Next Article in Journal
A Programming-Based Algorithm for Probabilistic Uncertain Linguistic Intuitionistic Fuzzy Group Decision-Making
Previous Article in Journal
A Dunkl–Type Generalization of Szász–Kantorovich Operators via Post–Quantum Calculus
Previous Article in Special Issue
Meta-Control in Pigeons (Columba livia) and the Role of the Commissura Anterior
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Relationship between Motor Laterality and Aggressive Behavior in Sheepdogs

by
Marcello Siniscalchi
1,*,
Daniele Bertino
2,
Serenella d’Ingeo
1 and
Angelo Quaranta
1
1
Department of Veterinary Medicine, Section of Behavioral Sciences and Animal Bioethics, University of Bari “Aldo Moro”, 70121 Bari, Italy
2
Med Vet, ENCI-CSEN certified professional dog trainer, 20060 Milano, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Symmetry 2019, 11(2), 233; https://doi.org/10.3390/sym11020233
Submission received: 10 January 2019 / Revised: 7 February 2019 / Accepted: 11 February 2019 / Published: 15 February 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Left Versus Right Asymmetries of Brain and Behaviour)

Abstract

:
Sheepdogs’ visuospatial abilities, their control of prey-driven behavior and their motor functions are essential characteristics for success in sheepdog trials. We investigated the influence of laterality on 15 sheepdogs’ (Canis familiaris) spontaneous turning motor pattern around a herd and on their behavior during the first encounter with sheep in a training session. The most relevant finding of this research was that the dogs displayed significantly more aggressive behavior toward the sheep when turning in a counterclockwise direction around the herd. Considering that in counterclockwise turns the sheep were in the dogs’ left visual hemifield, the high frequency of aggressions registered during counterclockwise turns suggests right hemisphere main activation. Overall, our results revealed the existence of a relationship between motor lateralization and aggressive behavior in dogs during sheepdog training and have practical implications for sheepdog training.

1. Introduction

Sheepdog trials are a worldwide sports competition involving dogs and handlers, in which the dogs’ ability to manage sheep properly during different daily working activities is tested (e.g., gathering, driving, shedding, penning, and singling). Historically, sheepdogs belong to different breeds, which were selected to cooperate with humans in sheep raising, specifically in guarding and herding the animals [1]. As a consequence, the selection and training of dogs are fundamental aspects for both farm work and sheepdog trials. Among the required characteristics, the visuospatial abilities of the dogs and their control of their motor functions and prey-driven behavior are essential for the success of sheepdog activities. Predatory aggressive behavior is a part of the predation functional system, which includes different behaviors aimed at capturing and killing the prey. According to the definition of aggression, animals display aggression with the intention to do harm. Therefore, predatory aggressive behavior falls into this category, even though the animal’s motivation is very different from that in other forms of aggression (e.g., social aggression) [2].
There is now clear evidence that visuospatial information is analyzed in an asymmetrical way by the dogs’ brains and that it is associated with asymmetries of the dogs’ motor functions [3,4]. Specifically, it has been found that agility-trained dogs displayed longer latencies to complete the weave pole obstacle (a task requiring dogs to work around pole obstacles secured in a straight line) when the owner was located in their left visual hemifield compared to when they were in the right one [3]. Given that in a dog’s brain the right hemisphere neural structures are mainly fed by inputs from the left visual hemifield and vice versa (crossed fibers at the optic nerve level are more than 70% [5]), these results support the general hypothesis of right hemisphere specialization for the analysis of stimuli with high emotional valence (i.e., the owner) [6,7,8,9,10,11]. In other words, visual analysis of the owner exerted predominantly by neural pathways of the left eye (activity of the right hemisphere) was likely to increase the dogs’ arousal state, distracting them during the performance of agility obstacles (resulting in longer latencies to complete the task).
A recent study has found a significant relationship between the lateralized processing of visuospatial attention and motor functions in canine species [4]. Briefly, dogs preferentially using their left paw in a motor task that required subjects to hold a puzzle feeder (namely, the “Kong test”) showed a leftward bias in the total number of food items eaten from a Plexiglas board (i.e., a food detection task resembling the so-called cancellation test). Similarly, a reversed rightward bias was observed in subjects significantly preferring their right paw in the motor task; whereas, no bias was found for ambi-pawed dogs. In addition, considering the order of eating food items, the above-mentioned significant sidedness effect (left vs. right hemispace attention) was revealed only in the left-pawed group that showed a clear leftward bias (right hemisphere activity), supporting the general hypothesis of the right hemispheric superiority in spatial attention control. Apart from contributing to basic knowledge about the biology of dogs, these results could improve human abilities in canine training during different activities (animal-assisted therapy, guide dogs for the vision impaired, or sport competitions). For instance, it could be useful for a dog trainer to know the dog’s visuospatial orienting bias in order to choose the best strategy to interact with it during training (e.g., to optimize the capture of his attention and/or to choose the handling side that interferes less with his orienting attention).
In light of the previous research evidence, the present study aimed at investigating the influence of laterality on sheepdog training and on their selection, evaluating the dogs’ turning motor pattern around the herd and their behavior during the first encounter with sheep in a training session.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects

Access to 15 healthy adult dogs (Canis familiaris: 6 males and 9 females) was obtained through an agility dog center “Mix and Breed” in Bussero, Italy. Their ages ranged from 2 to 8 years (4.6 ± 0.45; mean years ± SEM). The studied population was composed of both pure and crossbred dogs of small, medium, and large body size. Only two males and four females were neutered. None of the observed dogs had previously been trained in sheepdog activities (see Table 1 for details). In addition, the dogs were checked by a veterinarian for the presence of estrous or mouthedness (e.g., broken canines or teeth issues), which may have biased their performance under the test conditions.

2.2. Procedure

Each subject was led by the owner within a large enclosed area where a group of 3 sheep was placed in the center (Figure 1). As soon as the owners reached a designated position, at a distance of about 4 meters from the sheep, they unleashed the dog and left the area. The dog’s interactions with the herd were recorded using two video cameras in order to have a full view of the working area. An expert sheepdog trainer was positioned close to the sheep to promptly interrupt the dog’s aggression toward the sheep by using voice commands or by shaking a black plastic envelope. The recordings lasted 6 minutes, from the dog’s release till the end of the working session. The dogs were initially allowed to interact freely with the sheep, turning around the herd according to their natural movement. Subsequently, they were induced by the trainer to interrupt their spontaneous rotational movement around the sheep every 20 s (if the dog kept its own rotation around the sheep for more than 20 s, the trainer stopped the movement by placing himself between the dog and the sheep). The dogs could then resume their rotation movement in the same or the reverse direction around the sheep (see Figure 1).
During the dogs’ spontaneous rotation movements around the herd, the trainer remained motionless to prevent any influence on the subjects’ behavior, but he intervened just to interrupt the dogs’ rotation movements (each 20 s) or to prevent aggression. The sheep were always the same during the tests, and they were familiar with sheepdog activities. The tests took place on three different days, during which 5 dogs were tested per day. The daily interactions of the dogs with the sheep were performed with 30 min intervals in order to give the sheep time to rest and recuperate.

2.3. Video Analysis

The recorded videos were scanned frame by frame by two trained experimenters. The following parameters, which included specific prey-driven behaviors, were analyzed [2]:
Turning preference: The total time (in s) spent turning clockwise and counterclockwise with respect to the sheep during the working session; and dogs’ behavior: Straight approach—the dog approached the sheep along a straight trajectory, shoving—the dog pushed one sheep using its shoulder, gripping—the dog approached the sheep aggressively gripping and pulling its leg wool, approach direction—indicates the dog’s approach direction toward the sheep (lateral, frontal, or backside approach), and sidedness—indicates the dogs’ side (left or right) on which the sheep was positioned in the lateral approach.
The frequency of straight approaches, shoving, and gripping during the working session was computed for each dog, and the direction and sidedness of each approach was described. Therefore, the total frequency of the above-mentioned behaviors, as well as the total frequency of the different direction of the dog’s approach toward the sheep (lateral, frontal, backside) were then obtained and analyzed.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Data distribution was tested using the Shapiro–Wilk test; subsequently, the paired-samples t-test and the Wilcoxon signed-rank test were used to test parametric and nonparametric data, respectively. For all statistical tests, SPSS software was used, and the results were considered significant if p < 0.05.

2.5. Ethics Statement

The experiments were conducted in accordance with directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council and were approved by the Department of Veterinary Medicine’s (University of Bari) Ethics Committee, EC (Approval Number: 12/17); in addition, before the experiment began, informed consent was obtained from all the participants included in the study.

3. Results

3.1. Dogs’ Turning Preference and Behavior

The relationship between lateralized turning motor pattern and sheepdog behavior is shown in Figure 2. Results revealed that gripping behavior occurred with a higher frequency during counterclockwise turns around the sheep compared to during the clockwise turns (clockwise turns: 1.06 ± 1.79; counterclockwise turns: 2.00 ± 3.07; z = 80.00, p = 0.016). In addition, when dogs ran in a circle around the flock in a counterclockwise direction, their approach to the sheep occurred significantly more along a straight trajectory (clockwise turns: 3.73 ± 0.53; counterclockwise turns: 5.33 ± 2.05; t(14) = −2.323, p = 0.036). Regarding shoving behavior, it is important to note that it occurred only on four occasions, and, in particular, three times during clockwise turns and only once during counterclockwise turns. No significant statistical differences were found in the total time spent by dogs turning clockwise and counterclockwise around the herd during sessions (clockwise: 53.54 ± 7.64 s; counterclockwise: 56.55 ± 8.47 s; t(14) = −0.340, p = 0.739). Dogs approached the sheep more frequently from their backside (87.5%) compared to their lateral (7.8%), and frontal (4.7%) sides. Regarding backside approaches, statistical analysis revealed that they occurred significantly more during counterclockwise than during clockwise turns (1.00 ± 0.37 and 2.73 ± 0.67, respectively; z = 87.00, p = 0.003). Although a slight tendency to approach sheep placed on dogs’ left side was found it did not reach a statistical significance (right vs. left: Clockwise turns: z = 18.00, p = 1.00; counterclockwise turns: z = 13.00, p = 0.579; straight approaches: z = 36.00, p = 0.383; gripping behavior: t(14) = 0.904, p = 0.381).

3.2. Sex Ratio

The statistical analysis revealed that female dogs approached the sheep significantly more along a straight trajectory (clockwise turns: 3.22 ± 0.98; counterclockwise turns: 5.77 ± 0.52; t(8) = −3.603, p = 0.007) and from their backside (backside approach: clockwise turns: 1.11 ± 0.53; counterclockwise turns: 2.88 ± 0.97; z = 280.00, p = 0.017) during the counterclockwise turns than during clockwise turns. No differences were found for male dogs (straight approach: t(5) = −0.139, p = 0.895; backside approach: z = 18.00, p = 0.096).
No other statistically significant differences between clockwise turns and counterclockwise turns were found in male and female dogs for the other analyzed parameters: gripping (male: z = 7.00, p = 0.461; female: z = 16.00, p = 0.236) and turning preference (male: t(5) = −0.671, p = 0.532; female: t(8) = 0.414, p = 0.690).

3.3. Age

The statistical analysis showed more backside approaches by adult dogs compared to younger ones (backside approach (2–3 years: z = 10.00, p = 0.059; 4–8 years: z = 42.50, p = 0.016)). The analysis showed no other significant differences between clockwise and counterclockwise turns in the analyzed parameters according to the dogs’ age: Gripping (2–3 years: z = 4.50, p = 0.414; 4–8 years: z = 19.50, p = 0.344) and straight approach (2–3 years: z = 9.00, p = 0.144; 4–8 years: z = 28.00, p = 0.160).

4. Discussion

The most relevant finding of this research was that the expression of the dogs’ aggressive behavior toward the sheep is lateralized. Specifically, among the aggressive behaviors scored, the gripping and the straight approach toward the sheep occurred with a higher frequency when the dogs ran in a circle around the livestock in a counterclockwise direction. During dogs’ rotational movements the sheep were viewed mainly in their monocular peripheral vision. Considering that the lateral field of each of the dogs’ eyes projects mainly to the contralateral side of the brain (crossing of fibers at the optic nerve level is 70% [5]), the visual analysis of the herd by the left visual hemifield during counterclockwise turnings indicates a prevalent activation of the right hemisphere. This result fits with previous evidences about the specialization of right neural structures of dogs’ brains in attending to arousal stimuli [6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. Previous studies employing the head-turning paradigm reported a right hemisphere main involvement in processing visually arousing stimuli, in particular the black silhouette of a cat displaying an agonistic posture (with an arched laterally displayed body and erected tail) and a snake silhouette, which is generally considered as an alarming stimulus for most mammals [7]. Moreover, a right hemisphere dominant activity was found in the dogs’ response to arousing acoustic, olfactory, and visual stimuli [8,9,10,11]. Specifically, the dogs consistently turned their head with the left ear leading (right hemisphere activation) in response to thunderstorm playbacks and conspecific and human vocalizations eliciting intense emotions [10,14]. On the other hand, the dogs consistently used the right nostril (right hemisphere) to sniff cotton swabs impregnated with arousing odors (e.g., conspecific odors collected during a stressful situation, adrenaline, and veterinarian sweat) [8,9]. In addition, a right hemisphere main involvement was found in the processing of human faces expressing intense and arousing emotions (e.g., anger and fear) [11].
Recent studies on several vertebrates have reported a general specialization of the right hemisphere in the expression of intense emotions, including aggression, escape behavior, and fear [15]. In particular, our findings are consistent with the right hemisphere specialization for aggressive responses previously reported for several species, including chicks [16], horses [17], and toads [18], which showed more aggressive responses to other conspecifics when they were positioned on the animal’s left side than when they were on its right.
A reasonable explanation for the reported asymmetry in dogs’ aggressive behavior toward the sheep is to assume that it reflects a different activation of the two brain hemispheres during visual analysis of the target (i.e., the sheep). In addition, neurobiological studies on rats reported that the preferred direction of rotation was contralateral to the brain hemisphere with higher levels of dopamine [19], a neurotransmitter directly involved in motor control and emotional functioning.
Prey drive is a carnivore’s inborn behavioral pattern to pursue and capture prey, and it is a fundamental characteristic of sheepdogs. As a matter of fact, herding behavior is modified and influenced by predatory behavior [20]. Through selective breeding, humans have been able to reduce sheepdogs’ prey-driven behavior while maintaining their hunting skills. There is now evidence that the left hemisphere neural structures are better suited for the control of prey-driven behavior in dogs [21]. A similar specialization of the left hemisphere in predatory behaviors has been documented in other species, like toads [22], zebrafish [23], and black-winged stilts [24]. An interesting explanation of the prey-driven behavior control in sheepdogs during herding could be found in the inhibition exerted by the left hemisphere on aggressive behaviors, whose expression is elicited by the right hemisphere [25]. Our results support this hypothesis, since aggressive behavior occurred predominantly during the dogs’ counterclockwise rotations around the sheep (right hemisphere dominant activity). Thus, it could be hypothesized that during clockwise turning, dogs controlled the herd with the prevalent use of the left hemisphere (specialized for predatory behavior), which plays a main role in sustaining the subject’s attention and in risk taking, by inhibiting a fast and emotive response mediated by the right hemisphere activation [25]. This hypothesis is supported by dogs’ tendency (but not statistically significant) to express “controlled” prey-driven behaviors toward the sheep during clockwise turns (left hemisphere activation). The left hemisphere activity and its functions are fundamental for the successful pursuit and capture of prey [25]. The sheep’s presence led the dogs’ arousal to increase, causing a right hemisphere activation, which regulates the expression of aggressive behaviors (shown in counterclockwise turns). In particular, the dogs’ perception of arousing stimuli (the sheep) and the intense emotions that the sheep’s presence elicited in the tested animals resulted in the dogs’ aggressive response toward the herd.
On the other hand, it could be possible that the dogs’ right hemisphere activation was elicited by the trainer’s presence that prevented dogs from attacking the sheep. In other words, the trainer’s presence could have increased the dogs’ arousal levels producing a conflicting inner state caused by the dogs’ ambivalent attitude to approaching the sheep (left hemisphere) and to withdrawing from them (right hemisphere) because of the trainer’s presence. As a result of this conflicting situation, the dogs’ stress levels (arousal) increased and the right hemisphere took control of dogs’ flight or fight behavioral response.
Apart from improving the training techniques of sheepdogs, our results contribute to defining novel parameters for the assessment of animals’ emotions, which could have a potential impact on their welfare. In particular, given the right–left hemisphere specializations, the evaluation of lateralized behavioral responses to an environmental stimulus, which reflects the activation of one hemisphere, could provide information about an animal’s emotional state. Dominance by the right hemisphere suggests that the animal perceives the stimulus or the situation as arousing (or stressing) and potentially leads to the expression of intense emotional expressions, including aggression [16,17]. Therefore, the assessment of lateralized patterns could help to determine whether an animal experiences a certain situation or event as positive or negative and, at the same time, it could be useful to improve and ensure human safety during interactions with the dogs.
Overall our results revealed the existence of a relationship between motor lateralization and aggressive behavior in dogs during sheepdog training, supporting previous evidences about the influence of brain lateralization on visually guided motor responses in dogs. These results have direct implications for both the personnel involved in the selection of dogs to be trained for herding and for the development of new training techniques.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.S., D.B., S.d., and A.Q.; methodology, M.S., D.B., S.d., and A.Q.; formal analysis, M.S., S.d., and A.Q.; investigation, M.S., D.B., S.d., and A.Q.; data curation, M.S., D.B., S.d., and A.Q.; writing—original draft preparation, M.S., S.d., and A.Q.

Funding

This research is part of the following project: “Lateralizzazione e disturbi del comportamento nel cane” supported by the University of Bari, Italy, through a financial grant to M.S.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Fabrizio Pertosa for technical assistance during the experiment.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. The International Sheep Dog Society-Rules for Trials. Available online: https://www.isds.org.uk/trials/sheepdog-trials/rules-for-trials/ (accessed on 12 December 2018).
  2. Handelman, B. Canine Behavior: A Photo Illustrated Handbook; Dogwise Publishing: Wenatchee, WA, USA, 2012; ISBN 0976511827. [Google Scholar]
  3. Siniscalchi, M.; Bertino, D.; Quaranta, A. Laterality and performance of agility-trained dogs. Laterality 2014, 19, 219–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Siniscalchi, M.; d’Ingeo, S.; Fornelli, S.; Quaranta, A. Relationship between visuospatial attention and paw preference in dogs. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 31682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Fogle, B. The Dog’s Mind; Pelham Editions: London, UK, 1992; p. 203. ISBN 072071964X. [Google Scholar]
  6. Quaranta, A.; Siniscalchi, M.; Vallortigara, G. Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, R199–R201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Siniscalchi, M.; Sasso, R.; Pepe, A.M.; Vallortigara, G.; Quaranta, A. Dogs turn left to emotional stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 208, 516–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Siniscalchi, M.; Sasso, R.; Pepe, A.M.; Dimatteo, S.; Vallortigara, G.; Quaranta, A. Sniffing with right nostril: Lateralization of response to odour stimuli by dogs. Anim. Behav. 2011, 82, 399–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Siniscalchi, M.; d’Ingeo, S.; Quaranta, A. The dog nose “KNOWS” fear: Asymmetric nostril use during sniffing at canine and human emotional stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 2016, 304, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Siniscalchi, M.; d’Ingeo, S.; Fornelli, S.; Quaranta, A. Lateralized behavior and cardiac activity of dogs in response to human emotional vocalizations. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Siniscalchi, M.; d’Ingeo, S.; Quaranta, A. Orienting asymmetries and physiological reactivity in dogs’ response to human emotional faces. Learn. Behav. 2018, 46, 574–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Racca, A.; Guo, K.; Meints, K.; Mills, D.S. Reading faces: Differential lateral gaze bias in processing canine and human facial expressions in dogs and 4-year-old children. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e36076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Siniscalchi, M.; Lusito, R.; Vallortigara, G.; Quaranta, A. Seeing left- or right-asymmetric tail wagging produces different emotional responses in dogs. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 2279–2282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Siniscalchi, M.; Quaranta, A.; Rogers, L.J. Hemispheric specialization in dogs for processing different acoustic stimuli. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e3349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Rogers, L.J.; Andrew, R. Comparative Vertebrate Lateralization; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002; ISBN 0-521-78161-2. [Google Scholar]
  16. Vallortigara, G.; Cozzutti, C.; Tommasi, L.; Rogers, L.J. How birds use their eyes: Opposite left-right specialization for the lateral and frontal visual hemifield in the domestic chick. Curr. Biol. 2001, 11, 29–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Austin, N.P.; Rogers, L.J. Limb preferences and lateralization of aggression, reactivity and vigilance in feral horses, Equus caballus. Anim. Behav. 2012, 83, 239–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Robins, A.; Rogers, L.J. Lateralized prey-catching responses in the cane toad, Bufo marinus: Analysis of complex visual stimuli. Anim. Behav. 2004, 68, 767–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Carlson, J.N.; Glick, S.D.; Hinds, P.A.; Baird, J.L. Food deprivation alters dopamine utilization in the rat prefrontal cortex and asymmetrically alters amphetamine-induced rotational behavior. Brain Res. 1988, 454, 373–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Renna, C.H. Herding Dogs: Selection and Training the Working Farm Dog; Kennel Club Books (KCB): Allenhurst, NJ, USA, 2008; ISBN 978-1-59378-737-0. [Google Scholar]
  21. Siniscalchi, M.; Pergola, G.; Quaranta, A. Detour behaviour in attack-trained dogs: Left-turners perform better than right-turners. Laterality 2013, 18, 282–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Lippolis, G.; Bisazza, A.; Rogers, L.J.; Vallortigara, G. Lateralization of predator avoidance responses in three species of toads. Laterality 2002, 7, 163–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Miklósi, Á.; Andrew, R.J.; Savage, H. Behavioural lateralisation of the tetrapod type in the zebrafish (Brachydanio rerio). Physiol. Behav. 1998, 63, 127–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Ventolini, N.; Ferrero, E.A.; Sponza, S.; Della Chiesa, A.; Zucca, P.; Vallortigara, G. Laterality in the wild: Preferential hemifield use d uring predatory and sexual behaviour in the black-winged stilt. Anim. Behav. 2005, 69, 1077–1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Rogers, L.J.; Vallortigara, G.; Andrew, R.J. Divided Brains: The Biology and Behaviour of Brain Asymmetries; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013; ISBN 0521540119. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Schematic representation of the positions of the dog, the trainer, and the sheep during the session and an example of a dog making a counterclockwise turn around the herd.
Figure 1. Schematic representation of the positions of the dog, the trainer, and the sheep during the session and an example of a dog making a counterclockwise turn around the herd.
Symmetry 11 00233 g001
Figure 2. Dogs’ behavior during clockwise and counterclockwise turns (means with SEM are shown; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01).
Figure 2. Dogs’ behavior during clockwise and counterclockwise turns (means with SEM are shown; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01).
Symmetry 11 00233 g002
Table 1. Subjects’ characteristics and their behavioral score (BS) during clockwise and counterclockwise turns. The BS was computed for each subject allocating a score of 1 for each of the behaviors analyzed.
Table 1. Subjects’ characteristics and their behavioral score (BS) during clockwise and counterclockwise turns. The BS was computed for each subject allocating a score of 1 for each of the behaviors analyzed.
DogSexNeuteredAge (years)Weight (Kg)BreedBS ClockwiseBS Counterclockwise
Ambrafemaleno619Border Collie06
Chobilmaleno421Border Collie57
Euforiafemaleno323Belgian Tervuren1426
Gokumaleno421Border Collie154
Gullivermaleno427Belgian Malinois625
Mimifemaleno624Belgian Groenendael1116
Nabifemaleyes422Belgian Malinois07
Smogmaleno317Mongrel77
Tulifemaleno222Belgian Groenendael38
Zoefemaleno316Border Collie27
Geppifemaleyes617Smooth Collie1013
Junomaleyes616Australian Kelpie511
Rustermaleyes317Border Collie56
Vickifemaleyes710Mongrel77
Vickyfemaleyes820Australian Shepherd46

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Siniscalchi, M.; Bertino, D.; d’Ingeo, S.; Quaranta, A. Relationship between Motor Laterality and Aggressive Behavior in Sheepdogs. Symmetry 2019, 11, 233. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym11020233

AMA Style

Siniscalchi M, Bertino D, d’Ingeo S, Quaranta A. Relationship between Motor Laterality and Aggressive Behavior in Sheepdogs. Symmetry. 2019; 11(2):233. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym11020233

Chicago/Turabian Style

Siniscalchi, Marcello, Daniele Bertino, Serenella d’Ingeo, and Angelo Quaranta. 2019. "Relationship between Motor Laterality and Aggressive Behavior in Sheepdogs" Symmetry 11, no. 2: 233. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym11020233

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop