Next Article in Journal
Herbicide Resistance to Metsulfuron-Methyl in Rumex dentatus L. in North-West India and Its Management Perspectives for Sustainable Wheat Production
Next Article in Special Issue
The Evolving Economic Employment of ICT Education: The Case of Norway
Previous Article in Journal
An Analysis of Factors Affecting the Severity of Cycling Crashes Using Binary Regression Model
Previous Article in Special Issue
COVID-19 in the Field of Education: State of the Art
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Emotional Cognitive Regulation in University Students during Lockdown: A Comparative Analysis of Students from Spanish Universities

by
Mercedes Cuevas López
1,
Inmaculada Ávalos Ruiz
2 and
Emilio Jesús Lizarte Simón
2,*
1
Department of Didactics and School Organization, Faculty of Education, Economics and Technology of Ceuta, University of Granada, 51001 Ceuta, Spain
2
Department of Didactics and School Organization, Faculty of Education Sciences, University of Granada, 18010 Granada, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2021, 13(12), 6946; https://doi.org/10.3390/su13126946
Submission received: 14 May 2021 / Revised: 6 June 2021 / Accepted: 8 June 2021 / Published: 21 June 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Digital Competences for a Sustainable Society)

Abstract

:
Studies focusing on strategies for the cognitive regulation of emotions are gaining importance due to the development and perpetuation of psychopathologies. The obligatory home confinement imposed in response to the COVID-19 pandemic has led to new virtual learning methodologies. Objective: Our objective aimed to analyze and compare the cognitive emotional regulation of students from universities on the Spanish mainland with that of students attending the Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria. Methods: An online Emotional Regulation Questionnaire was applied, together with a survey covering the students’ beliefs about the pandemic, including information about their housing conditions and beliefs about online learning. The study included a sample of 1030 university students. Results: On the mainland and at Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, the students most frequently used adaptive strategies. Three of the strategies were used in both groups but to different extents (Acceptance, Positive reappraisal, Putting into perspective), while the other strategies were used in both groups to the same extent (Refocusing on planning, Positive refocusing, Rumination, Blaming others, Catastrophizing, Self-blame). Meanwhile, the results were quite similar regarding the students’ housing conditions and beliefs about the pandemic and online learning.

1. Introduction

The management of emotions is among the most important activities in everyday life. Emotional regulation influences a series of processes that systematize emotions, and it manifests itself as a conglomerate of different mechanisms that can modify, model, or suppress an affective state [1,2], attending to precedents or elements of responses [3]. When people do not properly regulate their emotions, the resulting uneasiness can be lasting and lead to different symptoms [4,5]. Accordingly, the regulation of emotions can either be positive or negative, depending on the context and a person’s interests [6]. A variety of psychological problems can interfere with emotional regulation, such as anxiety, depression, and post-traumatic stress disorder [7,8]. Emotional regulation models include some that are based on strategies. Such strategies may be adaptive, for example, those associated with psychological processes, like acceptance, or maladaptive, for example, those related to rumination. Flexibility is a key factor when putting those strategies into practice [9]. Effective emotional regulation is related to such positive effects as well-being and proper work performance, whereas ineffective emotional regulation can be associated with such problems as anxiety and depression [10,11]. The current consensus regards emotional regulation as a process in which an individual can contribute to the type of emotion, the moment it occurs, and the way it is expressed [8,12]. For Gross [13], emotional regulation can affect a subject’s well-being by means of two processes. The first process is based on the formulation of the emotion and on regulating the precedents of the emotional process, locating the cognitive restructuring processes in reevaluating a situation—referred to as essential strategies [14]. The second process is based on the involvement in the emotional response, leading to an alteration in said response, for example, by being able to reduce negative emotions and increasing positive ones [13,14]. Here it is worth highlighting resilience from among other psychological factors that can affect emotional regulation [15]. Resilience is regarded as a group of positive personal qualities that can improve the individual subject’s adaptation to adverse situations and contexts [15,16]. Starting with Gross’ process model [13], which relevantly explains emotional regulation strategies, we can highlight the main stages in the emotional generation process laid out by Pérez et al. [13] (p. 116): (a) presentation of situations or stimuli, (b) identification and meaning of the same, (c) choice of techniques for handling those evaluations and, lastly, (d) implementation of responses for facilitating the desired change. Gross’ process model [13] (p. 116) includes different categories of emotional regulation strategies in the various stages of the process: (a) choice of the situation and modification of that situation, (b) attention deployment, (c) cognitive reevaluation, and (d) modification of the response.
Stress is currently one of the most frequently studied psychosocial factors arising from the new measures and adaptations related to COVID-19 [17,18]. Stress is regarded as a behavioral, psychological, or physiological response to a situation or stimulus that can be considered a risk [19,20]. Such response can cause difficulties associated with adaptation and the performance of activities. This focus factors in a number of variables related to the description above, such as “academic-related stress”, “subjective stress experiences”, “academic stress moderators”, and “academic stress effects” [21]. Academic activities carried out in the university environment can lead to anxiety and stress that, in turn, can affect students’ psychological and physical well-being [22]. In the university context, studies such as those in [21,23,24] and [25] show that anxiety and stress can affect a student’s academic performance. In [26], it is shown that students earning low grades may be influenced by high academic demands to feel stress.
A study by Husky [27] has shown that students who remained in their student residence halls, instead of the family home, during the lockdown had higher levels of anxiety and stress. In [28], the psychological impact of the pandemic on university students in Ethiopia is analyzed, highlighting a constant increase in the levels of stress, anxiety, and depression. Similar data can be seen in [29], a study on North American students showing an increase in stress, especially among female students. In the Spanish context, it has been shown, in [30], that university students with the greatest emotional cognitive regulation difficulties are the most susceptible to suffering possible effects on their studies and a possible increase in stress. As for the general population, González-Sanguino [31] provided evidence that women with symptoms associated with COVID-19 or a close family member affected by the virus have a higher level of depression and anxiety.
Academic persistence is a specific virtue that can be altered by anxiety and depression. Such persistence can also be affected by a student’s academic and social integration, which, in turn, affects one’s sense of belonging, academic commitment, and self-efficacy, all of which are involved in one’s academic performance [32]. Depression and anxiety are associated with academic performance [33], and they regularly inhibit that performance [34]. We should also note that university changes can be stressful for students, leading to a reduced academic persistence [35]. Some factors associated with academic persistence include students’ ability to build up and maintain the kind of personal resources, like support relationships, that can arise at university [35,36,37]. Thus, with the current provisional virtual learning model, a student’s attitude and motivation could be directly or indirectly affected, especially in online learning environments that were not implemented at the beginning of the school year. Nevertheless, studies, such as [38] (p. 1) show that students’ “attitude to online learning was not found to mediate their intrinsic motivation to make accomplishments and develop relationships. A negative mediation effect was partially supported by amotivation and engagement”.
Given all the aforementioned information, it is clear that there is a wide range of references in the literature showing that psychological well-being is a factor that can affect persistence and early university dropout possibilities, and that the lockdown situation can cause stress [23,30,39]. We believe that this is a good time to analyze and compare the emotional cognitive regulation of students from mainland Spanish universities with that of students from the Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria. It is worth noting that between the 2001–2002 and 2017–2018 school years, the Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria expanded considerably, with a 33.2% increase in new student enrollments [40]. University mobility within the Canary Island community is particularly expensive, given the impossibility of land-based transportation. Any movement between islands or provinces must be done by air or sea, increasing the expense of transport fees, thereby making daily transportation difficult. Thus, displacements must be permanent and not temporary, bringing with them the added cost of residing outside the family home. 95.5% of the students newly entering the ULPGC (Las Palmas de Gran Canaria University) in the 2012–13 period were born in Spain, with over half of the rest from Latin America. [41] (p254.): Of all new students (degree and post-degree) born in Latin America, 97.1% have their place of residence in the Autonomous Community of the Canary Islands.

2. Materials and Methods

A total of 1030 students from Spanish and Latin American universities participated in this research. The sample was divided into two sub-groups: students from Spanish mainland universities and students from Spanish universities at Las Palmas de Gran Canaria. In the case of the mainland universities, the sample comprised 266 women (20.05%), 1055 men (75.50%), and six missing values (0.45%). The age of the students ranged from 18 to 37 (M = 25.18, DS = 9.15). In the case of the Universidad de las Palmas de Gran Canaria, the sample comprised 168 women (29%), 409 men (70%), and six missing values (1%). The age of the students ranged from 18 to 35 (M = 23.58, DT = 7.67).

2.1. Procedure

An invitation was sent to university professors, explaining the general objective of the research and requesting their voluntary collaboration. Interested professors helped to send the instrument to the students online during class hours. It was explained to those students that their participation was strictly voluntary and that all their information would remain confidential. The questionnaire was applied at the end of March 2020 in the Spanish universities.

2.2. Measurement Instruments

The CC/COVID-19 adaptation of Cruz [30] is an instrument that was designed specifically for this study. It focuses on discovering the beliefs of university students about the pandemic and the housing conditions of the place where they spent their home confinement. The primary objective of the instrument is to evaluate the possibility of implementing online learning. Some of the questions were answered by means of a Likert scale, while others were answered by a dichotomous reply.
This questionnaire was administered at the same time as the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire [42,43,44], adapted from [45], the adjustment of which was developed according to the guidelines of the International Test Commission [46], using a retro-translation method based on the original English version. It consists of nine scales, each with four items and conceptually different: Self-blame, Acceptance, Rumination, Positive refocusing, Refocus on planning, Positive reappraisal, Putting into perspective, Catastrophizing, and Blaming others. The instrument contains nine dimensions, with one per strategy: five are adaptive, i.e., they help adapt to the situation, and four are maladaptive, i.e., they make managing the experience more difficult. This instrument is made up of five dimensions: Beliefs about the Covid-19 pandemic, Beliefs about the effectiveness of virtual learning, Social conditions of the lockdown and description of the cohabitation situation, Description of the lockdown housing, and the Perception of the suitability of the lockdown housing. Some of the questions required answers according to a Likert scale, while others were of dichotomous reply type. This information was collected through 36 items, which were answered by means of a five-point Likert scale (from 1 = almost never to 5 = almost always), making it possible to know how often each individual uses each of the strategies.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Analysis, CC/COVID-19

Table 1 shows the beliefs of Canary Island students concerning the pandemic in comparison with the overall sample. While the results point to the same conclusions, the Canary Island sample shows slightly higher scores for most items. When asked whether they believe themselves to be capable of spreading the virus to older people, the Canary Island students seemed to be more aware of this (4.14) than the general sample (3.87). Similarly, when asked about their immunity to the virus, the Canary Island students showed an average score of 1.43, compared with the 1.46 figure for the general sample. This would seem to indicate that the Canary Island students have a higher level of awareness when it comes to this factor. Similar results were obtained when asking them if they believed that older people can transmit the virus (4.29 in the Canary Island sample and 4.06 in general). As for government actions, we can see that the Canary Island sample shows a higher score (2.81) than the general sample (2.71), with students denying, in both cases, that the authorities knew how to act properly, although it is also clear that there was less agreement regarding this. Finally, on the question related to the health system’s preparation for confronting the pandemic, the scores were practically the same (1.96 in the Canary Islands and 1.97 in the general sample), showing a lack of preparation.
In addition to their beliefs concerning the pandemic, the students’ evaluation of virtual learning—to which they were suddenly subjected—is worthy of note. These items took different agents into consideration, including the university itself, the professors, and the students themselves. Regarding the university, it is interesting to see that the Canary Island population thought their university is not as well prepared (2.81 compared to 3.01 in the general sample), while believing, at the same time, that the professors had more resources for virtual teaching. Indeed, that item obtaining a score of 3.03, compared to the general sample figure of 3.02. Apart from the resources, when asked about the preparation of teachers for giving online classes, the Canary Island students showed a score of 2.19, while the general sample showed a score of 2.58. As for their personal skills in dealing with online classes, the Canary Island students regarded themselves as more competent (4.06), digitally speaking, than the general sample (3.64). In regard to the planning of study time, the Canary Island sample showed a 2.87 score, which is very close to the general average of 2.96. All of this information can be seen in Table 2.
Table 3 brings together information on the social conditions of the lockdown and a description of the cohabitation situation.
The average number of people in any given home confinement situation varied between 3 and 4 (3.49 for the Canary Island group and a bit higher (3.62) for the general group). The minimum was 1 (student confined at home and alone), with a maximum of 10 for the Canary Island students and 13 for the rest.
The vast majority of students were confined, in both cases, to the family residence (84% and 83% for Canary Island and general students, respectively) and did not cohabitate with people older than 70 (regarded as a population at risk) nor with pets that need to be walked, which would justify a daily and momentary break from confinement.
As for the housing conditions in the residence where the students spent the lockdown (compiled in Table 4), it is worth highlighting that the vast majority, both in the Canary Island sample (72.2) and in the general sample (74.9), spent the lockdown in a dwelling located in an urban area, generally with the possibility of enjoying the outside without leaving the home. In the case of the Canary Island group, 30.2% of the students had no outside space beyond the home, whereas the figure for the general sample was 20.1%. On another front, 13.6% of the students in the general sample did not have any outside views, whereas 9.1% of the surveyed Canary Island students suffered from this situation.
Table 5 brings together the perceptions on the suitability of the dwelling where the students spent the lockdown. The scores are quite similar for the Canary Island and general samples, although with slight differences. The great majority of the students (87.7% of the Canary Island sample and 89% of the general sample) spent the lockdown in a dwelling they considered to be comfortable. Only 15.4% of the Canary Island group and 11.4% of the general group thought that their dwelling was oppressive. Almost all of the surveyed students had the ability to use WiFi in their home, with 94% of the Canary Island students and 92% of the general sample. As for their studies, 20.9% of the Canary Island sample and 20% of the general sample opined that their dwelling was not properly equipped for this purpose. Regarding the possibility of carrying out physical exercise and having the required equipment available, almost half of the Canary Island sample (49.4%) had what was needed, while this figure was 44.9% for the general group. Finally, as for entertainment, most students in the Canary Island group (63.6%) and the general group (64.6%) said that they had the equipment needed for entertainment in the dwelling where they spent the lockdown.

3.2. Descriptive Analysis of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ)

After applying the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, in Table 6, we present the main descriptive characteristics, broken down into nine groups for each of the cognitive strategies analyzed.
Below, we present the nine strategies used in the general and Canary Island samples, which are ordered according to the scores obtained, with a maximum of four points. One can see that the order in the two samples is quite similar, with changes in the three most commonly used strategies, while the others appear in the same order, albeit with different scores. This may be due, in the case of the Canary Island sample, to the feeling caused by the students’ insularity, which allows them to put their experience into perspective more easily, while in the general sample it would seem easier to accept the situation.
Regarding the Canary Island sample, Table 7 shows the three most commonly used strategies to be Putting into Perspective (3.52), a strategy by which one compares one’s situation with another’s which is worse, making their own situation seem milder; Acceptance (3.38), which consists in accepting the situation and thus avoiding judgments about having the same and associated feelings; and finally, Positive Reappraisal (3.29), which is the attempt to focus on the possible positive effects that might arise from the experience. The following strategies—which are still above the average score—are Refocusing on Planning, which consists in confronting the situation by thinking of ways to solve the problem (3.05); Positive Refocusing, which involves diverting one’s attention toward more agreeable thoughts when feeling stress from an experience (3 points); Rumination, by which one’s thoughts focus excessively on the lockdown situation (2.9); and Blaming Others, which involves making others responsible for the situation (2.51). Catastrophizing (1.86), which involves focusing one’s attention on the most negative aspects, and Self-blame (1.31), that is, feeling that one is responsible for everything being experienced, showed below-average scores.
Table 8, meanwhile, brings together data from the general sample. Here, we can see that the first three strategies are the same, yet they are ordered differently. First, we have Acceptance, with a score of 3.4. Second and third are Positive Reappraisal and Putting into Perspective, both with 3.39 points. In terms of their scores, the other strategies line up in the same order as that observed in the case of the Canary Island sample: Refocusing on Planning (3.14), Positive Refocusing (3.11), Rumination (2.96), Blaming Others (2.57), Catastrophizing (2.05), and Self-blame (1.44). As in the Canary Islands sample, the last strategies obtained below-average scores.
After finding which of the strategies were the most commonly used in each group, we decided to apply the t-Student test to check which of the differences were significant.
The results shown in Table 9 allow us to ascertain that there are significant differences (p > 0.05) in four of the strategies used by both groups: the differences in Self-blame, Positive Refocusing, and Catastrophizing are in favor of the general group; and the differences in Putting into Perspective are in favor of the Canary Island students.

3.3. Cognitive Strategies and Lockdown Conditions

Table 10 shows the results of both questionnaires related to the cognitive strategies used by the students and certain situations that make home confinement more tolerable. These situations include living with a pet that needs to be walked (this being among those circumstances that justified leaving one’s home), having WiFi available at one’s home (which reduces the feeling of isolation through the sense of being connected to the outside world), having good physical exercise equipment available (making it possible to maintain a healthy lifestyle), and having good entertainment equipment (as a basic part of an individual’s mental health).
Of the nine strategies used by the students, those that were to a greater or lesser extent affected were the following: Blaming oneself in the case of the availability or lack thereof of WiFi in the home in the general group (p < 0.01); the Rumination strategy correlated with the availability of WiFi (p < 0.05), or good entertainment equipment (p < 0.01) in the Canary Island student group; Positive refocusing was related to having a pet (p < 0.05) in both groups, along with having good physical exercise and entertainment equipment available (p < 0.01); Refocusing on planning was also correlated with good exercise and entertainment equipment in both groups at this level (p < 0.01); Positive reappraisal showed a statistically significant relation (p < 0.05) to the availability of WiFi for Canary Island students and to good exercise and the availability of entertainment equipment for both groups (p < 0.01); Putting into perspective was related to the availability of WiFi (p < 0.05) among the Canary Island students and to the availability of physical exercise and entertainment equipment for both groups (p < 0.05 y p < 0.01); Catastrophizing correlated in both groups with WiFi availability (p < 0.05 for the Canary Island students and p < 0.01 for the general group), while a relation with good physical exercise equipment was only seen in the Canary Island group, and good entertainment equipment was related to this strategy in both groups (p < 0.01); finally, the strategy of Blaming others seemed to be related to good entertainment equipment in the Canary Island group (p < 0.05) and, in the general group, to living with a pet (p < 0.05) and having WiFi available (p < 0.01).
Next, we investigated whether these special lockdown conditions caused some sort of differential response when employing the different cognitive strategies.
First, we checked if the greater lockdown flexibility, which is associated with having to go out several times a day to walk the pet, results in a significant difference between those who have a pet and those who do not. In order to determine the independence of the results for both groups, and after checking for compliance with the requirements of normalcy and equality of variances, the t-Student test was applied. The main values are shown in Table 11 and Table 12.
Given the above results, we can conclude that being able to walk a pet did not affect the cognitive strategies used, as none of the differences seen are at a level of significance below 5%.
Similarly, the next step was to analyze whether the availability of a WiFi network led to the use of different cognitive strategies by those students who had such networks and those who did not and if these differences were significant. The main results after applying the t test are shown in Table 13 and Table 14.
Table 13 allows us to see if, in the case of students from the Canary Islands, the use of such strategies as Rumination, Positive refocusing, Refocusing on planning, Positive reappraisal, Putting into perspective, and Catastrophizing was affected by the availability or unavailability of a WiFi network. The differences found (p < 0.05) provide evidence of a correlation. As for the students in the general sample (Table 14), significant differences were found in relation to the use of such strategies as Positive refocusing, Refocusing on planning (at 0.05), Self-blame, Catastrophizing, and Blaming others, at a level of 0.01.
In the case of students who were confined in a home with good physical exercise equipment, significant differences could be seen in the use of some of the cognitive strategies being analyzed. Table 15 shows the results obtained for the Canary Island student sample.
Table 16 allows us to see if, in the case of students in the general sample, significant differences appear when using strategies including Positive refocusing, Refocusing on planning, Positive reappraisal (p < 0.01), and Putting into perspective (p < 0.05).
Finally, we looked at whether having had good entertainment equipment at home during the lockdown affected the way different cognitive strategies were employed. With the Canary Island students, Table 17 shows that the differences found were significant at a level of 0.01 in the use of Rumination, Positive refocusing, Refocusing on planning, Positive reappraisal, Putting into perspective, and Catastrophizing, while the strategy of blaming others was only significant to a level of 0.05.
In the group of students included in the general sample (Table 18), spending the lockdown in a home with good entertainment equipment displayed significant differences (p < 0.01) in terms of the use of the following strategies: Positive refocusing, Refocusing on planning, Positive reappraisal, Putting into Perspective, and Catastrophizing.

4. Discussion

The home confinement measures imposed due to COVID-19 have had ongoing and damaging effects on university students, leading to more intense and long-lasting emotions [47]. In this situation, the proper regulation of emotions is associated with a number of positive effects, such as a better performance at work, well-being, and prosperity in personal and professional relations [11]. Nevertheless, these same strategies can have negative effects if used in a maladaptive manner, such as an increase in anxiety, depression, and other symptoms [10,48]. In this context, we decided to analyze and compare the general emotional regulation strategies among Spanish university students on the mainland and at the Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, along with the students’ beliefs about the pandemic and the living conditions in the dwellings where they spent the lockdown.
Using the “Emotional Regulation Questionnaire”, nine cognitive strategies were analyzed, showing significant differences between the two groups of students in some of these strategies. Significant differences were seen in such strategies as “Self-blame”, “Positive refocusing”, “Putting into perspective”, and “Catastrophizing” in terms of the averages for the students in general. The least frequently used strategies for both groups were “Self-blame”, “Catastrophizing”, “Blaming others”, and “Rumination”, which are all associated with negative effects and emotional interference [9]. The most commonly used strategies for both groups were “Acceptance”, “Positive reappraisal”, and “Putting into perspective”—strategies that are not associated with negative effects [49]. We believe, as in [49], that the abovementioned strategies call for a more rational information process, which would allow for a more solid application. Moreover, the coping strategies (in our case, “Acceptance”, “Positive reappraisal”, and “Putting into perspective”) can activate the coping mechanism through cognitive strategies and behaviors in order to resist anxiety in interactions with environmental factors [30,50].
With the results obtained, we can show that a higher degree of emotional regulation is related to a lower experience of stress factors, in accordance with the research of [51]. We believe that the ability to manage less adaptive emotions significantly favors interpersonal relations, while decreasing the number of controversies as the result of one’s preparedness for academic and social contexts [51,52].
Every student has their own way of confronting the social isolation resulting from the lockdown, as they must deal with uncertainty, anxiety, and worries caused by the situation [53]. Family support becomes an important element that can help to lessen some of the less adaptive emotions. Throughout the home confinement required by the COVID-19 pandemic, not all young people had the same life conditions or adequate access to technology, nor did they have similar behaviors and skills for confronting those situations [54,55]. Ref. [56] shows that families who are resistant to painful situations—such as the lockdown resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic—are capable of making continual adaptations and changes [56]. In our research, confinement in a home with WiFi availability and good entertainment equipment led to changes in emotional regulation, highlighting adaptive strategies. Accordingly, we can show that such changes may well be due to family influence and support, among other causes, such that resilient families could demonstrate a higher level of adaptation to the changes and greater life satisfaction, leading to a better ability to adapt to new academic situations on the part of the student [57,58].
Based on the claims of [6] that emotions may be positive or negative depending on the context and interest required, we coincide with [59] in obtaining evidence that positive emotions foster relations with others, and that participating in productive tasks and leisure activities increases well-being, along with the ability to adapt.
Our results are similar to the ones obtained in the research carried out by [60], showing that a student’s effort to adapt to a new learning model demands more commitment. This situation calls for more management of emotions, considering such variables as social isolation, internet access, the economic context, and the general uneasiness associated with the pandemic [61]. Based on our research, we coincide with [62] in thinking that academic adaptation is not only a question of means, but also of teaching models that can best minimize the problems associated with the lack of classroom attendance. Accordingly, the university must adapt itself to a new teaching model that allows for students’ academic development. Regarding the students, the Psycho-pedagogical Department/Academic Orientation Unit must offer programs that help students to better adapt to the new model, preventing possible early dropouts caused by poor academic and social adaptation [23,39].
It is worth keeping in mind such research as that reported in [63], in which it is shown that many young people from the Canary Islands eventually develop a feeling of isolation in comparison with those on the mainland. In the case of the Canary Islands, the students’ higher levels of regulation through acceptance—as the most commonly used strategy—could be attributable to the fact that they are more accustomed to isolation and to having feelings of solitude than the students on the mainland.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.C.L. and E.J.L.S.; methodology, M.C.L. and E.J.L.S.; data collection, M.C.L., I.Á.R., and E.J.L.S.; data analysis, M.C.L., I.Á.R., and E.J.L.S.; data interpretation, M.C.L.; article writing, M.C.L., I.Á.R., and E.J.L.S.; review and editing conducted by all authors. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study has been carried out under the FEDER/Junta de Andalucía-Consejería de Economía y Conocimiento/Proyecto B-SEJ-516-UGR18 funds.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed.

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation, 2nd ed.; Gross, J.J., Ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 3–20. [Google Scholar]
  2. Koole, S.L. The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cogn. Emot. 2009, 23, 4–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Del Valle, M.V.; Zamora, E.V.; Grave, L.; Merlo, E.; Maidana, J.; Urquijo, S. El rol de las estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional en la depresión: Estudio en población universitaria. Eureka 2021, 18, 7–25. [Google Scholar]
  4. Del Valle, M.; Betegón, E.; Irurtia, M.J. Efecto del uso de estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional sobre la ansiedad en adolescentes españoles. Suma Psicol. 2018, 25, 153–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Hur, J.; Smith, J.F.; Deyoung, K.A.; Anderson, A.S.; Kuang, J.; Kim, H.C.; Tillman, R.M.; Kuhn, M.; Fox, A.S.; Shackman, A.J. Anxiety and the neurobiology of temporally uncertain threat anticipation. J. Neurosci. 2020, 40, 7949–7964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Advíncula, C. Regulación Emocional y Bienestar Psicológico en Estudiantes Universitarios. Bachelor’s Thesis, Pon-tificia Universidad Católica del Perú, Lima, Peru, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  7. Goldsmith, R.E.; Chesney, S.A.; Heath, N.M.; Barlow, M.R. Emotion regulation difficulties mediate associations between betrayal trauma and symptoms of posttraumatic stress, depression, and anxiety. J. Trauma. Stress 2013, 26, 376–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sloan, E.; Hall, K.; Moulding, R.; Bryce, S.; Mildred, H.; Staiger, P.K. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic treatment construct across anxiety, depression, substance, eating and borderline personality disorders: A systematic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 57, 141–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Medrano, L.; Moretti, L.; Ortiz, A.; Pereno, G. Validación del cuestionario de regulación emocional cognitiva en Universitarios de Córdoba, Argentina. Psykhe 2013, 22, 83–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Eftekhari, A.; Zoellner, L.A.; Vigil, S.A. Patterns of emotion regulation and psychopathology. Anxiety Stress. Coping 2009, 22, 571–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Morawetz, C.; Riedel, M.C.; Salo, T.; Berboth, S.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Laird, A.R.; Kohn, N. Multiple large-scale neural networks underlying emotion regulation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020, 116, 382–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Pérez, J.; Delgado, A.R.; Prieto, G. Propiedades psicométricas de las puntuaciones de los test más empleados en la evaluación de la regulación emocional. Papeles Psicól. 2020, 41, 116–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Gross, J.J. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev. General Psychol. 1998, 2, 271–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Limonero, J.T.; Tomás-Sábado, J.; Fernández-Castro, J.; Gómez-Romero, M.; Ardilla-Herrero, A. Estrategias deafrontamiento resilientes y regulación emocional: Predictoresde satisfacción con la vida. Behav. Psychol. 2012, 20, 183–196. [Google Scholar]
  15. Rodríguez, A.; Ramos, E.; Ros, I.; Fernández, A.; Revuelta, L. Bienestar subjetivo en la adolescencia: El papel de la resiliencia, el autoconcepto y el apoyo social percibido. Suma Psicol. 2016, 23, 60–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Connor, K.M.; Davidson, J.T. Development of a new resilience scale: The Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC). Depress. Anxiety 2003, 18, 76–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Charles, N.E.; Strong, S.J.; Burns, L.C.; Bullerjahn, M.R.; Serafine, K.M. Increased mood disorder symptoms, perceived stress, and alcohol use among college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res. 2021, 296, 113706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Gao, J.; Zheng, P.; Jia, Y.; Chen, H.; Mao, Y.; Chen, S.; Wang, Y.; Fu, H.; Dai, J. Mental health problems and social media exposure during COVID-19 outbreak. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0231924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lazarus, R.; Folkman, S. Estrés y Procesos Cognitivos; Martínez Roca: Barcelona, Spain, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  20. Restrepo, J.; Sánchez, O.A.; Castañeda Quirama, T. Estrés académico en estudiantes universitarios. Rev. Psicoespacios 2020, 14, 17–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Martín Monzón, I.M. Estrés académico en estudiantes universitarios. Apunt. Psicol. 2007, 25, 87–99. [Google Scholar]
  22. Feldman, L.; Goncalves, L.; Chacón-Puignau, G.; Zaragoza, J.; Bagés, N.; De Pablo, J. Relaciones entre estrés acadé-mico, apoyo social, salud mental y rendimiento académico en estudiantes universitarios venezolanos. Univ. Psychol. Ica 2008, 3, 739–751. [Google Scholar]
  23. Lizarte, E.J. Early dropout in college students: The influence of social integration, academic effectiveness and financial stress. In En Experiencias e Investigaciones en Contextos Educativos; Lucena, F.J.H., Ramos, F.J.S., Núñez, J.A.L., Rodríguez, J.M.R., Eds.; Dykinson: Madrid, Spain, 2020; pp. 519–531. [Google Scholar]
  24. Misra, R.; McKean, M. College student’s academic stress and its relation to their anxiety, time management and leisure satisfaction. Am. J. Health Stud. 2000, 16, 41–52. [Google Scholar]
  25. Kumaraswamy, N. Academic stress, anxiety, and depression among college students—A brief review. Int. Rev. Soc. Sci. Humanit. 2013, 5, 135–143. [Google Scholar]
  26. Knoster, K.C.; Goodboy, A.K. A conditional process model of academic demands and student learning. Commun. Educ. 2020, 69, 335–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Husky, M.M.; Kovess-Masfety, V.; Swendsen, J.D. Stress and anxiety among university students in France during Covid-19 mandatory confinement. Compr. Psychiatry 2020, 102, 152191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Aylie, N.S.; Mekonen, M.A.; Mekuria, R.M. The psychological impacts of COVID-19 pandemic among university students in Bench-Sheko zone, south-west Ethiopia: A community-based cross-sectional study. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2020, 13, 813–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Aiyer, A.; Surani, S.; Gill, Y.; Iyer, R.; Surani, Z. Mental health impact of Covid-19 on students in the USA: A cross-sectional web-based survey. J. Depress. Anxiety 2020, 5, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  30. Cruz, M.F.; Rodríguez, J.Á.; Ruiz, I.Á.; López, M.C.; de Barros Camargo, C.; Rosas, F.D.; Castellón, E.G.; González, D.G.; Fernández, A.H.; Cubillas, P.I.; et al. Evaluation of the emotional and cognitive regulation of young people in a lockdown situation due to the Covid-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 565503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. González-Sanguino, C.; Ausín, B.; Castellanos, M.Á.; Saiz, J.; López-Gómez, A.; Ugidos, C.; Muñoz, M. Mental health consequences during the initial stage of the 2020 Coronavirus pandemic (COVID-19) in Spain. Brain Behav. Immun. 2020, 87, 172–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Lizarte, E.J.; Fernández, M. Determinantes sociales e institucionales y percepción de los estudiantes de la Universidad de Granada sobre su eficacia en el estudio. In Pozo. Investigación Edu-Cativa e Inclusión. Retos Actuales en la Sociedad del Siglo XXI.; Sola, T.J.A., López, A.J., Moreno, J.M., Sola, Y.S., Eds.; Dykinson: Madrid, Spain, 2020; pp. 415–430. [Google Scholar]
  33. Ferrel-Ortega, F.R.; Vélez-Mendoza, J.; Ferrel-Ballestas, L.F. Factores psicológicos en adolescentes escolarizados con bajo rendimiento académico: Depresión y autoestima. Encuentros 2014, 12, 35–47. [Google Scholar]
  34. Rueger, S.Y.; Malecki, C.K.; Pyun, Y.; Aycock, C.; Coyle, S. A meta-analytic review of the association between perceived social support and depression in childhood and adolescence. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 142, 1017–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Krause, K.; Hartley, R.; James, R.; McInnis, C. The First Year Experience in Australian Universities: Findings from a Decade of National Studies; AGPS: Canberra, NSW, Australia, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  36. Brodie, R.J.; Ilic, A.; Juric, B.; Hollebeek, L. Consumer engagement in a virtual brand community: An exploratory analysis. J. Bus. Res. 2013, 66, 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Upcraft, M.L.; Gardner, J.N.; Barefoot, B.O. Challenging and Supporting the First-Year Students: A Handbook for Improving the First Year of College; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  38. Ferrer, J.; Ringer, A.; Saville, K.; Parris, M.A.; Kashi, K. Students’ motivation and engagement in higher education: The importance of attitude to online learning. High. Educ. 2020, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Lizarte, E.J. Caracterización de las variables que influyen en el abandono de estudiantes de primer año de universi-dad en el ámbito de las ciencias sociales. El caso de la Facultad de Ciencias de la Educación de la Universidad de Granada. In Investigar e Innovar Para Cambiar: Una Apuesta Necesaria en la Docencia Universitaria; Fernandez, S.S., Ruiz, G.R., Estrada, A.C.M., Eds.; Comares: Granada, Spain, 2019; pp. 153–171. [Google Scholar]
  40. Rodríguez, M.A.; Cespón, M.; Ginés de la Nuez, C.; Cespón, M. Perfil sociodemográfico de los estudiantes univer-sitarios canarios. Rev. Educ. Super. 2020, 49, 81–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rodríguez Rodríguez, M. La Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria: Análisis Territorial y Perfil Sociodemográfico de su Estudiantado. Ph.D. Thesis, Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Canarias, España, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  42. Domínguez-Sánchez, F.J.; Lasa-Aristu, A.; Amor, P.J.; Holgado-Tello, F.P. Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Assessment 2011, 20, 253–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Garnefski, N.; Kraaij, V. The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2007, 23, 141–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Garnefski, N.; Kraaij, V.; Spinhoven, P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2001, 30, 1311–1327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Cabello, R.; Fernández-Berrocal, P.; Salguero, J.; Gross, J. A Spanish adaptation of the emotion regulation questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2013, 29, 234–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Hambleton, R.K. Issues, designs, and technical guidelines for adapting tests into multiple languages and cultures. In Adapting Educational and Psychological Tests for Cross-Cultural Assessment; Hambleton, R.K., Merenda, P., Spielberger, C., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, United States, 2006; pp. 3–38. [Google Scholar]
  47. Huicho, D. Influencia de la Regulación Emocional Sobre las Estrategias de Afrontamiento al Estrés ante la Situación de Pandemia en Universitarios de Lima. Ph.D. Thesis, Universidad San Ignacio de Loyola, Lima, Peru, 2020. Available online: http://repositorio.usil.edu.pe/handle/USIL/11237 (accessed on 17 February 2021).
  48. Wang, C.; Zhao, H. The impact of COVID-19 on anxiety in Chinese university students. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Dominguez-Lara, S.; Medrano, L. Invarianza factorial del cognitive emotional regulation questionnarie (CERQ) en universitarios limeños y cordobeses. Univ. Psychol. 2016, 15, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Muñoz, S.I.; Molina, D.; Ochoa, R.; Sánchez, O.; Esquivel, J.A. Estrés, respuestas emocionales, factores de riesgo, psi-copatología y manejo del personal de salud durante la pandemia por COVID-19. Acta Pediatr. Méx. 2020, 41 (Suppl. S1), S127–S136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Cabanach, R.G.; Fernández-Cervantes, R.; Souto-Gestal, A.; Suárez-Quintanilla, J.A. Regulación emocional y estrés en estudiantes universitarios de fisioterapia. Rev. Mex. Psicol. 2018, 35, 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Caruso, D.R.; Salovey, P.; Brackett, M.; Mayer, J.D. The ability model of emotional intelligence. In Positive Psychology in Practice: Promoting Human Flourishing in Work, Health, Education, and Everyday Life; Joseph, S., Ed.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015; pp. 545–558. [Google Scholar]
  53. Gaeta, M.L.; Gaeta, L.; Rodriguez, M.S. The impact of COVID-19 home confinement on mexican university students: Emotions, coping strategies, and self-regulated learning. Front. Psychol. 2021, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Díaz, F. Nivel de competencias de estudiantes universitarios en la ciudad de Ceuta. In Formación por Competencias y Competencias Para la Formación: Perspectivas Desde la Investigación; Gijón, J., Ed.; Síntesis: Madrid, Spain, 2016; pp. 171–182. [Google Scholar]
  55. Lizarte, E.J.; Gijón, J. Ambientes de aprendizaje para las nuevas -y viejas- metodologías en la Educación Superior. In Investigación, Innovación Docente y TIC. Nuevos Horizontes Educativos; Garcia, S.A., Rodriguez, J.M.R., Rodriguez-Jimenez, C., Reche, J.M.S., Eds.; Dykinson: Madrid, Spain, 2019; pp. 689–701. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wade, M.; Prime, H.; Browne, D.T. Why we need longitudinal mental health research with children and youth during (and after) the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 290, 113143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Lacomba, L.; Valero, S.; De la Barrera, U.; Pérez, M.; Montoya, I. Ajuste psicológico en cuidadores familiares de adolescentes: Un análisis longitudinal durante el confinamiento por la COVID-19 en España (Psychological adjustment in family caregivers of adolescents: A longitudinal analysis during confinement by COVID-19 in Spain). Anál. Modif. Conducta 2021, 47, 35–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Satici, B.; Gocet-Tekin, E.; Deniz, M.E.; Satici, S.A. Adaptation of the fear of COVID-19 scale: Its association with psychological distress and life satisfaction in turkey. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 2020, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. King, L.A.; Hicks, J.A.; Krull, J.L.; Del Gaiso, A.K. Positive affect and the experience of meaning in life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 90, 179–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Pérez-López, E.; Atochero, A.V.; Rivero, S.C. Educación a distancia en tiempos de COVID-19: Análisis desde la perspectiva de los estudiantes universitarios. RIED Rev. Iberoam. Educ. Distancia. 2021, 24, 331–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Del Arco, I.; Silva, P.; Flores, O. University teaching in times of confinement: The light and shadows of compulsory online learning. Sustainability 2021, 13, 375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Fernández, A.; Paricio, J.; Ibarra-Sáiz, M.S.; Rodríguez-Gómez, G. No es cuestión de medios, sino de modelo. Escenarios de reducción de la presencialidad. In REDU 2020 Red Estatal de Docencia Universitaria; REDU: Zaragoza, Spain, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  63. Mejías, R. Juventud Isleña y Posible Sensación de Aislamiento; APA Style Blog; 11 February 2020. Available online: https://hannikells.wordpress.com/2020/02/11/juventud-islena-yposible-sensacion-de-aislamiento/ (accessed on 17 January 2021).
Table 1. Beliefs about the COVID-19 pandemic.
Table 1. Beliefs about the COVID-19 pandemic.
BeliefCanary IslandsGeneral
N X DSN X DS
Young people are agents spreading the disease to older people 5604.140.96612563.871.042
Young people are immune to the virus5601.430.70912561.460.681
Older people can infect me with the virus5604.290.87812564.060.938
Governments have known how to take proper measures5602.811.13412562.711.108
The national health system is prepared for a pandemic5601.960.93512561.970.934
Table 2. Beliefs concerning the effectiveness of virtual teaching.
Table 2. Beliefs concerning the effectiveness of virtual teaching.
BeliefCanary IslandsGeneral
N X DSN X DS
My university has adequate services and resources for virtual teaching5602.811.22912563.011.118
The professors are properly trained for engaging in online education5602.191.12312562.581.103
The professors have the necessary resources for engaging in online education5603.031.18212563.021.098
I am digitally capable of engaging in online learning5604.060.97312563.641.081
I have improved the planning of my study time during the lockdown5602.871.34212562.961.243
Table 3. Social conditions of the lockdown and a description of the cohabitation situations.
Table 3. Social conditions of the lockdown and a description of the cohabitation situations.
Canary IslandsGeneral
Yes %No %NYes %No %N
Family residence84.014.157283.014.31292
Home of another family member8.691.35826.992.81324
Temporary student residence31.267.657621.677.81320
Cohabitation with children19.280.157924.075.41319
Cohabitation with people older than 7012.387.358114.385.41323
Cohabitation with a pet that needs to be walked32.965.557434.564.11309
Table 4. Description of the lockdown dwellings.
Table 4. Description of the lockdown dwellings.
Canary IslandsGeneral
Yes %No %NYes %No %N
Dwelling in an urban area72.225.957274.922.91298
Dwelling in a rural area24.574.457722.276.91314
House with a large yard14.185.958117.681.81318
House with a small yard8.691.458115.884.11326
Dwelling with a large terrace29.769.557827.472.01319
Dwelling with a small terrace24.574.657835.663.11309
Dwelling with a large interior patio10.389.258018.581.01321
Dwelling with a small interior patio19.280.358026.473.51325
Dwelling with no exterior space30.269.157920.179.41320
Dwelling with outside views 88.39.156883.713.61291
Table 5. Perception of the suitability of the lockdown dwellings.
Table 5. Perception of the suitability of the lockdown dwellings.
Canary IslandsGeneral
Yes %No %NYes %No %N
Large home57.141.057262.536.01307
Comfortable home87.710.357189.08.51294
Small dwelling34.165.057828.870.11312
Oppressive dwelling15.483.557711.488.21321
WiFi available in the home94.03.356792.05.21290
Dwelling well equipped for studying77.220.957277.820.01298
Dwelling well equipped for physical exercise49.449.757844.953.91311
Dwelling well equipped for entertainment63.634.056964.633.41300
Table 6. Group statistics.
Table 6. Group statistics.
StrategiesCanary IslandsGeneral
N X DSN X DS
Self-blame5831.310.4113271.440.52
Acceptance5833.380.8813273.400.81
Rumination5832.901.0613272.961.02
Positive refocusing5833.001.1213273.111.07
Refocusing on planning5833.050.9213273.140.86
Positive reappraisal5833.291.1013273.391.04
Putting into perspective5833.520.8913273.390.88
Catastrophizing5831.860.7813272.050.81
Blaming others5832.511.2813272.571.16
Table 7. The order of the strategies most commonly used by the Canary Island students.
Table 7. The order of the strategies most commonly used by the Canary Island students.
Strategy X
Putting into perspective3.52
Acceptance3.38
Positive Reappraisal3.29
Refocusing on planning3.05
Positive refocusing3
Rumination2.9
Blaming others2.51
Catastrophizing1.86
Self-blame1.31
Table 8. Order of the strategies most commonly used by students in the general sample.
Table 8. Order of the strategies most commonly used by students in the general sample.
X
Acceptance3.4
Positive reappraisal3.39
Putting into perspective3.39
Refocusing on planning3.14
Positive refocusing3.11
Rumination2.96
Blaming others2.57
Catastrophizing2.05
Self-blame1.44
Table 9. t test for the equality of means.
Table 9. t test for the equality of means.
Mean DifferencetSignificance
Self-blame−0.12902−5.3140.000 *
Acceptance−0.01660−0.4020.688
Rumination−0.06029−1.1750.240
Positive Refocusing−0.11722−2.1750.030 *
Refocusing on planning−0.08339−1.9080.057
Positive reappraisal−0.09849−1.8770.061
Putting into perspective0.130762.9730.003 *
Catastrophizing−0.18759−4.7210.000 *
Blaming others−0.06836−1.1510.250
Note: *: The difference is significant at a level of 0.05.
Table 10. Correlations between lockdown conditions and cognitive strategies.
Table 10. Correlations between lockdown conditions and cognitive strategies.
StrategiesCanary IslandsGeneral
ABCDABCD
Self-blame−0.004−0.019−0.050−0.009−0.0430.122 **0.0370.030
Acceptance−0.003−0.0130.0470.0270.036−0.031−0.036−0.011
Rumination−0.0520.094 *0.0290.124 **−0.0010.025−0.0360.048
Positive refocusing0.015−0.083 *−0.159 **−0.170 **0.036−0.060 *−0.085 **−0.147 **
Refocusing on planning−0.023−0.041−0.124 **−0.113 **0.045−0.054−0.096 **−0.117 **
Positive reappraisal0.011−0.084 *−0.193 **−0.134 **0.008−0.036−0.119 **−0.156 **
Putting into perspective0.018−0.096 *−0.149 **−0.170 **0.033−0.044−0.061 *−0.116 **
Catastrophizing0.0060.093 *0.115 **0.217 **−0.0300.138 **0.0300.126 **
Blaming others−0.0070.0620.0470.087 *−0.069 *0.076 **−0.0150.038
Note: A: Living with a pet that needs to be walked; B: Availability of a WiFi network in the home; C: Dwelling with good equipment for physical exercise; D: Dwelling with good equipment for entertainment; **. The correlation is significant at the 0.01 level; *. The correlation is significant at the 0.05 level.
Table 11. Comparison of the independence of groups who spent the lockdown with a pet that needed to be walked and those who did not in the Canary Island sample.
Table 11. Comparison of the independence of groups who spent the lockdown with a pet that needed to be walked and those who did not in the Canary Island sample.
M1–M2tglSig.
Self-blame0.003610.003615720.921
Acceptance0.005830.005835720.941
Rumination0.115970.115975720.215
Positive refocusing−0.03651−0.036515720.713
Refocusing on planning0.045350.045355720.578
Positive reappraisal−0.02632−0.026325720.787
Putting into perspective−0.03469−0.034695720.659
Catastrophizing−0.01036−0.010365720.88
Blaming others0.020190.020195720.858
Table 12. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown with a pet that needed to be walked and those who did not in the general sample.
Table 12. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown with a pet that needed to be walked and those who did not in the general sample.
M1–M2tglSig.
Self-blame0.046581.54313060.123
Acceptance−0.05987−1.28813060.198
Rumination0.001380.02313060.981
Positive refocusing−0.0805−1.29913060.194
Refocusing on planning−0.0804−1.61113060.107
Positive reappraisal−0.01845−0.30713060.759
Putting into perspective−0.06104−1.18713060.235
Catastrophizing0.046581.54313060.123
Blaming others−0.05987−1.28813060.198
Table 13. Comparison of the independence of the groups in home lockdown with a home WiFi network and those without such a network in the Canary Island sample.
Table 13. Comparison of the independence of the groups in home lockdown with a home WiFi network and those without such a network in the Canary Island sample.
M1–M2tglSig.
Self-blame0.096570.4595650.646
Acceptance0.208070.35650.764
Rumination0.24625−2.2545650.025 *
Positive refocusing0.262471.9765650.049 *
Refocusing on planning0.215490.9775650.329
Positive reappraisal0.256331.9945650.047 *
Putting into perspective0.207212.2895650.022 *
Catastrophizing0.18098−2.2135650.027 *
Blaming others0.29751−1.4775650.14
Note: *. The difference is significant at a level of 0.05.
Table 14. Comparison of the independence of the groups in lockdown with a home WiFi network and those without such a network in the general sample.
Table 14. Comparison of the independence of the groups in lockdown with a home WiFi network and those without such a network in the general sample.
M1–M2tglSig.
Self-blame−0.28036−4.40312870 **
Acceptance0.110281.11212870.266
Rumination−0.11501−0.91212870.362
Positive refocusing0.284352.14912870.032 *
Refocusing on planning0.2041.92312870.055 *
Positive reappraisal0.167711.30512870.192
Putting into perspective0.174751.59612870.111
Catastrophizing−0.49421−4.98712870 **
Blaming others−0.39066−2.73612870.006 **
Note: **. The difference is significant at a level of 0.01; *. The difference is significant at a level of 0.05.
Table 15. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown with good physical exercise equipment and those who did not in the Canary Island sample.
Table 15. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown with good physical exercise equipment and those who did not in the Canary Island sample.
M1–M2tglSig.
Self-blame0.041051.1975760.232
Acceptance−0.08299−1.1255760.261
Rumination−0.0604−0.6855760.493
Positive refocusing0.355533.8555760 **
Refocusing on planning0.228763.0055760.003 **
Positive reappraisal0.424174.7245760 **
Putting into perspective0.264833.6285760 **
Catastrophizing−0.17829−2.7755760.006 **
Blaming others−0.17829−2.776571,8910.006 **
Note: **. The difference is significant at a level of 0.01; *. The difference is significant at a level of 0.05.
Table 16. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown in a home with good physical exercise equipment and those who did not in the general sample.
Table 16. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown in a home with good physical exercise equipment and those who did not in the general sample.
M1–M2tglSig.
Self-blame−0.03872−1.3413080.181
Acceptance0.057611.29513080.196
Rumination0.07431.31613080.188
Positive refocusing0.183333.10113080.002 **
Refocusing on planning0.166283.50513080 **
Positive reappraisal0.247794.34313080 **
Putting into perspective0.108272.2113080.027 *
Catastrophizing−0.0492−1.09413080.274
Blaming others0.034170.53213080.595
Note: **. The difference is significant at a level of 0.01; *. The difference is significant at a level of 0.05.
Table 17. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown in homes with good entertainment equipment and those who did not in the Canary Island sample.
Table 17. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown in homes with good entertainment equipment and those who did not in the Canary Island sample.
M1–M2tglSig.
Self-blame0.008210.2255670.822
Acceptance−0.04983−0.6365670.525
Rumination−0.27463−2.975670.003 **
Positive refocusing0.403384.1195670 **
Refocusing on planning0.219872.7195670.007 **
Positive reappraisal0.309453.2175670.001 **
Putting into perspective0.318364.1115670 **
Catastrophizing−0.35472−5.2865670 **
Blaming others−0.23246−2.0725670.039 *
Note: **. The difference is significant at a level of 0.01; *. The difference is significant at a level of 0.05.
Table 18. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown in a home with good entertainment equipment and those who did not in the general sample.
Table 18. Comparison of the independence of the groups who spent the lockdown in a home with good entertainment equipment and those who did not in the general sample.
M1–M2tglSig.
Self-blame−0.0331−1.09112970.275
Acceptance0.01870.39712970.692
Rumination−0.10323−1.7312970.084
Positive refocusing0.332525.3612970 **
Refocusing on planning0.212394.24812970 **
Positive reappraisal0.342355.69112970 **
Putting into perspective0.217554.2212970 **
Catastrophizing−0.21539−4.57812970 **
Blaming others−0.09158−1.35212970.177
Note: **. The difference is significant at a level of 0.01.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cuevas López, M.; Ávalos Ruiz, I.; Lizarte Simón, E.J. Emotional Cognitive Regulation in University Students during Lockdown: A Comparative Analysis of Students from Spanish Universities. Sustainability 2021, 13, 6946. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13126946

AMA Style

Cuevas López M, Ávalos Ruiz I, Lizarte Simón EJ. Emotional Cognitive Regulation in University Students during Lockdown: A Comparative Analysis of Students from Spanish Universities. Sustainability. 2021; 13(12):6946. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13126946

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cuevas López, Mercedes, Inmaculada Ávalos Ruiz, and Emilio Jesús Lizarte Simón. 2021. "Emotional Cognitive Regulation in University Students during Lockdown: A Comparative Analysis of Students from Spanish Universities" Sustainability 13, no. 12: 6946. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13126946

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop