems from the Near and Middle East. However, the native distribution of the different subspecies has been altered gravely by human interference and the current situation no longer represents the original distribution [5,9,12]. The worldwide demand for highly profitable honeybee colonies and the breeding efforts focusing on certain behavioral traits such as low aggressiveness has promoted the introduction of subspecies to locations outside their natural range, causing accidental and deliberate hybridization and leading to the jeopardization of native populations of A. mellifera subspecies.
Selection has not only shaped the morphology of the different subspecies but also behavioral traits such as defensiveness (e.g., breeding for low aggressiveness) and annual colony development cycles (e.g., early spring development in the economically popular A. m. carnica [2,3,5,7,13,14,15]). However, huge behavioral differences have not only been observed between different subspecies of honeybees, but also among members of the same colony. A good example is individual responsiveness to sucrose, which has frequently been employed as a general indicator of the physiological state of a honeybee [16,17,18]. It differs grossly between individuals performing different social tasks [19,20,21,22,23,24], and between seasons [25]. Importantly, it allows us to make predictions about the appetitive learning performance of the individual, because it correlates positively with cognitive performance in appetitive associative learning [23,24,25,26,27]. The more responsive a honeybee is to sucrose, the higher is her learning score, i.e., the better is her cognitive performance. So far, all of these experiments have been performed with workers of two subspecies of the Western honeybee, i.e., A. m. carnica and A. m. ligustica. Whether this relationship between cognitive performance and sensory responsiveness to sucrose is universal for different honeybee subspecies is an intriguing question. Similarly, it is an exciting open question whether different honeybee subspecies share similar cognitive capacities.
We here studied for the first time the appetitive olfactory learning performance in different subspecies of Apis mellifera maintained in a common garden apiary. We performed the experiments with colonies of six different European subspecies of A. mellifera, covering three of the five known evolutionary lineages: A. m. mellifera and A. m. iberiensis of lineage M, A. m. carnica, A. m. macedonica, and A. m. ligustica from lineage C, and A. m. ruttneri from lineage A (Figure 1). We hypothesized that the different subspecies should only differ in their appetitive learning performance if they differed in their responsiveness to sucrose, because this factor is an important determinant of learning performance [16,19,23,24,27]. Sucrose responsiveness might be shaped by local adaptation to climate and habitat. Secondly, we aimed to investigate whether the correlation between individual sucrose responsiveness and appetitive learning performance described for A. m. carnica and A. m. ligustica is also present in the other subspecies, pointing towards a general rule for appetitive associative learning.
The Carniolan bee A. m. carnica (Figure 1), a representative of lineage C, is native to South-Eastern Austria and the North-Western Balkan Peninsula. It is currently one of the most frequently used subspecies in commercial beekeeping and bee science worldwide. From the late 19th century on, hives have been exported from the Westernmost tip of its native distribution area to various destinations [7]. The “grey Carniolan bee” has become very popular in Germany, because of its gentle temperament and low swarming tendency, important characteristics for beekeeping. Nowadays, the Carniolan bee, A. m. ligustica, and the commercial hybrid Buckfast have by far the widest distribution area, often threatening other subspecies.
The A. m. iberiensis bees used in our experiment came from Bragança in Northern Portugal (Figure 1). The natural distribution of this subspecies covers the whole Iberian Peninsula and the Balearic Islands [7,14,28,29], where it partially overlaps with an imported commercial stock of other subspecies [5]. Ruttner [7] describes different ecotypes adapted to both cold and warm climate conditions. Brood rearing is highly economical without wasting resources and has good overall potential for applied beekeeping. This subspecies is further known for using high quantities of propolis, displaying a nervous behavior on the combs, and being ferociously defensive [7,15].
The original distribution of A. m. mellifera (lineage M) extended throughout central Europe North of the Alps including the United Kingdom, Ireland, and Scandinavia in the North, all over France in the West, and across Poland to the Ural mountain range in the East [7]. Today, it has been replaced by other subspecies in large parts of its former distribution area [5,13,30,31,32]. This includes wide parts of Germany, where Carniolan stock (A. m. carnica) is now predominant. Various ecotypes have been described which share a brooding rhythm adapted to nectar flow, ample use of propolis, and a nervous behavior on the combs with a tendency to defensiveness [7,15]. Our A. m. mellifera bees stem from a breeding apiary in Belgium (Figure 1).
The subspecies Apis mellifera ruttneri (lineage A) is endemic to the archipelago of Malta, which constitutes its former and actual range of distribution (Figure 1; [14,33]). Due to its small area of distribution and frequent imports of other commercially used subspecies, A. m. ruttneri is highly prone to genetic introgression [14,33]. Its brood cycle is adjusted to seasonal nectar flows and xeric conditions, while colonies show a moderate use of propolis and sometimes fierce defensive behavior [33]. While the distinct defensive behavior, especially under hot and dry weather conditions, is an unfavorable trait for their use in apiculture, A. m. ruttneri can cope with predatory wasps and the challenging seasons of the respective habitat [33].
The natural distribution range of A. m. macedonica (lineage C) extends from Northern Greece across North Macedonia, Bulgaria, Romania, and Moldova to Southern Ukraine in the North [2,7]. Ruttner [7] describes A. m. macedonica as gentle but sometimes inclined to swarm and susceptible to Nosemosis. He also reports ample use of propolis and brood reduction as a reaction to unsuitable (especially hot and dry) weather conditions. Our bees were derived from Northern Greece (Figure 1).
The original distribution range of A. m. ligustica (lineage C) covers the Apennine Peninsula [7]. In recent times, however, A. m. ligustica has been spread around the globe for its beekeeping value. Apart from A. m. carnica, this subspecies is by far the most commonly used in commercial apiculture [5,13]. Beekeepers appreciate its high fertility, gentleness, little use of propolis, and high productivity under good foraging conditions [7,15]. Under poor foraging conditions or in cold climates, however, colonies of A. m. ligustica do not adjust their brood rearing: a trait resulting in lower honey yields for beekeepers and/or starvation of colonies through accelerated store consumption [7,15]. Our A. m. ligustica bees originated in Northern Italy (Figure 1).
Here we asked whether and in how far selection for traits important for commercial beekeeping and natural adaptation to local climate and habitat have shaped sensory and cognitive capacities of the different honeybee subspecies.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Bees and Hive Management

Experiments were conducted in the summer of 2018 at the University of Würzburg (Bavaria, Germany). The experimental bees were derived from 23 queen-right colonies of six different subspecies of the Western honeybee (Apis mellifera). The colonies were headed by open mated queens from A. m. carnica, A. m. ligustica, A. m. macedonica, A. m. mellifera, A. m. ruttneri, or A. m. iberiensis (n = 3–4 resp.). The queens were derived from populations central to the current distribution area of their respective subspecies (Figure 1) and introduced into shook swarms of A. m. carnica at least ten weeks prior to data collection. All queens had been shipped according to valid veterinary legislation and were registered in the TRACES-Database. To prevent genetic pollution of the local honeybee population, all hives were equipped with excluder grids at the entrance, allowing only worker bees to fly freely. In addition, all drone brood was removed from the colonies, and the tip of one wing of each queen was clipped following the protocol of Human et al. [34]. Hive stands were grouped according to subspecies to avoid drifting of homecoming forager bees between subspecies. For the same reason, only one pair of two differently colored hives from one subspecies was placed on each hive stand with opposite flying directions (Figure 2). Despite the occurrence of some drought periods, the foraging conditions were comparatively good throughout the experimental period, with rich floral nectar flows and honeydew.

2.2. Harnessing of Bees

Returning foragers were collected at the hive entrance. Only foragers without pollen in their corbiculae were selected, assuming that they were nectar foragers. All bees were caught between morning and midday. Bees were caught individually in small glass vials, they were immobilized on ice and mounted in small brass tubes according to the standard protocol of Scheiner et al. [16]. Mounted bees were fed with 5 μL of a 30% sucrose solution (according to Matsumoto et al. [35]) and placed in a dark incubator (temperature 35 °C, relative humidity 70%) for one hour to recover from anesthesia and for sufficient motivation to learn [19,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,36].

2.3. Sucrose Responsiveness

Sucrose responsiveness was tested prior to training as described elsewhere [16,22,23,36,37]. Briefly, both antennae of each bee were sequentially stimulated with water and a series of sucrose concentrations (0.1%, 0.3%, 1.0%, 3.0%, 10%, 30% w/v) in ascending order. The inter-trial interval was two min to prevent intrinsic sensitization [36]. It was recorded which sucrose concentration elicited the proboscis extension response (PER) for each bee. The total number of proboscis extension responses is the gustatory response score (GRS) of a bee [16].

2.4. Appetitive Olfactory Learning and Memory Tests

To quantify associative olfactory learning we used a standard protocol for classical conditioning with one odor described elsewhere [16,21]. Bees are required to associate the odor with a sugar-water reward. Only bees not displaying a spontaneous response to the conditioned odor were used. Bees were trained with 30% sucrose solution as unconditioned stimulus and reward and 5 µL of 1-hexanol (Sigma Aldrich, Steinheim, Germany) as the conditioned stimulus. During training, each bee was placed in a constant air stream. We delivered 5 mL of the odor/air mixture manually to the antennae of each bee. The bee experienced the odor for 1 s before the PER was elicited by touching the antennae with a 30% sucrose droplet. As soon as the bee extended her proboscis, she was fed with a small droplet of sucrose solution for 1 s. The odor was removed approximately 0.5 s after the onset of feeding, so that conditioned odor and sucrose reward overlapped in time. The inter-trial interval was 5 min. During each training trial, we recorded whether the bee displayed the conditioned PER before this response was elicited by applying sucrose to the antennae [16,21]. The total number of conditioned PERs of a bee during the 6 training trials constitutes her acquisition or learning score [16,37]. The following number of bees were conditioned: ncarnica: 86, niberiensis: 56, nmellifera: 71; nruttneri: 74; nmacedonica: 45; nligustica: 18.

2.5. Statistics

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics 26 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). The GRS and acquisition scores were tested for normal distribution using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Since both scores were not distributed normally within each subspecies, non-parametric Kruskal Wallis H tests were performed with post hoc tests using p values corrected for multiple comparisons. To test for effects of the colony on acquisition scores we also performed Kruskal–Wallis H tests within each subspecies. The number of bees showing the correct response during the acquisition phase (i.e., acquisition curves) was analyzed with generalized estimation equations (GEE) using the binary responses in each acquisition trial as dependent variable and subspecies and GRS as factors. Sidak tests were used as post hoc tests. Correlation analyses between GRS and acquisition scores were performed using Spearman rank correlation.

3. Results

Gustatory response scores (GRS) of bees trained to 1-hexanol were overall very high (Figure 3A) and did not differ between subspecies (p > 0.05, Kruskal–Wallis H Test), which indicated high responsiveness to sucrose. Based on earlier experiments with A. m. carnica and A. m. ligustica we, therefore, expected high acquisition scores in all subspecies. We pooled data from foragers from different hives within each subspecies, because the colony did not have an effect on acquisition score within each subspecies (p > 0.05, Kruskal–Wallis H test).
Comparison of acquisition scores yielded a significant effect of subspecies on learning performance (Figure 3B; p < 0.01, Kruskal–Wallis H Test). Foragers of the A. m. iberiensis subspecies displayed significantly lower acquisition scores than those of the A. m. carnica and A. m. ruttneri subspecies (p < 0.01; Table S2). For a more detailed analysis of learning behavior, we compared the learning curves of the different subspecies and included GRS as a within-subject factor in the model. Subspecies had a significant effect on learning curves (Figure 3C; χ2(5,349) = 12.75, p < 0.05, GEE). A. m. iberiensis foragers performed significantly more poorly than A. m. carnica foragers (p < 0.01; Table S3) and A. m. ruttneri foragers (p < 0.01, Table S3), while all of the other subspecies did not differ in their learning curve. GRS had a large and significant effect on learning curves (χ2(6,349) = 36.87; p < 0.001). Bees with higher GRS performed better than bees with lower GRS across subspecies. We further tested for a correlation between GRS and acquisition scores within each subspecies.
GRS correlated with acquisition scores significantly positively in the subspecies A. m. carnica (ρ = 0.34, p < 0.001), A. m. mellifera (ρ = 0.40, p < 0.001) and A. m. ruttneri (ρ = 0.32, p < 0.01) but not in the other subspecies (Figure 3D; p > 0.05, Spearman rank correlations). However, the same trend is observable in the other subspecies and it appears that the unequal distribution of GRS and partially a low sample size are related to the absence of a significant correlation.

4. Discussion

This is the first study comparing the cognitive abilities of six different Apis mellifera subspecies from across Europe under standardized conditions in a common apiary. The bees tested for their appetitive olfactory learning performance were all highly motivated, i.e., displaying a similarly high sucrose responsiveness, which is an indicator of their “learning motivation” and can predict learning performance to a high degree [19,24,27,38]. If the correlation between GRS and acquisition scores frequently demonstrated in A. m. carnica [24,27] and A. m. ligustica [19,23] were also present in the other honeybee subspecies, there should be few differences in the learning performance of the different subspecies, since the majority of differences in appetitive olfactory learning can be attributed to differences in the sucrose responsiveness of the bees [27]. In fact, most of the European honeybee subspecies we analyzed did not differ in their learning performance from each other, supporting this hypothesis. However, A. m. iberiensis, a subspecies native to Southern Europe, performed significantly more poorly in our classical olfactory learning paradigm compared to A. m. carnica and A. m. ruttneri and displayed a non-significant tendency to perform less well than the other subspecies. This led us to question the general nature of the correlation between GRS and acquisition scores in a simple classical olfactory conditioning assay. We found a significant and expected positive correlation in A. m. carnica, A. m. mellifera, and A. m. ruttneri. The reason why we did not see this correlation in A. m. ligustica might be due to the lower sample size in this experiment, but we nevertheless decided to include those data for comparison, and earlier experiments demonstrated a significant positive correlation between GRS and acquisition scores in this subspecies, too [19,23]. In fact, the trend for a positive correlation between GRS and acquisition scores is observable in most subspecies tested (Figure 3D), but the distribution of GRS was sometimes suboptimal for correlation analyses because most bees were highly responsive. Here, further experiments with larger samples sizes within each subspecies can help to ultimately show this correlation in all subspecies.
Learning differences between different strains of honeybees (A. m. ligustica) selected for high or low amounts of stored pollen [39], for example, could be explained by a different sucrose responsiveness, which correlated with the probability to collect pollen or nectar [19]. However, sucrose responsiveness did not differ between subspecies in the current experiment, and we only tested nectar foragers (Figure 3A). Thus we can largely exclude the possibility that A. m. iberiensis foragers were simply “less motivated” to learn compared to the A. m. carnica or A. m. ruttneri foragers. Therefore, the lower learning performance of this subspecies might be related to other factors including genetic differences related to foraging behavior. The differences in the learning performance of A. m. iberiensis foragers and foragers of the other subspecies appear not to be linked to the lineage of this subspecies, because A. m. mellifera, which is the second representative of lineage M, performs as well as the other subspecies.
It is conceivable that a differential adaptation to temperature-related stress may be linked to different learning performances. Iberian bees might have a higher energy demand to cope with heat stress and/or higher foraging activity compared to honeybees from Central Europe, similar to what has been suggested by Iqbal et al. [40] for Arabian honeybees. A greater foraging activity, in turn, leads to the accumulation of oxidative stress [41] and reduces learning performance [42,43]. However, A. m. ruttneri faces similar climatic stress because of high temperatures. Nevertheless, it performed significantly better than A. m. iberiensis. It is, therefore, more likely that a combination of factors contributes to the lower learning performance of A. m. iberiensis compared to A. m. carnica and A. m. ruttneri, possibly including a differential responsiveness to odors, which could further contribute to their lower learning performance, as discussed elsewhere [27].
Differential foraging strategies and related learning performance might be a further reason for a differential performance of A. m. carnica and A. m. iberiensis. Diverse mechanisms involving learning appear to be important in decision-making of individual foragers (e.g., [44]). Pérez-Claudio et al. [45] discussed the lower learning ability of A. m. syriaca compared to A. m. caucasia in reverse association task in light of a higher predation rate in their native habitat. This might favor a risk-minimizing foraging strategy with higher floral fidelity. Conversely, A. m. caucasia showed higher flexibility in a reverse association learning task, consistent with a previously described lower floral fidelity [10] and a lower predation rate in their foraging range [45]. Whether the foraging strategy of A. m. carnica, A. m. ruttneri, and the other subspecies differ from that of A. m. iberiensis is currently being investigated in our lab.
In an olfactory PER learning experiment comparing the performance of the Africanized honeybees in North America (Apis mellifera scutellata hybrid) with that of honeybees of European origin (A. m. carnica), the former performed significantly more poorly [46]. The authors suggested the hypothesis that the Africanized honeybee might have been ecologically more successful than the Western honeybee, which might have been related to their lower learning performance. The authors suggest that if learning did not induce additional costs, there should be universal selection for high learning performance [46]. However, a high degree of variation is maintained in natural populations of insects [47], suggesting that learning could theoretically incur a fitness cost [48,49,50]. Experiments in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster demonstrate an evolutionary trade-off between learning ability and competitive ability, supporting the hypothesis that selection for improved learning is consistently linked to a decreased competitive ability for limited food resources in larvae [51]. A similar trade-off between improved learning performance and successful, aggressive strategies is conceivable for the honeybee. Africanized honeybees are notorious for their highly defensive behavior. Similarly, the Iberian honeybee is typically more defensive than the Carniolan honeybee [7,15], which was also apparent in our apiary, where all of the subspecies were hosted under equal climatic conditions.
A recent study comparing the learning performance of A. m. carnica, A. m. ligustica, and A. m. jemenitica showed that similar to our findings, the former two did not differ in their PER learning performance, whereas A. m. jemenitica performed less well [40]. In their experiments, the smaller body size of A. m. jemenitica was considered a possible reason for poorer learning performance, based on studies of body size and learning performance in bumblebees [52]. However, it is debatable whether brain size is an appropriate indicator of learning behavior because there are different outcomes of experiments trying to link this factor with behavioral repertoires and cognitive abilities in animals [46,53,54]. Further, it would not explain our differences between A. m. iberiensis and A. m. carnica, since they have a similar size [7]. In addition, A. m. ruttneri is slightly smaller than either A. m. carnica or A. m. iberiensis. If size did matter, we would expect learning differences here, too.
An intriguing hypothesis that awaits further investigation is that A. m. iberiensis bees differ from A. m. carnica bees, A. m. ruttneri bees and the other subspecies tested in their amount of neurotransmitters in the brain. A higher baseline brain titer of the biogenic amine octopamine, which itself is involved in the mediation of the reward in appetitive PER learning [55], might be responsible for better learning performance. In support of this hypothesis, we could show that bees performing different social tasks (i.e., nurse bees vs. foragers or pollen collectors vs. nectar collectors), do not only differ in their titers of octopamine and its metabolic precursor tyramine [20,21,22] but also in their appetitive learning capability [21,22,24]. The appetitive learning performance of honeybees can be improved by treating the bees with octopamine [56] or tyramine [21]. Differential octopamine brain titers may be related to different foraging strategies of the different races and also to aggression. This interesting neuroecological question awaits further study. In addition, other neuromodulators such as neuropeptide F might be involved in the learning differences between A. m. iberiensis and the other subspecies. Studies in fruit flies show that artificial activation of neuropeptide F neurons inhibits appetitive olfactory learning by modulating the sugar reward signal during acquisition [57]. Future experiments should correlate the activity and amount of this peptide in good and poor learners of the different subspecies to test for a similar function of this peptide in honeybees. Further, A. m. iberiensis foragers may have a reduced size of the mushroom bodies, important brain centers involved in learning and multimodal processing, or may have fewer synapses in their calyces of the mushroom bodies, leading to reduced appetitive learning performance. This question has to be studied in future experiments.

5. Conclusions

The most significant finding of our study is that differences in cognitive abilities are part of the intraspecific diversity in A. mellifera, similar to what has been demonstrated for other behavioral traits [2,3,58,59,60]. While most of the subspecies tested here were very similar in associative learning capacities and the correlation between sucrose responsiveness and appetitive learning performance, the Iberian honeybee surprised with a reduced learning performance, independent of the main motivational factor sucrose responsiveness. It could be linked to different foraging strategies, the higher aggressiveness of the Iberian bees, different amounts of neurotransmitters in the brain, different sizes of brain neuropils important for learning, or to genetic differences, all of which might have been shaped by ecological factors. It is an exciting open question how the neuroecology of foraging behavior and learning might thus be interlinked and shaped by adaptation to local climate and habitat.

Supplementary Materials

https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/insects12090768/s1, Table S1: Number of bees in each GRS class of each subspecies, Table S2: Comparison of acquisition scores. Test statistics for Kruskal–Wallis H tests (KW) and p values corrected for multiple comparisons are given, Table S3: Comparison of acquisition curves. Test statistics for Sidak post hoc tests are given.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.S., M.S.G. and M.D.M.; methodology, R.S., M.S.G. and M.D.M.; formal analysis, R.S.; data curation, K.L.; writing—original draft preparation, R.S.; writing—review and editing, M.D.M., M.S.G. and K.L.; visualization, R.S. and M.S.G.; supervision, R.S.; project administration, R.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

We did not receive any funding for this study.

Data Availability Statement

Data will be uploaded to Dryad.

Acknowledgments

We thank our colleagues Dirk Ahrens, Cecilia Costa, Fani Hatjina, Dylan Elen, Thomas Galea, Alice Pinto, and Léon Quiévy who provided queens of the different subspecies as well as their invaluable knowledge of the relevant biology. We also gratefully acknowledge the collaboration with Aleksandar Uzunov, who provided important comments on the study setup. Furthermore, we would like to thank Stefanie Schmidt and Wiebke Schäfer for their help in data collection and comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Chen, C.; Liu, Z.; Pan, Q.; Chen, X.; Wang, H.; Guo, H.; Liu, S.; Lu, H.; Tian, S.; Li, R.; et al. Genomic analyses reveal demographic history and temperate adaptation of the newly discovered honey bee subspecies Apis mellifera sinisxinyuan n. ssp. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2016, 33, 1337–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Uzunov, A.; Meixner, M.D.; Kiprijanovska, H.; Andonov, S.; Gregorc, A.; Ivanova, E.; Bouga, M.; Dobi, P.; Büchler, R.; Francis, R.; et al. Genetic structure of Apis mellifera macedonica in the Balkan Peninsula based on microsatellite DNA polymorphism. J. Apicult. Res. 2014, 53, 288–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Uzunov, A.; Costa, C.; Panasiuk, B.; Meixner, M.; Kryger, P.; Hatjina, F.; Bouga, M.; Andonov, S.; Bienkowska, M.; Le Conte, Y.; et al. Swarming, defensive and hygienic behaviour in honey bee colonies of different genetic origin in a pan-European experiment. J. Apicult. Res. 2014, 53, 248–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Sheppard, W.S.; Meixner, M.D. Apis mellifera pomonella, a new honey bee subspecies from Central Asia. Apidologie 2003, 34, 367–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Meixner, M.D.; Costa, C.; Kryger, P.; Hatjina, F.; Bouga, M.; Ivanova, E.; Büchler, R. Conserving diversity and vitality for honey bee breeding. J. Apicult. Res. 2010, 49, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Ruttner, F.; Tassencourt, L.; Louveaux, J. Biometrical-statistical analysis of the geographic variability of Apis mellifera L. I. Material and Methods. Apidologie 1978, 9, 363–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Ruttner, F. Biogeography and Taxonomy of Honeybees; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1988; ISBN 3540177817. [Google Scholar]
  8. Han, F.; Wallberg, A.; Webster, M.T. From where did the Western honeybee (Apis mellifera) originate? Ecol. Evol. 2012, 2, 1949–1957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Bouga, M.; Alaux, C.; Bienkowska, M.; Büchler, R.; Carreck, N.L.; Cauia, E.; Chlebo, R.; Dahle, B.; Dall’Olio, R.; de La Rúa, P.; et al. A review of methods for discrimination of honey bee populations as applied to European beekeeping. J. Apicult. Res. 2011, 50, 51–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Cakmak, I.; Song, D.S.; Mixson, T.A.; Serrano, E.; Clement, M.L.; Savitski, A.; Johnson, G.A.; Giray, T.; Abramson, C.I.; Barthell, J.F.; et al. Foraging response of Turkish honey bee subspecies to flower color choices and reward consistency. J. Insect Behav. 2010, 23, 100–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Moritz, R.F.A.; Kraus, F.B.; Kryger, P.; Crewe, R.M. The size of wild honeybee populations (Apis mellifera) and its implications for the conservation of honeybees. J. Insect Conserv. 2007, 11, 391–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Meixner, M.D.; Worobik, M.; Wilde, J.; Fuchs, S.; Koeniger, N. Apis mellifera mellifera in eastern Europe—Morphometric variation and determination of its range limits. Apidologie 2007, 38, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. De La Rúa, P.; Jaffé, R.; Dall’Olio, R.; Muñoz, I.; Serrano, J. Biodiversity, conservation and current threats to European honeybees. Apidologie 2009, 40, 263–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Zammit-Mangion, M.; Meixner, M.; Mifsud, D.; Sammut, S.; Camilleri, L. Thorough morphological and genetic evidence confirm the existence of the endemic honey bee of the Maltese Islands Apis mellifera ruttneri: Recommendations for conservation. J. Apicult. Res. 2017, 56, 514–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Adam, B. In Search of the Best Strains of Bees and the Results of the Evaluations of the Crosses and Races; Peacock Press: Garland, TX, USA, 1983; ISBN 0907908063. [Google Scholar]
  16. Scheiner, R.; Abramson, C.I.; Brodschneider, R.; Crailsheim, K.; Farina, W.M.; Fuchs, S.; Grünewald, B.; Hahshold, S.; Karrer, M.; Koeniger, G.; et al. Standard methods for behavioural studies of Apis mellifera. J. Apicult. Res. 2013, 52, 1–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Scheiner, R.; Erber, J. Sensory thresholds, learning and the division of foraging labor in the honey bee. In Organization of Insect Societies: From Genomes to Socio-Complexity; Gadau, J., Fewell, J., Eds.; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2009; pp. 335–356. [Google Scholar]
  18. Scheiner, R.; Page, R.E.; Erber, J. Sucrose responsiveness and behavioral plasticity in honey bees (Apis mellifera). Apidologie 2004, 35, 133–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Scheiner, R.; Page, R.E.; Erber, J. The effects of genotype, foraging role, and sucrose responsiveness on the tactile learning performance of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2001, 76, 138–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Reim, T.; Scheiner, R. Division of labour in honey bees: Age- and task-related changes in the expression of octopamine receptor genes. Insect Mol. Biol. 2014, 23, 833–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Scheiner, R.; Reim, T.; Søvik, E.; Entler, B.V.; Barron, A.B.; Thamm, M. Learning, gustatory responsiveness and tyramine differences across nurse and forager honeybees. J. Exp. Biol. 2017, 220, 1443–1450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Scheiner, R.; Entler, B.V.; Barron, A.B.; Scholl, C.; Thamm, M. The effects of fat body tyramine level on gustatory responsiveness of honeybees (Apis mellifera) differ between behavioral castes. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Scheiner, R.; Page, R.E.; Erber, J. Responsiveness to sucrose affects tactile and olfactory learning in preforaging honey bees of two genetic strains. Beh. Brain Res. 2001, 120, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Scheiner, R.; Erber, J.; Page, R.E. Tactile learning and the individual evaluation of the reward in honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). J. Comp. Physiol. A 1999, 185, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Scheiner, R.; Barnert, M.; Erber, J. Variation in water and sucrose responsiveness during the foraging season affects proboscis extension learning in honey bees. Apidologie 2003, 34, 67–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Behrends, A.; Scheiner, R. Evidence for associative learning in newly emerged honey bees (Apis mellifera). Anim. Cogn. 2009, 12, 249–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Scheiner, R.; Kuritz-Kaiser, A.; Menzel, R.; Erber, J. Sensory responsiveness and the effects of equal subjective rewards on tactile learning and memory of honeybees. Learn. Mem. 2005, 12, 626–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Radloff, S.E.; Hepburn, H.R.; Hepburn, C.; de La Rúa, P. Morphometric affinities and population structure of honey bees of the Balearic Islands (Spain). J. Apicult. Res. 2001, 40, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. De la Rúa, P.; Galián, J.; Serrano, J.; Moritz, R.F. Genetic structure and distinctness of Apis mellifera L. populations from the Canary Islands. Mol. Ecol. 2001, 10, 1733–1742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Ruottinen, L.; Berg, P.; Kantanen, J.; Kristensen, T.N.; Praebel, A. Status and Conservation of the Nordic Brown Bee: Final Report; Nordic Genetic Resource Center: Alnarp, Sweden, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  31. VanEngelsdorp, D.; Meixner, M.D. A historical review of managed honey bee populations in Europe and the United States and the factors that may affect them. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 2010, 103 (Suppl. 1), S80–S95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Jensen, A.B.; Palmer, K.A.; Boomsma, J.J.; Pedersen, B.V. Varying degrees of Apis mellifera ligustica introgression in protected populations of the black honeybee, Apis mellifera mellifera, in northwest Europe. Mol. Ecol. 2005, 14, 93–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Sheppard, W.S.; Arias, M.C.; Grech, A.; Meixner, M.D. Apis mellifera ruttneri, a new honey bee subspecies from Malta. Apidologie 1997, 28, 287–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Human, H.; Brodschneider, R.; Dietemann, V.; Dively, G.; Ellis, J.D.; Forsgren, E.; Fries, I.; Hatjina, F.; Hu, F.-L.; Jaffé, R.; et al. Miscellaneous standard methods for Apis mellifera research. J. Apicult. Res. 2013, 52, 1–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Matsumoto, Y.; Menzel, R.; Sandoz, J.-C.; Giurfa, M. Revisiting olfactory classical conditioning of the proboscis extension response in honey bees: A step toward standardized procedures. J. Neurosci. Methods 2012, 211, 159–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Scheiner, R.; Müller, U.; Heimburger, S.; Erber, J. Activity of protein kinase A and gustatory responsiveness in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). J. Comp. Physiol. A 2003, 189, 427–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Hesselbach, H.; Scheiner, R. Effects of the novel pesticide flupyradifurone (Sivanto) on honeybee taste and cognition. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 4954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Scheiner, R. Birth weight and sucrose responsiveness predict cognitive skills of honeybee foragers. Anim. Behav. 2012, 84, 305–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Page, R.E.; Fondrk, M.K. The effects of colony-level selection on the social organization of honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) colonies: Colony-level components of pollen hoarding. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1995, 36, 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Iqbal, J.; Ali, H.; Owayss, A.A.; Raweh, H.S.A.; Engel, M.S.; Alqarni, A.S.; Smith, B.H. Olfactory associative behavioral differences in three honey bee Apis mellifera L. races under the arid zone ecosystem of central Saudi Arabia. Saudi J. Biol. Sci. 2019, 26, 563–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Margotta, J.W.; Roberts, S.P.; Elekonich, M.M. Effects of flight activity and age on oxidative damage in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. J. Exp. Biol. 2018, 221, jeb183228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Scheiner, R.; Amdam, G.V. Impaired tactile learning is related to social role in honeybees. J. Exp. Biol. 2009, 212, 994–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Behrends, A.; Scheiner, R.; Baker, N.; Amdam, G.V. Cognitive aging is linked to social role in honey bees (Apis mellifera). Exp. Gerontol. 2007, 42, 1146–1153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Ferguson, H.J.; Cobey, S.; Smith, B.H. Sensitivity to a change in reward is heritable in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Anim. Behav. 2001, 61, 527–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Pérez Claudio, E.; Rodriguez-Cruz, Y.; Arslan, O.C.; Giray, T.; Agosto Rivera, J.L.; Kence, M.; Wells, H.; Abramson, C.I. Appetitive reversal learning differences of two honey bee subspecies with different foraging behaviors. PeerJ 2018, 6, e5918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Couvillon, M.J.; DeGrandi-Hoffman, G.; Gronenberg, W. Africanized honeybees are slower learners than their European counterparts. Naturwissenschaften 2010, 97, 153–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. McGuire, T.R.; Hirsch, J. Behavior-genetic analysis of Phormia regina: Conditioning, reliable individual differences, and selection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1977, 74, 5193–5197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Laughlin, S. Energy as a constraint on the coding and processing of sensory information. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2001, 11, 475–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Dukas, R. Costs of memory: Ideas and predictions. J. Theor. Biol. 1999, 197, 41–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Johnston, T.D. Selective costs and benefits in the evolution of learning. Adv. Study Behav. 1996, 12, 65–106. [Google Scholar]
  51. Mery, F.; Kawecki, T.J. A fitness cost of learning ability in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2003, 270, 2465–2469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Worden, B.D.; Skemp, A.K.; Papaj, D.R. Learning in two contexts: The effects of interference and body size in bumblebees. J. Exp. Biol. 2005, 208, 2045–2053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Kotrschal, A.; Rogell, B.; Bundsen, A.; Svensson, B.; Zajitschek, S.; Brännström, I.; Immler, S.; Maklakov, A.A.; Kolm, N. Artificial selection on relative brain size in the guppy reveals costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 168–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Chittka, L.; Skorupski, P. Information processing in miniature brains. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2011, 278, 885–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Hammer, M.; Menzel, R. Multiple sites of associative odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections of octopamine in honeybees. Learn. Mem. 1998, 5, 146–156. [Google Scholar]
  56. Behrends, A.; Scheiner, R. Octopamine improves learning in newly emerged bees but not in old foragers. J. Exp. Biol. 2012, 215, 1076–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Rohwedder, A.; Selcho, M.; Chassot, B.; Thum, A.S. Neuropeptide F neurons modulate sugar reward during associative olfactory learning of Drosophila larvae. J. Comp. Neurol. 2015, 523, 2637–2664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Köppler, K.; Vorwohl, G.; Koeniger, N. Comparison of pollen spectra collected by four different subspecies of the honey bee Apis mellifera. Apidologie 2007, 38, 341–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Kamel, S.M.; Strange, J.P.; Sheppard, W.S. A scientific note on hygienic behavior in Apis mellifera lamarckii and A. m. carnica in Egypt. Apidologie 2003, 34, 189–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Brillet, C.; Robinson, G.E.; Bues, R.; Le Conte, Y. Racial differences in division of labor in colonies of the honey bee (Apis mellifera). Ethology 2002, 108, 115–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Map of lineage distribution ranges and points of origin of the different honeybee subspecies studied.
Figure 1. Map of lineage distribution ranges and points of origin of the different honeybee subspecies studied.
Insects 12 00768 g001
Figure 2. Location of experimental hives in a common apiary with four colonies of five subspecies of the Western honeybee Apis mellifera located in Southern Germany.
Figure 2. Location of experimental hives in a common apiary with four colonies of five subspecies of the Western honeybee Apis mellifera located in Southern Germany.
Insects 12 00768 g002
Figure 3. (A) Sucrose responsiveness measured as gustatory response scores (GRS) of foragers belonging to the six different honeybee subspecies trained in this study. Median GRS (dots) and quartiles (25%: lower line, 75%: upper line) are presented. Subspecies were all highly responsive to sucrose and did not differ in their GRS (p > 0.05). (B) Median acquisition scores (dots) of bees from subspecies trained in classical olfactory conditioning and quartiles (see A). Subspecies that differed significantly have different letters (for details see Table S2). (C) Acquisition curves of subspecies based on individual responses to the conditioned odor in each of the 6 training trials. There was a significant effect of subspecies on learning performance (p < 0.05), with A. m. iberiensis foragers performing significantly more poorly than those of A. m. carnica and A. m. ruttneri (for details see Table S3). (D) Correlation between GRS and acquisition scores in each subspecies. Median acquisition scores are shown for bees in each GRS class. Different subspecies are indicated by colors. Generally, the higher the GRS, the higher was the acquisition score, demonstrating a better learning performance. For the number of bees tested see Table S1.
Figure 3. (A) Sucrose responsiveness measured as gustatory response scores (GRS) of foragers belonging to the six different honeybee subspecies trained in this study. Median GRS (dots) and quartiles (25%: lower line, 75%: upper line) are presented. Subspecies were all highly responsive to sucrose and did not differ in their GRS (p > 0.05). (B) Median acquisition scores (dots) of bees from subspecies trained in classical olfactory conditioning and quartiles (see A). Subspecies that differed significantly have different letters (for details see Table S2). (C) Acquisition curves of subspecies based on individual responses to the conditioned odor in each of the 6 training trials. There was a significant effect of subspecies on learning performance (p < 0.05), with A. m. iberiensis foragers performing significantly more poorly than those of A. m. carnica and A. m. ruttneri (for details see Table S3). (D) Correlation between GRS and acquisition scores in each subspecies. Median acquisition scores are shown for bees in each GRS class. Different subspecies are indicated by colors. Generally, the higher the GRS, the higher was the acquisition score, demonstrating a better learning performance. For the number of bees tested see Table S1.
Insects 12 00768 g003
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Scheiner, R.; Lim, K.; Meixner, M.D.; Gabel, M.S. Comparing the Appetitive Learning Performance of Six European Honeybee Subspecies in a Common Apiary. Insects 2021, 12, 768. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects12090768

AMA Style

Scheiner R, Lim K, Meixner MD, Gabel MS. Comparing the Appetitive Learning Performance of Six European Honeybee Subspecies in a Common Apiary. Insects. 2021; 12(9):768. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects12090768

Chicago/Turabian Style

Scheiner, Ricarda, Kayun Lim, Marina D. Meixner, and Martin S. Gabel. 2021. "Comparing the Appetitive Learning Performance of Six European Honeybee Subspecies in a Common Apiary" Insects 12, no. 9: 768. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects12090768

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop