Next Article in Journal
Sharp Bounds for Trigonometric and Hyperbolic Functions with Application to Fractional Calculus
Previous Article in Journal
Subjective and Objective Evaluation of the Symmetry of Maxillary Incisors among Residents of Southwest Poland
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Motor Skills Training Program Reinforces Crossing the Body’s Midline in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder

by
Orifjon Saidmamatov
1,2,*,
Paula Rodrigues
1,3 and
Olga Vasconcelos
1
1
Motor Control and Learning Laboratory, Centre of Research, Education, Innovation and Intervention in Sport, Faculty of Sport, University of Porto, 4200-450 Porto, Portugal
2
Faculty of Physical Culture, Urgench State University, Urgench 220100, Uzbekistan
3
Kinesiolab, Laboratory of Human Movement Analysis, Piaget Institute, 1950-157 Almada, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Symmetry 2022, 14(6), 1259; https://doi.org/10.3390/sym14061259
Submission received: 12 April 2022 / Revised: 1 June 2022 / Accepted: 13 June 2022 / Published: 17 June 2022

Abstract

:
Midline crossing problems have been associated with children with atypical development. When compared to typical development (TD) children, they are less likely to cross the midline into contralateral space with their preferred hand. A motor skills training program is the most beneficial intervention for children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD). However, there is not enough information on how this intervention will affect crossing the midline. The goal of this study was to find out midline crossing behavior after an intervention program for children with DCD. The Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC-2) was used to assess motor coordination, and Bishop’s card-reaching task measured the ability of children to cross the midline. The study included 48 right-handed children, 28 with TD and 20 with DCD (5.17 ± 0.70 years) from four preschools in the Khorezm region (Uzbekistan). Participants identified as having DCD were placed in an experimental group (EG: n = 15), receiving ten weeks of a motor skills training program, and a control group (CG: n = 5). Concerning midline crossing behavior before intervention, DCD children showed more contralateral and less midline right-hand use compared to TD children. After intervention, a significant group × position interaction was found between the EG and CG at positions 1 and 4, and between the EG and TD group at position 4. Concerning the midline reaching, fewer right-hand reaches were made by the EG group compared to the other two groups. The same could be seen at position 1 when compared to the CG group. It can be concluded that, after intervention, DCD children in the EG showed fewer right-hand reaches in the contralateral space, but they improved their right-hand reaches in the midline, showing a similar behavior to TD children.

1. Introduction

The midline is a hypothetical line drawn through the center of human bodies. Crossing the body midline is the ability to move over spontaneously with a body part, such as the arms or legs, to reach across the body’s center. The ability to cross the midline indicates that both sides of the brain are communicating, and that the child can coordinate both the left and right sides at the same time, which is required for learning skills and to perform movements efficiently [1]. Subsequently, crossing the midline is a necessary developmental milestone. This is because if the child learns to cross the midline, he/she will become more likely to use the preferred hand, and he/she will keep practicing every day or acquire sports skills more easily [2]. Moreover, the midline of the body has been credited with contributing to the formation of the spatial orientation, bilateral integration, body scheme, directionality, and laterality [2,3], indicating the integration of the body’s midline, which makes bilateral coordination possible [4]. Postures of gross or body movements, such as standing upright, walking, turning, sitting, or lying, include both halves of the body, moving parallel to, on the opposite side of, or in a circular manner about the central axis of laterality [3]. Additionally, it has been hypothesized that midline behavior plays an important part in the development of ego capabilities [5,6]. These researchers advanced the hypothesis that individuals orient themselves in a manner that is directly tied to their symmetrical structure and that all motions in space are similarly related to this symmetry around the midline.
A developmental trend has been observed in typically developing (TD) children from age 3 to age 8 while crossing the midline for unimanual movements; the number of midline crossings developed with age, with younger children showing a lower incidence of crossing the midline (i.e., doing more right-hand reaches into the right hemispace) [4,7]. Moreover, this trend was independent of gender and the handedness of the participants [8]. When taking into consideration the spatial position of the crossing behavior, midline crossings happened more often near the middle of the body than in the mid-way and farthest positions [8,9,10].
Failure to participate in an adequate amount of midline behavior in early infancy is a forerunner of difficulties later in life, which, when interacting with other factors, unfold as disturbances in laterality, which may result in a learning disadvantage [5]. Midline crossing problems have been associated with children with atypical development, such as learning disabilities [2,4,11,12], attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or autism spectrum disorder [13], down syndrome [14], and developmental coordination disorder (DCD) [15,16]. Results of these studies show that those children tended to make fewer midline crossing reaches, tended to be less ready to cross the midline into contralateral space with their preferred hand, and tended to be less consistent in their hand preference within and across activities. Furthermore, they showed deficits for tasks in the contralateral workspace. A kinematic investigation carried out by Smits-Engelsman [16] provides a rigorous measurement of children with and without DCD in both accuracy and speed when aim-directed movements were made across the midline. In contrast to expectations, this research discovered that DCD children were not preferentially impaired when making movements in a contralateral workspace, even though they were not as good as their peers overall. This supported the findings of Zoia [17] in a reaching task performed by children with and without DCD under normal and perturbed vision. Specifically, in children with DCD, Smits-Engelsman [16] proposed a general neuromotor deficit hypothesis, suggesting that these children have a weakness in movement initiation, and contralateral inaccuracy is added to their overall inaccuracy. These findings are supported by numerous studies [18,19] that have established that children with DCD exhibit an unexplained inability to acquire necessary motor skills, such as maintaining balance or writing, despite the absence of any known medical condition or identifiable neurological disease. Performances of children with DCD tend to be slower, more dependent on vision, and more variable than those of TD children [20]. Nevertheless, for children with DCD, a motor skills training program can enhance the ability of their motor competence [21]. Sherick, Greenman, and Legg [5] stated that external stimulation helps myelination, vascularization, and dendritic arborization in the brain; all of which are thought to be necessary for the brain to reach its genetic potential and work well. Moreover, multiple researchers have stated that hand preference may be associated with sensory–motor experience [22,23,24,25]. Consequently, we hypothesized that a motor skill program would provide such stimulation, especially in midline crossing, because of the manual skills (manual dexterity and aiming and catching) activities proposed. Since a relationship has been reported between an inconsistency of using the preferred hand in children with DCD and less frequent use of the preferred hand in tasks requiring reaches across the midline, it was expected that a motor intervention program induces a more consistent handedness behavior expressed by more frequent midline crossing of the preferred hand. To our knowledge, there is no study that has compared midline crossing behavior after an intervention motor program. As a result, the purpose of this study was to investigate the impact of an intervention program on midline crossing behavior for children with developmental coordination disorder compared to typical development children.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Design

With a control group, a quasi-experimental design was used with pre- and post-test measurements.

2.2. Participants

A selected sample of children from 4 preschools in Urgench (a city in the Khorezm region of Uzbekistan) was recruited during September 2020. The sample comprised 48 right-handed children: 28 (14 male) with TD, and 20 (12 male) with DCD, aged between 4 and 6 years old. Children with learning difficulties or children with attention deficit disorder, prenatal complications, neurological disorders, or sensory problems, preterm infants, and children with epilepsy or other chronic diseases were not included in the study. As a result, none of the participants suffered from any neurological or physical issues. These data were gathered from the preschool records of the children, as well as feedback from their parents. A total of 28 TD children (14 male) had: (1) no evidence of functional motor problems as observed by their teacher; (2) no MABC-2 score at or above the 16th percentile; (3) no serious medical diagnosis reported by parents or relatives; and (4) no cognitive or intellectual impairment as reported by their teacher. On the other hand, seven children (four of them were male) appeared to perform the parameter for DCD. In DSM-5 [26], they scored in the 5th percentile or lower on the MABC-2 (Criterion A), the teacher identified them as having a motor coordination problem (Criterion B), they were 3–6 years old (Criterion C), and their parents or relatives reported no significant medical condition that could affect motor performance, and their teacher confirmed the absence of intellectual or cognitive impairment (Criterion D). Thirteen children (eight male) fell between the 6th and 15th percentile, which means they were at risk for DCD. From these 20 DCD children, 15 children were randomly assigned to the experimental group (EG), and the remaining 5 children were assigned to the control group (CG). Prior to participation, the parents supplied verbal consent and the children provided informed consent. Through the permission given by parents and teachers, we selected 15 children in the EG to be engaged in a 10-week motor skills training program from October to December 2020 (Figure 1).
The approach provided in the MABC-2 was used to assess hand preference. A pen was placed on the table in front of the child, and he/she was asked to make a tiny drawing of a figure.

2.3. Instruments

The Movement Assessment Battery for Children—Second Edition (MABC-2) [27] was used to evaluate DCD. Besides being one of the most common instruments applied in this domain, MABC-2 is an assessment tool that can easily be used in a school setting, being a valid and reliable instrument [28,29]. Moreover, equal validity to the original UK version of the MABC-2 was shown in Greek, German, Japanese, and Thai studies [30,31,32,33]. The MABC-2 test is designed to identify and describe impairments in the motor performance of children and adolescents from 3 to 16 years of age, divided into three age bands (3–6 years, 7–10 years, and 11–16 years). The test consists of children completing a series of eight fine and gross motor tasks divided into three subscales: manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and balance [27].
Bishop’s card-reaching task (Quantification of Hand Preference, QHP) was used to evaluate the crossing midline behavior. Bishop and her colleagues proposed a card-reaching test to determine hand preference in 1996 [34]. The test measures the degree and direction of hand preference in a task that allows participants to cross their body midline (Figure 2).
Good metric qualities were found for the QHP [34]. The previous literature suggests that the QHP is a suitable measure of human handedness in children and adults. It should also be noted that the above instrument has been used by many researchers [4,5,10,18,21,35,36].

2.4. Procedures

Participants were intentionally allocated to a predetermined and unbalanced group. The reason for such imbalance was that since data were gathered in four kindergartens, children in one of the kindergartens were assigned to be the CG. As a result, the intervention program was carried out in each of the 3 remaining kindergartens (approximately 5 children in each) by two trained professionals. For ten weeks, children assigned to the EG participated in a 45 min, twice-weekly motor skills training program, with an average of 15 h of training. Each training session had two main parts: a 5 min warm-up and 40 min of working on motor skills. Detailed information about the training sessions can be obtained elsewhere [21]. All sessions were recorded with permission. All children continued to perform their physical education classes and regular classroom activities as scheduled. Moreover, in the EG, the motor intervention program replaced the daily walk of 45 min, an activity besides the physical education classes that all children performed in the indoor facilities of each school. During the study, enrolled children did not attend additional out-of-preschool sports. A variety of functional activities and exercises were used to improve common motor issues experienced by children with DCD, such as poor agility, balance, core stability, movement coordination, and ball skills. It is worth noting that exercises involving hand skills were performed with each hand and with both hands, enhancing uni- and bi-manual skills. As the training progressed, the motor tasks were modified to ensure successful task execution while also providing a sufficient challenge to the child’s motor ability. The assessment in pre- and post-tests was carried out by the same professional and recorded on video.

2.5. Ethics

The research was approved by the Ethics Committee of Urgench State University (Code 12356), in accordance with the recommendations of the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

When a card was reached with the right hand at positions 1, 2, and 3, the subject crossed the midline of the body, and position 1 extended into the contralateral hemispace, the furthest point to the left, with position 3 being the closest place to the midline. When a card was reached with the right hand in positions 5, 6, and 7, the children reached into the ipsilateral hemispace, with position 5 being the farthest point to the right, and position 7 being the closest to the body (see Figure 2).
Gender was included as a factor in preliminary analyses, but no significant effect was found for any of the variables analyzed; therefore, data from males and females were pooled. Furthermore, as the number of participants was too small within each age group, with the majority being 5 years old, the age effect could not be analyzed. The hand use for the different groups at the 7 hemispace positions was analyzed by a multivariate analysis ANOVA. Following these analyses, a comparison between pre- and post-intervention was made by a non-parametric Wilcoxon test in each DCD group (EG and CG).
All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (SPSS v.25, IBM Corporation, New York, NY, USA). The statistical significance level was set at p ≤ 0.05. Exact p values are provided. Post hoc comparisons were performed using the LSD (least small difference) procedure [37]. When possible, the size of the statistical effects (partial eta-squared, abbreviated as ηp2, which indicates the part of the variance explained by the factor with other non-error sources if the variance is partially led out) [38] has been given.

3. Results

The results before the intervention, in terms of right-hand reaches (Table 1), indicate a main effect of group (F(12,82) = 3.514, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.340), and a group × position interaction (F(10,84) = 4.167, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.332). Post hoc comparisons revealed significant differences between the EG and TD group at position 3 (p = 0.049) and 4 (p = 0.002). At position 3, more right-hand reaches were made by the EG, and the opposite was observed at the midline, where the DCD group appeared less right-handed than the TD children.
Because of COVID-19, data from the TD group could not be collected after the intervention period, and only the DCD group could be reassessed. So, we decided to compare the results from the pre-intervention time of the TD group and the post-intervention of the DCD groups (experimental and control). The results of the comparisons between the groups, after applying the training program, are as follows (Table 2). A main influence of group was found (F(10,82) = 2.236, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.210), as well as a group × position interaction (F(8,86) = 2.801, p = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.207). Post hoc comparisons revealed significant differences between the EG and CG at position 1 (p = 0.015) and 4 (p = 0.040), and between the EG and TD group at position 4 (p = 0.033). Concerning the midline reaching, fewer right-hand reaches were made by the EG group compared to the other two groups. The same could be seen at position 1, when compared to the CG group.
A comparison between pre- and post-intervention in each DCD group, EG, and CG, was made. Results reveal that children in the CG were not different in pre- and post-test (p > 0.050). On the other hand, significant differences in the EG were found only in position 3 (p = 0.020), where, after intervention, fewer right-hand reaches were observed. The frequency of right-hand reaches was plotted for the seven different spatial positions comparing pre- and post-intervention in the EG (see Figure 3).

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate midline crossing behavior after an intervention program for children with DCD. For ten weeks, the acquisition and development of motor skills were meticulously applied, and the effectiveness of the motor skills training program for midline crossing was evaluated.
As expected, the results of this study indicate that the frequency of the right-hand use was dependent upon position in hemispace and the group. Typical development participants used the right hand primarily in ipsilateral space and the left hand to reach contralateral space. However, DCD children revealed a different behavior, showing less consistent right-hand use. While in position 3, the DCD group was more often willing to use the right hand, and then cross the hemispace, the opposite occurred in position 4 at the midline, where they exhibited a more inconsistent behavior. Within the TD children, the distribution of hand use in the hemispace indicated that almost all actions were carried out with the dominant hand in the ipsilateral hemispace, while only a few were performed with the dominant hand in the contralateral hemispace [39]. However, reaching into the opposite hemispace contralaterally is widespread and may be a result of the development of hand preference. [40]. Namely, younger children have a weaker hand preference and are less likely to reach across the middle to grab an object, while older children use their preferred hand more and reach across the middle more often [41]. This stage of skill refining is necessary for creating a hand preference in childhood, supported by developed processing systems such as maturation of the interhemispheric pathways [42]. Concerning the DCD children, findings from this study do not support previous research with DCD children [15]. This was because we noticed that the right hand was less used to cross the contralateral hemispace. The explanation for these opposite results may rely on the age effect, because in Hill and Bishop [15], children were older than in this study. It could be said that reaching across the body’s midline requires more complex motor programming than an ipsilateral reaching behavior. When faced with the choice of making a hard and a more complex movement with the preferred hand, children with poor motor skills will choose the easy movement, using the non-preferred hand in the contralateral hemispace. Another possible explanation was provided by Verfaellie, Bowers, and Heilman, [43] and Verfaellie and Heilman, [44] and later supported by Gabbard and Rabb [45]. The hemispheric bias hypothesis proposes a hemisphere (attentional) bias in favor of using the hand on the same side as the stimulus.
Another perspective proposed by Gabbard and Rabb [45] is the kinesthetic hypothesis, which proposes that the individual recognized the biomechanical restrictions (e.g., degrees of freedom) involved in carrying out the task and programmed the pleasant and most efficient response by using the hand closer to the object. This hypothesis was supported by Vasconcelos et al. [46], who found that fewer lateralized children seem to program hand selection based on biomechanical efficiency and proximity to the object to be grasped and not on motor dominance of the preferred hand, as more lateralized children do. Additionally, and according to Liedermen [47], manual midline crossing repeats a transition from extracellular to colossal control of interhemispheric communication and is, therefore, a prerequisite for increasing a skilled dominant hand [43,44,45]. Hand preference is a behavioral mirror of brain lateralization for handedness; it reveals how strongly a person prefers one hand over the other. In addition, and according to Smits-Engelsman et al. [16], an alternative explanation for the clinical monitor is that impairments for activities in the contralateral workspace are more obvious in DCD children because these children already have a deficit, and contralateral inaccuracy is added to the overall inaccuracy (general neuromotor deficit hypothesis).
After intervention, DCD participants were more willing to use the left hand in contralateral space, and the difference was particularly evident in position 3. Additionally, differences at the midline position were found, where the DCD group appeared to be less right-handed than the TD group, but with an increase in the right-hand reaches, confirming results of other studies [8,14]. The willingness of using the left hand in contralateral space is in line with results from TD children [8,39], confirming that intervention programs seem to have an effect on the hemispace behavior and midline, showing that DCD children developed a more similar behavior to TD children.
So far, many researchers have tried using different intervention programs to develop motor skills in children with DCD. In a systematic review article, Saidmamatov et al. [36] documented that motor skill training programs are very useful for children with DCD. Moreover, results are scientifically substantiated in another study [21] that postulated a 10-week motor skills training program develops motor skills in children with DCD, and also noted that DCD problems do not go away on their own. Going beyond the investigation of Saidmamatov et al. [21], the present study concluded that a structured 10-week motor training program not only develops motor skills of children with DCD but also helps them to improve their midline crossing process. Additionally, those results partially support the hypothesis of many researchers that hand preference may be related to emotional–motor experience, along with the expected maturational process [22,23,24,25]. So, the results justify the initial research hypothesis that a motor skill program would provide external stimulation, especially in midline crossing, because of the manual skills (manual dexterity and aiming and catching) activities proposed. Moreover, the experiment provides new insight into the relationship between a motor skills training program and midline crossing.

5. Conclusions

It can be concluded from this study that right-handed children with DCD, after participating in a motor skills program, reinforced the handedness behavior and acquired the ability to form a left-handed working process in the contralateral space by engaging in an effective intervention program. Furthermore, results from this study demonstrate that by providing developmentally appropriate motor skills, such as crossing the midline of the body, through a structured motor training program, we can help young children build developmental readiness for competitive sports that rely on efficient cross-lateral processing speed.

Study Limitations

Certain limitations must be considered. Some methodological issues limit the validity of the research results, including convenience sampling. This study suffered from the limitations associated with the pandemic caused by COVID-19, which meant TD children were unable to be reassessed. Furthermore, the small sample size of DCD groups, especially in the CG, was also a limitation. The study only involved right-handers, so we suggest the inclusion of left-handed children in future investigations.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, O.S.; methodology, P.R.; software, O.V.; validation, P.R.; formal analysis, O.V.; investigation, O.S.; resources, O.S.; data curation, P.R.; writing—original draft preparation, O.S.; writing—review and editing, P.R.; visualization, O.V.; supervision, P.R.; project administration, O.S.; funding acquisition, O.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The APC (Article Processing Charge) was funded by the “El-yurt Umidi” Foundation under the President Administration of the Republic of Uzbekistan. “El-Yurt Umidi” Foundation was created for training specialists abroad and to promote the dialogue with compatriots under the Cabinet of Ministers of the Republic of Uzbekistan”. Scholarships will be awarded within the framework of five areas of selection: education, science and research, healthcare, public administration, and emerging sectors of the economy, as well as in four areas of study: doctoral (PhD), master, internship and qualification upgrading programs. Concerning the present article, a doctoral Scholarship was awarded to the first author of this article.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Urgench State University (protocol code 12356 approved on 7 August 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ayers, A. Sensory Integration and Learning Disorders; Western Psychological Services: Los Angles, CA, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  2. Schilder, P. The Image an Appearance of the Human Body; International University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1950. [Google Scholar]
  3. Stilwell, J.M. The development of manual midline crossing in 2- to 6-year-old children. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 1987, 41, 783–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Smith, S.M. Performance difference between hands in children on the motor accuracy test-revised. Am. J. Occup. Ther. Off. Publ. Am. Occup. Ther. Assoc. 1983, 37, 96–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Sherick, I.; Greenman, G.; Legg, C. Some comments on the significance and development of midline behavior during infancy. Child Psychiatry Hum 1976, 6, 170–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Goody, W.; Reinhold, M. Some aspects of human orientation in space: Sensation and movement. Brain 1952, 75, 472–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Serrien, D.J.; O’Regan, L. The development of motor planning strategies in children. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2021, 18, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Carlier, M.; Anne-Lise, D.; Lamard, C. Midline crossing: Developmental trend from 3 to 10 years of age in a preferential card-reaching task. Brain Cogn. 2006, 61, 255–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cermak, S.A.; Quintero, E.J.; Cohen, P.M. Developmental age trends in crossing the body midline in normal children. Am. J. Occup. Ther. Off. Publ. Am. Occup. Ther. Assoc. 1980, 34, 313–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Pedersen, S.J.; Young, M.N. Midline crossing movements: A teachable skill for developing children. In Proceedings of the AARE 2010 Conference Proceedings, Melbourne, VIC, Australia, 28 November–2 December 2010. [Google Scholar]
  11. Michell, D.; Wood, N. An investigation of midline crossing in three-year old children. Physiotherapy 1999, 85, 607–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Murata, N.M.; Tan, C.A. Collaborative teaching of motor skills for preschools with developmental delays. Early Child. Educ. 2009, 36, 483–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Begum Ali, J.; Charman, T.; Johnson, M.H.; Jones, E.J. Early motor differences in infants at elevated likelihood of autism spectrum disorder and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 50, 4367–4384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Groen, M.A.; Yasin, I.; Laws, G.; Barry, J.G.; Bishop, D.V. Weak hand preference in children with Down syndrome is associated with language deficits. Dev. Psychobiol. J. Int. Soc. Dev. Psychobiol. 2008, 50, 242–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Hill, E.L.; Bishop, D.V. A reaching test reveals weak hand preference in specific language impairment and developmental co-ordination disorder. Laterality 1998, 3, 295–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Smits-Engelsman, B.C.; Bloem-van der Wel, H.E.; Duysens, J. Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder respond similarly to age-matched controls in both speed and accuracy if goal-directed movements are made across the midline. Child Care Health Dev. 2006, 32, 703–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Zoia, S.; Blason, L.; D’Ottavio, G.; Bulgheroni, M.; Pezzetta, E.; Skabar, A.; Castiello, U. Evidence of early development of action planning in the human foetus: A kinematic study. Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation Cérébrale 2007, 176, 217–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Goez, H.; Zelnik, N. Handedness in Patients with Developmental Coordination Disorder. J. Child Neurol. 2008, 23, 151–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Freitas, C.; Vasconcelos, O.; Botelho, V. Handedness and developmental coordination disorder in Portuguese children: Study with the M-ABC test. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2014, 6, 655–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Biancotto, M.; Skabar, A.; Bulgheroni, M.; Carrozzi, M.; Zoia, S. Neuromotor deficits in developmental coordination disorder: Evidence from a reach-to-grasp task. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 1293–1300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Saidmamatov, O.; Raximov, Q.; Rodrigues, P.; Vasconcelos, O. A Ten-Week Motor Skills Training Program Increases Motor Competence in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Children 2021, 8, 1147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Coryell, J.F.; Michel, G.F. How Supine Postural Preferences of Infants can contribute toward the development of handedness. Infant Behav. 1978, 1, 245–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Nudo, R.J.; Milliken, G.W.; Jenkins, W.M.; Merzenich, M.M. Usedependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J. Neurosci. 1996, 16, 785–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Provins, K.A. Handedness and speech: A critical reappraisal of the role of genetic and environmental factors in the cerebral lateralization of function. Psychol. Rev. 1997, 104, 554–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Corbetta, D.; Thelen, E. Behavioral fluctuations and the development of manual asymmetries in infancy: Contributions of the dynamic systems approach. Handb. Neuropsychol. 2002, 8, 311–330. [Google Scholar]
  26. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5 Task Force, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Henderson, S.E.; Sugden, D.A.; Barnett, A.L. Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2; Harcourt Assessment: London, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lander, N.; Morgan, P.J.; Salmon, J.; Barnett, L.M. Teachers’ Perceptions of a Fundamental Movement Skill (FMS) Assessment Battery in a School Setting. Meas. Phys. Educ. Exerc. Sci. 2016, 20, 50–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Lander, N.; Morgan, P.J.; Salmon, J.; Logan, S.W.; Barnett, L.M. The reliability and validity of an authentic motor skill assessment tool for early adolescent girls in an Australian school setting. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2017, 20, 590–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Wagner, M.O.; Kastner, J.; Petermann, F.; Bös, K. Factorial validity of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (age band 2). Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 674–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Kita, Y.; Suzuki, K.; Hirata, S.; Sakihara, K.; Inagaki, M.; Nakai, A. Applicability of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-Second Edition to Japanese children: A study of the Age Band 2. Brain Dev. 2016, 38, 706–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ellinoudis, T.; Evaggelinou, C.; Kourtessis, T.; Konstantinidou, Z.; Venetsanou, F.; Kambas, A. Reliability and validity of age band 1 of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children--second edition. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 1046–1051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Jaikaew, R.; Satiansukpong, N. Movement Assessment Battery for Children-(MABC2): Cross-Cultural Validity, Content Validity, and Interrater Reliability in Thai Children. Occup. Ther. Int. 2019, 2019, 4086594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Bishop, D.V.M.; Ross, V.; Daniels, M.S.; Bright, P. The measurement of hand preference. A validation study comparing three groups of right-handers. Br. J. Psychol. 1996, 87, 269–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Doyen, A.-L.; Carlier, M. Measuring Handedness: A validation study of the Bishop’s Card-reaching test. Laterality 2002, 7, 115–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Saidmamatov, O.A.; Nascimento, M.M.; Cerqueira, J.C.; Rodriguez, P.; Vasconcelos, O. Motor skill training programs for children with developmental coordination disorder: Does gender matter? Neuropsychiatr. L’enfance L’adolescence 2022, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Howell, D.C. Statistical Methods for Psychology, 4th ed.; Wadsworth Publishing: Belmont, CA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  38. Cohen, J. Eta-squared and partial eta-squared in fixed factor ANOVA design. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1973, 33, 107–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bryden, P.J.; Pryde, K.M.; Roy, E.A. A performance measure of the degree of hand preference. Brain Cogn. 2000, 44, 402–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. O’Regan, L.; Serrien, D. Individual Differences and Hemispheric Asymmetries for Language and Spatial Attention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Doyen, A.; Dufour, T.; Caroff, X.; Cherfouh, A.; Carlier, M. Hand preference and hand performance: Cross sectional developmental trends and family resemblance in degree of laterality. Laterality 2008, 13, 179–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bishop, D.V.; Adams, C. A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1990, 31, 1027–1050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Verfaellie, M.; Bowers, D.; Heilman, K.M. Attentional factors in the occurrence of stimulus-response compatibility effects. Neuropsychologia 1988, 28, 435–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Verfaellie, M.; Heilman, K. Hemispheric asymmetries in attentional control: Implications for hand preference in sensorimotor tasks. Brain Cogn. 1990, 14, 70–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Gabbard, C.; Rabb, C. What determines choice of limb for unimanual reaching movements? J. Gen. Psychol. 2000, 127, 178–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Vasconcelos, O.; Rodrigues, P.; Barreiros, J.; Jacobsohn, L. Laterality, developmental coordination disorders and posture. In Estudos em Desenvolvimento Motor da Criança II; Rodrigues, L.P., Saraiva, L., Barreiros, J., Vasconcelos, O., Eds.; Escola Superior de Educação, Instituto Politécnico de Viana do Castelo: Viana do Castelo, Portugal, 2009; pp. 19–26. ISBN 978-989-95980-3-4. [Google Scholar]
  47. Liederman, J. Mechanisms underlying instability in the development of hand preference. In Manual Specialization and the Developing Brain; Young, G., Segalworitz, S.J., Carter, C.M., Trehub, S.E., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 82–85. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. CONSORT flow diagram.
Figure 1. CONSORT flow diagram.
Symmetry 14 01259 g001
Figure 2. Set up the task for the QHP. Position 4 = midline, child faces this. For right-handed participants, position 1–3 = contralateral; positions 5–7 = ipsilateral.
Figure 2. Set up the task for the QHP. Position 4 = midline, child faces this. For right-handed participants, position 1–3 = contralateral; positions 5–7 = ipsilateral.
Symmetry 14 01259 g002
Figure 3. Mean number of reaches as a function of the position of the card from the body midline in the EG. Ipsilateral hemispace: positions 5, 6, and 7; midline: position 4; and contralateral hemispace: positions 1, 2, and 3—see Figure 2.
Figure 3. Mean number of reaches as a function of the position of the card from the body midline in the EG. Ipsilateral hemispace: positions 5, 6, and 7; midline: position 4; and contralateral hemispace: positions 1, 2, and 3—see Figure 2.
Symmetry 14 01259 g003
Table 1. Results of right-hand reaches to the hemispace before intervention based on the group type. Number of participants, mean, and standard deviation values.
Table 1. Results of right-hand reaches to the hemispace before intervention based on the group type. Number of participants, mean, and standard deviation values.
GroupEGCGTD
N15528
PositionMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
10.671.180.801.100.320.82
20.471.100.000.000.391.00
31.271.490.400.890.501.04
41.871.251.890.702.710.54
52.870.523.000.002.930.38
63.000.003.000.002.930.40
73.000.003.000.002.930.40
Table 2. Results of right-hand reaches to the hemispace after intervention based on the group type. Number of participants, mean, and standard deviation values.
Table 2. Results of right-hand reaches to the hemispace after intervention based on the group type. Number of participants, mean, and standard deviation values.
GroupEGCGTD
N15528
PositionMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
10.000.001.001.400.320.82
20.000.000.000.000.390.99
30.200.770.400.890.501.04
42.201.083.000.002.710.53
53.000.003.000.002.930.38
63.000.003.000.002.930.38
73.000.003.000.002.930.38
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Saidmamatov, O.; Rodrigues, P.; Vasconcelos, O. Motor Skills Training Program Reinforces Crossing the Body’s Midline in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Symmetry 2022, 14, 1259. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym14061259

AMA Style

Saidmamatov O, Rodrigues P, Vasconcelos O. Motor Skills Training Program Reinforces Crossing the Body’s Midline in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Symmetry. 2022; 14(6):1259. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym14061259

Chicago/Turabian Style

Saidmamatov, Orifjon, Paula Rodrigues, and Olga Vasconcelos. 2022. "Motor Skills Training Program Reinforces Crossing the Body’s Midline in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder" Symmetry 14, no. 6: 1259. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym14061259

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop