Next Article in Journal
Traditional Foods at the Click of a Button: The Preference for the Online Purchase of Romanian Traditional Foods during the COVID-19 Pandemic
Next Article in Special Issue
A Firm’s Financial Reputation vs. Sustainability Reputation: Do Consumers Really Care?
Previous Article in Journal
A multi-Criteria Wetland Suitability Index for Restoration across Ontario’s Mixedwood Plains
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Impact of Self-Quantification on Consumers’ Participation in Green Consumption Activities and Behavioral Decision-Making
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Environmental Impact of Consumption Lifestyles: Ethically Minded Consumption vs. Tightwads

by
Laurie Touchette
* and
Marcelo Vinhal Nepomuceno
*
HEC Montréal, 3000 Chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, QC H3T 2A7, Canada
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2020, 12(23), 9954; https://doi.org/10.3390/su12239954
Submission received: 20 October 2020 / Revised: 20 November 2020 / Accepted: 23 November 2020 / Published: 28 November 2020

Abstract

:
This study investigates the environmental impact of anti-consumption lifestyles and compares it to environmental concern and ethically minded consumption. Environmental impact was measured in a sample of 357 individuals with a carbon footprint calculator capturing all greenhouse gases released by an individual’s activities. Three types of anti-consumption lifestyles were considered: frugality, voluntary simplicity, and tightwadism. Results suggest that tightwadism is negatively associated with environmental impact. This negative association is stronger when participants are knowledgeable about the emissions impact of their behaviors. These findings suggest that tightwadism can lead to positive outcomes to achieve sustainability. Surprisingly, frugality and voluntary simplicity, as well as environmental concern, are not significantly associated with environmental impact, whereas ethically minded consumption correlates positively with the latter. This study demonstrates that increasing consumers’ environmental and ethical concerns alone might not be an effective way to lead them towards a more sustainable lifestyle. Such findings have important implications for sustainability and public policy makers.

1. Introduction

The environmental consequences of human activity have earned the interest of academic research since the 1960s [1]. To find solutions to environmental issues, the key role of consumption in driving environmental impact has long been recognised. To this end, authors have examined the psychological processes and motivations behind individuals’ environmentally friendly behaviors [2,3,4]. However, given today’s significant environmental challenges such as climate change [5], policies that seek to promote pro-environmental behaviors (e.g., to encourage incremental consumption shifts from regular products to “greener” ones) may not be a sufficient solution in favour of sustainability [1]. Some authors question the environmental reform strategy largely based on promoting green and ethical consumption behaviors, as it will be offset by substantial growth in global consumption [6,7]. They suggest that more effective alternatives to environmental degradation in industrialized nations lie in changing the dominant lifestyles and consumption patterns, which are considered unsustainable. More specifically, these scholars discuss the need to limit consumption for sustainable development purposes [7,8,9,10,11]. In particular, research has focused on understanding how to reduce meat consumption [12,13,14], energy consumption [15], and car usage and purchase [16,17,18], as these have strong impact on the environment.
This paper builds on previous literature and stems from the intersection between the fields of consumer behavior and sustainability. Past research suggests that the adoption of anti-consumption lifestyles might contribute to achieving sustainability goals [1,2,19,20,21]. Anti-consumption urges consumers to buy less products [2], resulting in a voluntary reduction in the acquisition, use, and disposal of commoditized goods and services [22]. Following this definition, individuals who adopt anti-consumption lifestyles are motivated to minimize their consumption levels [23], which should lower their negative impact on the environment [20]. This occurs because when consumers systematically resist consumption as an integral part of their lifestyle, fewer resources are requested, and less impact is generated on the environment [24]. However, while the emerging topic of anti-consumption is gaining more attention in the field of consumer behavior, its contribution to sustainability remains largely underexplored. In particular, it is still unclear to what degree anti-consumption lifestyles should reduce one’s environmental impact.
To the best of our knowledge, only one study to date has investigated the extent to which the adoption of anti-consumption lifestyles might be associated with lower environmental impact [20]. Its authors found that tightwadism and voluntary simplicity are associated with lower environmental impact than environmental concern and frugality. The present study adds to this existing research and assesses the validity and reproducibility of the findings. It does so by using a more comprehensive measure of environmental impact (i.e., a carbon footprint calculator—Carbon Footprint Ltd., Hampshire, UK 2017). This calculator uses emissions factors considering all greenhouse gases (i.e., CO2, N2O, methane, etc.). Differently from past research, this method considered individual carbon emissions caused by not only food-related consumption, but also energy, transportation, clothing, and numerous other types of products and services (see Table 1 and Table 2) allowing for a better representation of all activities undertaken over a year in a highly detailed manner [25].
In addition to using a more comprehensive measure of environmental impact, the current research compares frugality, tightwadism, and voluntary simplicity not only to environmental concern, but also to ethically minded consumption. Within the literature, there remains a debate as to the commitment towards ethical purchasing [26]. Since many studies have found a gap between the ethical attitudes consumers say they hold, and their actual purchase behavior [26,27,28,29,30,31], it was thus important to include ethically minded consumption in this study, as it has yet to prove its positive environmental effects.
Finally, this research examines the moderating role of emissions impact knowledge, as we hope to find that it might further reduce one’s environmental impact. Past studies have shown that consumers with environmental awareness are more willing to engage in environmentally friendly behaviors [32,33,34]. We believe that knowledge of high and low emissions actions can further reduce anti-consumers’ impact on the environment, since they make behavioral choices as part of their lifestyle which have great potential to lower negative emissions.
The anti-consumption lifestyles considered in this study—voluntary simplicity, frugality, and tightwadism—lead to less consumption in general, even though they each operate under different psychological mechanisms [20]. Frugality refers to a resourceful use of material goods and services given the pleasure of saving [35,36]. Therefore, a frugal consumer resists the consumption of all products and services because (s)he feels pleasure in saving resources. Voluntary simplicity is adopted by individuals who reduce spending on goods and services to live a simple life and obtain satisfaction by cultivating non-materialistic values [37,38]. A simplifier resists consumption by acquiring only the minimum and living very modestly. Tightwadism is characterized by a consumption reduction caused by the pain of spending [36]. Thus, a tightwad minimizes the consumption of products and services because spending money represents a psychologically difficult process. The degree to which individuals identify with these anti-consumption lifestyles might lower their environmental impact [20], since the consumption reduction practices incorporated in their daily lives form an essential part of sustainable living [2].
The contradictory results from studies regarding pro-environmental and ethical concerns cast doubt on their real contribution to environmental protection. Many researchers observe inconsistencies between what consumers say about the importance of ethical consumerism and their actual purchasing behavior, which have been termed the “attitude–behavior gap” [39,40,41,42,43]. Similarly, environmental concern might not be reflected by environmentally friendly consumer behaviors. Although individuals portray themselves as being benevolent in surveys, they continue to give up on their green buying intentions by expressing their preference for unsustainable traditional products, usually because of economic motives [27]. Therefore, in practice, environmentally friendly consumer behavior is significantly lower than expected for environmental concern [28]. Hence, given that environmental and ethical concerns appear to have limited effect on one’s environmental impact reduction, this research investigates whether anti-consumption could be an alternative driver, without the environment being a prime motivator for consumption reduction, as it is the case for environmental and ethical concerns.
Furthermore, this study takes a fresh look at the moderating effect of emissions impact knowledge on the environmental impact of anti-consumption lifestyles. Researchers have found that consumers with more environmental knowledge tend to view individual efforts as important contributors to solving environmental problems [44]. To that end, this study proposes to conceptualize consumers’ environmental knowledge by introducing a new measure seeking to attest the knowledge about individual actions’ effectiveness in reducing carbon footprint, based on a recent study compiled a list of actions offering the best potential for reducing individual greenhouse gas emissions [45]. Since the most efficient actions listed involve a restriction of consumption and are similar to those adopted by anti-consumers, we propose that an increased emissions impact knowledge coupled with anti-consumption is even less harmful to the environment.
This research has important theoretical implications. First, by examining the environmental impact of anti-consumption lifestyles through a unique carbon footprint measure, it brings empirical evidence to theories that consider anti-consumption (i.e., the reduction of overall consumption) as a step towards sustainable development [1,19]. It also draws on previous literature claiming that the adoption of certain anti-consumption lifestyles helps reduce one’s overall consumption level [2,20]. More specifically, this study reveals a negative relationship between tightwadism and environmental impact, which suggests that the pain of spending and a strong attachment to money seem to push some consumers to significantly reduce their overall consumption, leading to a decreased carbon footprint. Second, this study shows that a negative association between environmental impact and tightwadism is even more negative when emissions impact knowledge is higher. In other words, individuals who tend to adopt tightwadism as part of their lifestyle are not only largely predisposed to saving resources but are also engaging in even more positive environmental behaviors when they are knowledgeable about their impact. Third, we found a positive relationship between ethically minded consumption and environmental impact. According to this result, the more an individual claim to consume based on ethical criteria, the higher the environmental impact. This supports the idea that attempts at ethical consumption present inherent contradictions that prevent ethical concerns from dominating the purchasing decision.
This research also presents significant implications for policy makers. Despite growing ecological concerns, public policies have so far failed to influence consumer behavior in favour of the environment [46,47], and current consumption trends are constantly moving away from global sustainability goals [7]. In the light of urgent and unresolved environmental challenges, this study intends to actively participate in the public debate on environmental concerns by proposing solutions to the problematic relationship between our consumerist society and the environment.
In the following section, we review the literature on anti-consumption, environmental concern, ethically minded consumption, and emissions impact knowledge. Subsequently, we present the method and test whether anti-consumption lifestyles are associated with a reduced environmental impact. In addition, we test if this association is stronger for respondents who are knowledgeable of the emissions impact of behaviors. We then compare these results with the environmental impact of environmental concern and ethically minded consumption. We conclude the paper with a discussion on future research, implications and limitations.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Anti-Consumption

Anti-consumption research focuses on phenomena that are against the acquisition, use, and dispossession of goods and services [48]. The construct, which “literally means against consumption” [23] (p. 145), refers to “a resistance to, distaste of, or even resentment of consumption” [48] (p. 121). It centres around the idea of limiting consumption; for example, restricting electricity use, but also avoiding the consumption of some goods and reclaiming a more authentic self-identity by repurposing objects and avoiding new consumption. Cherrier et al. [49] suggest that anti-consumption involves specific acts against consumption that relate to a person’s self-identity project. Anti-consumers may be motivated by underlying social–psychological stresses of a consumerist-driven life. It has been argued that anti-consumption results from a process of self-inquiry triggered by the failure to feel authentic through traditional consumption activities [48].
Consumer resistance is closely and oftentimes associated with anti-consumption in the literature. Where anti-consumption constitutes a form of private but persistent refusal to go along with consuming products or services [50], consumer resistance rather refers to the voluntary opposition to marketing activities or corporations, which can lead consumers to engage in a variety of anti-consumption actions [23] and revenge behaviors against corporations [51,52]. For instance, voluntary simplifiers perform many acts of anti-consumption that are primarily motivated by a desire to reclaim an authentic sense of self, rather than to oppose a dominant force such as corporations [22].
Anti-consumption lifestyles, although difficult to endorse due to the pressures of consumerism and existing social, cultural, and identity barriers [53], are voluntarily adopted by a growing number of individuals [48] who want to avoid consumption, by either buying less and/or efficiently using resources [23]. These lifestyles reflect a general reduction in consumer activities [54]. Following this definition, we consider voluntary simplicity, tightwadism, and frugality as three distinct anti-consumption lifestyles driven by a similar feeling of opposition toward consumption and operating under personal motivations that are specific to each one of them [20]. Voluntary simplicity is the degree to which one selects a simplified lifestyle intended to maximize direct control over daily activities, while minimizing consumption and dependency [37,38,55]. It is centered on the idea that personal satisfaction, fulfilment, and happiness result from a commitment to the nonmaterial aspects of life [48]. Therefore, voluntary simplifiers tend to refuse to purchase items that fail to improve their level of happiness, as well as reject all consumption activities that do not correspond to their desired self-concept [56,57].
Tightwadism refers to consumers who rely on an immediate “pain of paying” to control their spending [58]. In other words, tightwads’ affective reaction to spending may lead them to spend less than their more deliberative selves would prefer [36]. For them, spending money or paying for a good or service—especially consumables such as personal care, coffee, beverages, or entertainment outside the home—causes them pain [36,58]. This pain of spending money plays an important role in consumer self-regulation; hence, people who mentally impute the total cost of consumption are likely to be tightwads [58]. Therefore, consumers who experience a pain of paying may behave by accumulating wealth and avoiding unnecessary spending. Tightwadism shapes shopping behaviors and acts as an effective reminder of the sacrifice a small purchase can entail [58].
Frugality is defined as “a consumer lifestyle trait characterized by the degree to which consumers are both restrained in acquiring and in resourcefully using economic goods and services to achieve longer-term goals” [35] (p. 88). Unlike tightwadism, frugal consumers reduce consumption because they enjoy saving, not because the prospect of spending is painful [36]. In a similar fashion to Kropfeld et al. [20], we opted for these lifestyles because they met the following criteria: they have been the focus of multiple studies, they are proven to be conceptually different, and they result in similar behaviors (i.e., decrease of consumption), but for different personal reasons that do not always appear to be rooted in the quest for sustainability.
Nested within the field of consumer behavior, the study of anti-consumption aims to understand the motivations and behaviors of consumers who are against consumption and reject the consumption process in its different stages (acquisition, use, and disposal of certain goods) [22]. Recently, research has investigated the adoption of anti-consumption lifestyles as a means of achieving greater environmental sustainability [1,2,19,20]. With its focus on consumption reduction behaviors inherent to anti-consumption lifestyles, this research bridges the consumer behavior and sustainability fields, exploring how reduction of consumption might play a role in lowering one’s ecological footprint.
Although most research in anti-consumption focuses on the reasons behind product/brand avoidance, a review of the literature indicates that practices of anti-consumption are, in fact, elements of sustainable lifestyles [2]. Black and Cherrier [2] found that living a sustainable life does not necessarily imply the consumption of green products but could rather be driven by strong self-interest and identity seeking [58]. The motivation to behave more sustainably may not stem from environmental concern in the first place [59]. Instead, anti-consumption practices of rejection, reuse, and recycle might be more sustainable because they provide consumers with opportunities to maintain consistency between their beliefs, values, and behaviors [2]. In particular, frugality [35], voluntary simplicity [19], and tightwadism [36] are primarily motivated by self-interest goals.
Consistent with the definition of anti-consumption, anti-consumers reduce their overall consumption levels (i.e., across product and service categories) as part of their lifestyle [23]. Because tightwadism, voluntary simplicity, and frugality motivate, each in their own specific way, to minimize consumption, consumers endorsing these lifestyles should possess fewer products than the average consumer. Furthermore, the strong orientation towards less consumption might lead to lower expenditures, which should, in turn, decrease the quantity of resources needed to meet consumption levels. Hence, for the above reasons, and in accordance with past research investigating frugality, voluntary simplicity, and tightwadism [20], environmental impact is expected to be negatively associated with anti-consumption lifestyles.

2.2. Environmental Concern

Marketing scholars often research sustainability by looking at consumers’ preference for environmentally friendly choices [49]. In seeking to understand pro-environmental behavior, previous research has highlighted the importance of environmental concern [60], which could be roughly equated with an awareness of negative consequences [61] and that such awareness would activate people’s norm to behave in a pro-environmental way [62]. According to Antil [63], consumers’ environmental attitudes are expressed through their concern for the environment and are an important motive for purchasing behaviors.
Environmentally concerned individuals may be very effective in reducing their environmental impact, as they are well informed about the impact of consumption on the environment [64]. Therefore, one might expect that environmental concern is negatively associated with one’s environmental impact. However, it appears that environmentally concerned individuals often find themselves on the horns of a dilemma about what to do in order to reduce their environmental impact, as they base their decisions on sometimes contradictory information [65]. Other authors add that environmentally responsible behavior usually involves difficult motivational conflicts arising from the incompatibility of collective goals to protect the environment and individuals’ personal benefits [31,66]. Furthermore, there usually are external constraints to green consumerism perpetuated by the cultural, infrastructural, political, and economic circumstances in the markets and society [67,68]. Hence, environmentally friendly consumption may be characterized as a highly complex form of consumer behavior [65], which involves a gap between positive attitudes of consumers toward the environment and actual purchase behavior [39].
Consequently, anti-consumption lifestyles might be associated with an even lower environmental impact than environmental concern, since the broad range of anti-consumption practices allow for self-expression and provide with opportunities to show consistencies between beliefs, values, and behaviors [2]. This is contrasted with environmentally friendly consumption, where green consumers sometimes compromise parts of themselves in order to live more sustainably [69]. Therefore, we expect:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Environmental impact will be more negatively associated with tightwadism (a), voluntary simplicity (b), and frugality (c) than with environmental concern.

2.3. Ethically Minded Consumption

Responses to society’s ethical concerns also come from consumers who want to be informed about how the products they buy are made and who benefits from their purchases [70]. The concept of ethical consumerism has evolved considerably over the last decade [48], as it primarily focused on environmental issues (i.e., green consumerism) and came to broadly include other aspects such as animal welfare, labour standards, human rights, and health-related issues [71,72,73]. Cowe and Williams [74] regard ethical consumers as people who are influenced by environmental or ethical considerations when choosing products and services. Some of the ways by which consumers can accomplish this more concretely is by refusing to purchase certain products to achieve some socially responsible outcome [75] or by paying a premium for ethically made goods [76,77,78,79]. Studies have shown that many consumers have developed favorable attitudes towards ethical products and companies with socially responsible practices [80,81], and they believe that they have the power to change companies’ behaviors [82].
However, there seems to be a difference between what consumers say about the importance of ethical consumerism and their actual purchasing behavior, as the market share of ethical products and brands remains low [83,84]. Therefore, the attitude–behavior gap described previously is not unique to environmental concern. This consumer attitude–behavior gap with respect to ethical issues [82,85] has been noted by several researchers [27,28,29,30,31,70]. Ethical intentions stated in surveys are rarely acted upon in terms of actual ethical choices and purchases [42]. It has been found that ethical consumers sometimes leave ethics aside when a decision involves a trade-off between ethical and traditional purchasing criteria, such as price or availability [86]. Any attempt to consume ethically may reveal inherent tensions among one’s divergent concerns [87]. For example, one’s goal of reducing food miles may conflict with supporting developing countries, a societal kind of concern. Therefore, consuming ethically often posits consistency challenges to consumers, and it is likely that corporate social responsibility might not be the dominant criterion at the time of an ethical consumer’s purchase decision [39].
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Environmental impact will be more negatively correlated with tightwadism (a), voluntary simplicity (b) and frugality (c) than with ethically minded consumption.

2.4. Emissions Impact Knowledge as a Moderator

Studies have reported that consumers with high environmental awareness are more likely to adopt environmentally friendly behaviors [32,33,34], since they are more aware of their potential damage to the environment [88,89]. Some researchers have also found that individuals with more environmental knowledge tend to view individual efforts as important factors that can contribute to solving environmental problems [44].
When evaluating different behavioral alternatives to adopt to reduce environmental footprint, a person’s knowledge as to their relative conservation effectiveness becomes essential [90]. One may try, for example, to save more energy, whether by purchasing a fuel-efficient automobile or by limiting driving. In that respect, Wynes and Nicholas [45] have compiled various individual actions’ potential in reducing negative emissions on the environment. The most effective emission-reduction actions listed, which help save more than 0.8 tCO2e per year, can contribute to systemic change and substantially reduce annual personal emissions. These actions (i.e., having one fewer child, living car-free, avoiding airplane travel, and eating a plant-based diet) imply a significant reduction in consumption and lifestyle orientation toward restriction. They contrast with less effective actions in terms of emissions reduction such as recycling or upgrading lightbulbs (only 0.2 tCO2e saved per year).
Consumption reduction behaviors are easier to incorporate into everyday habits at no upfront costs [91], as opposed to efficiency improvements that generally involve more research, effort, and higher expenses (e.g., getting a new energy-efficient refrigerator). Since anti-consumption lifestyles involve prioritizing consumption reduction behaviors (i.e., high impact actions such as avoiding driving) over efficiency improvement actions (i.e., moderate to low impact actions such as upgrading lightbulbs), we argue that emissions impact knowledge will moderate the influence of anti-consumption on environmental impact. That is, when emissions impact knowledge is high, anti-consumption lifestyles should be associated with reduced environmental impact.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Environmental impact will be more negatively associated with tightwadism (a), voluntary simplicity (b), and frugality (c) when emissions impact knowledge is high.

3. Method

3.1. Participants and Procedure

A sample of 357 participants completed the online survey. These participants were recruited via Qualtrics panel, and they were all located in California. A single state was selected to standardize costs of electricity, transportation, and other categories of expenses involved in the carbon footprint calculator, since respondents were asked to indicate the average amount spent ($USD) by their household on different categories of products and services in the last twelve months. Items measuring participants’ scores on the anti-consumption lifestyles, as well as environmental and ethical concerns, were presented to participants before the spending questions to minimize social desirability. Questions within these two sections appeared in a random order to minimize order response bias. Randomization was insured by randomly distributing the questionnaire across members of the panel.
Seven participants were excluded because they failed the attention manipulation check [92] or provided unlikely spending habits (e.g., participants who indicated driving over 100,000 miles per year). Eighty percent of the participants were female. Respondents’ mean age was 46 years old (range: 18–91) with 41% of them with a university degree. Their reported annual revenues (before taxes) show significant variance; while 26% earn less than US $30,000 per household per year, 24% earn more than $90,000. Finally, there is an average of three people per household among participants (standard deviation = 1.77), and more than half of them are married or common-law partners.

3.2. Measures

To measure the constructs related to the anti-consumption lifestyles as well as environmental and ethical concerns, we used multi-item self-reported scales that have been tested and reviewed in past research and proved to be valid and reliable. For tightwadism, we used the four-item scale by Rick et al. [36]; for frugality, the eight-item scale developed by Lastovicka et al. [35]; and for voluntary simplicity, the nine-item scale used by Iwata [37,93] as adapted by Nepomuceno and Laroche [94]. We measured environmental concern with the New Ecological Paradigm scale by Dunlap et al. [95], and we measured ethically minded consumption with the scale by Sudbury-Riley and Kohlbacher [96].
We tested the emissions impact knowledge by asking participants to rate twelve individual actions according to their perceived effectiveness in reducing personal emissions. Participants had to select, for each of them, on a Likert-type frequency scale ranging from “1” (the least effective action to reduce carbon footprint) to “7” (the most effective action to reduce carbon footprint), the figure that best matches their perception of reality. This scale estimated the knowledge they have about the extent to which each individual action can reduce one’s negative impact on the environment, as listed by Wynes and Nicholas [45]. In order to measure the participant’s total emissions impact knowledge, the numbers selected for each item were subtracted from their real score (refer to Table 1), which resulted in a score that ranged from 0 (right answer; no difference with real score) to a positive or negative score between 1 to 6 (wrong answer; action effectiveness is overrated or underrated). The twelve scores were combined in an index of emissions impact knowledge, where a low index represented an adequate knowledge and a high index represented little knowledge. This method was preferred to existing scales, because it measures participants’ real knowledge of emissions impact as opposed to a self-report of their self-perceived knowledge.
Environmental impact was measured with a carbon footprint calculator [97] that uses emissions factors considering all greenhouse gases (i.e., CO2, N2O, methane, etc.). Participants indicated the amount spent on numerous activities, products, and services (e.g., plane trips, energy consumption, food and clothes, etc.) in the last 12 months. Different steps were taken to help respondents estimate their expenses as accurately and easily as possible. For example, instead of having to report the number of kWh of electricity consumed by their household in the past year, which is difficult to know for the average consumer, respondents had to indicate the amount spent on electricity in $USD. They were invited to do so by consulting their electricity company’s website, bank account statements, or electricity bills. Using the price of electricity in California during the data collection period, we converted respondents’ expenses into kWh, which we entered in the calculator. The calculator used the amount in kWh to calculate the amount of tCO2 derived from electricity consumption.
To allow fair comparisons between respondents, the amount spent in certain categories was divided by the number of individuals living in the same household (refer to Table 2). For example, this was done with the amount spent on electricity and groceries (for a full list, please refer to Table 2). In order to estimate the total carbon footprint generated by the respondent, the carbon footprint calculator added, in tons of CO2 emitted for a full year, the score obtained in each of the consumption categories listed in Table 2. Nonetheless, we observed that the data distribution was skewed (skewness = 2.55), so we transformed the total footprint calculator by calculating its log. This new value was no longer skewed (skewness < 1) and was used in the analyses reported below. The same issue was observed for the remaining categories of expenditures listed in Table 2, and the same solution was adopted for each category.
The survey also included questions assessing the participants’ demographic and socioeconomic dimensions with respect to their age, income, highest level of education completed, gender, US state of residence, and marital status. These sociodemographic characteristics were collected to control the effects of these variables on environmental impact and to detect clues as to the use of potential moderators that can be tested by subsequent research.

3.3. Validity and Reliability

The Cronbach alphas are equal to 0.81 for voluntary simplicity, 0.92 for frugality, 0.69 for tightwadism, 0.92 for ethically minded consumption, and 0.83 for environmental concern, thereby confirming the respective reliability of each measure. Three of the five constructs present good convergent validity, with coefficients exceeding the recommended minimum of 0.5 (average variance extracted [AVE] for frugality = 0.700; AVE for tightwadism = 0.556; AVE for ethically minded consumption = 0.592). Voluntary simplicity and environmental concern share only 41.7% and 46.7% variance with their respective measures. The conditions of discriminant validity were met; the AVE for each construct is higher than the squared correlation between them [98]. These squared correlations ranged between 0 and 0.21, remaining lower than the average variance extracted. The items used in the scales along with relevant statistics are presented in Table 3.

4. Results

4.1. Environmental Impact of Lifestyles

Our first analysis aims at testing the association between anti-consumption lifestyles on environmental impact (see Table 4). In accordance with our prediction, we found that tightwadism correlates negatively with overall environmental impact (r = −0.12, p = 0.03), thereby confirming past research [20]. We found the strongest correlation for the other expenses category (r = −0.14, p = 0.01). A detailed analysis considering each product and service listed in the other expenses category showed that tightwadism is negatively associated with spending in food and beverages (r = −0.20; p < 0.001), clothing (r = −0.33; p < 0.001), paper-based products (r = −0.14; p = 0.04), furniture (r = −0.18; p = 0.02), pharmaceuticals (r = −0.32; p < 0.001), telecommunications (r = −0.15; p < 0.01), insurance (r = −0.13; p = 0.02), and marginally associated with spending in hotels (r = −0.10; p = 0.07). These findings indicate that a tightwad perspective tends to lower consumption of several different kinds of products and services, putting a downward pressure on overall environmental impact.
We found no significant correlation between voluntary simplicity and environmental impact (r = −0.07; p = 0.17; CI95%: −0.221 to −0.0164). However, we found that the other expenses category (r = −0.14, p = 0.01; CI95%: −0.24 to −0.037) and the flight category (r = −0.17; p = 0.03; CI95%: −0.269 to −0.067) were negatively associated with voluntary simplicity. A detailed analysis within other expenses category shows that voluntary simplicity is negatively associated with spending in computer equipment (r = −0.17; p = 0.03; CI95%: −0.269 to −0.067), furniture (r = −0.18; p = 0.02; CI95%: −0.279 to −0.078), hotels (r = −0.15; p = 0.04; CI95%: −0.25 to −0.047), and marginally associated with spending in food and beverages (r = −0.10; p = 0.08; CI95%: −0.202 to 0.004), clothing (r = −0.11; p = 0.08; CI95%: −0.211 to −0.006), and paper-based products (r = −0.11; p = 0.09; CI95%: −0.211 to −0.006).
In addition, we found no significant association between frugality and environmental impact (r = 0.01; p = 0.86; CI95%: −0.094 to 0.114). Frugality is marginally associated with car and motorcycle usage (r = 0.10; p = 0.08; CI95%: −0.202 to −0.004), but not significantly associated with any of the categories listed in Table 4, nor with any of the product and services listed in the other expenses category (p > 0.05). Overall, our findings with frugality and voluntary simplicity do not support expectations that environmental impact is lower for these anti-consumption lifestyles.
We found that environmental concern was not associated with environmental impact (r = 0.03; p = 0.58; CI95%: −0.076 to 0.135). Environmental concern is also not significantly associated with any of the categories listed in Table 1 (p > 0.05). We found only a marginally negative association between motor vehicles (r = −0.137; p = 0.06; CI95%: −0.272 to 0.007) and banking and financial sector (r = −0.136; p = 0.06; CI95%: −0.211 to 0.067) for the analyses with the products and services within other expenses category. Given that voluntary simplicity and frugality were also not associated with environmental impact, H1b and H1c were not supported. However, we did find support for H1a, because tightwadism is negatively associated with environmental impact, whereas environmental concern does not.
The relationship between ethically minded consumption and environmental impact is significant and positive (r = 0.11; p = 0.03; CI95%: 0.211 to 0.006). This finding supports H2a, H2b, and H2c, albeit in a surprising way, as we did not expect to find a positive association. In any case, this finding supports the idea that adopting ethical behaviors is actually damaging to the environment. The analyses with the categories of expenses showed that only other expenses category is marginally associated with ethical consumption (r = 0.10; p = 0.07; CI95%: −0.202 to 0.004), and that this association is driven by spending in food and beverages (r = 0.09; p = 0.08; CI95%: −0.014 to 0.192). The remaining categories are not associated with ethical consumption.

4.2. Moderating Role of Emissions Impact Knowledge

We conducted a simple moderation analysis using 10,000 bootstrap resamples (PROCESS model 1) [99]. It examined how environmental impact was influenced by anti-consumption lifestyles, environmental concern, and ethical consumption for different levels of emissions impact knowledge. In these analyses, we included income, age, education, sex, and marital status as control variables. Emissions impact knowledge was included as a moderator. In addition, environmental concern, ethically minded consumption, and the anti-consumption lifestyles (tightwadism, frugality, and voluntary simplicity) were included as independent variables. We found that tightwadism is negatively associated with environmental impact (b = −0.0124; CI95%: −0.0203 to −0.0046). Furthermore, the interaction between tightwadism and emissions impact knowledge was significantly associated with environmental impact (b = 0.0011; CI95%: 0.0001 to 0.0020). Specifically, tightwadism was negatively associated with environmental impact among highly (−1 SD; CI95%: −0.0291 to −0.0104) and average (CI95%: −0.0203 to −0.0046) emissions knowledgeable participants. Among participants with low emission impact knowledge (+1 SD), there was no significant association between tightwadism and environmental impact (b = −0.0051; CI95%: −0.0160 to 0.0058). Note that the environmental knowledge scale is inversed, thus a high score in environmental impact knowledge denotes low knowledge about one’s environmental impact. Overall, these results are in line with H3a, suggesting that tightwads have an even lower environmental impact when they are knowledgeable about their emissions.
The analyses conducted with frugality, voluntary simplicity, environmental concern, and ethical consumption yielded similar results. That is, none of these constructs were significantly associated with environmental impact (all p > 0.11). Furthermore, the interaction between these constructs and emission impact knowledge were not significantly associated with environmental impact (all p > 0.32). Therefore, we found no support for H3b and H3c, which is line with recent research [20].

5. Discussion

Building on previous literature defending anti-consumption as a path to more sustainable societies [1,2,20], this study investigates the environmental impact of tightwadism, frugality, and voluntary simplicity, as well as environmental concern and ethically minded consumption. The results reveal that environmental impact is negatively associated with tightwadism, unrelated to frugality, voluntary simplicity, and environmental concern, and positively associated with ethically minded consumption. We also looked into the moderating role of emissions impact knowledge between anti-consumption lifestyles and environmental impact and found that tightwadism is associated with lower environmental impact among highly emissions knowledgeable consumers. As predicted, exploring anti-consumption lifestyles in a sustainable development perspective highlights the importance of consumption reduction’s contribution to the deceleration of environmental degradation. The findings reported in this research demonstrate the downstream consequences of adopting a tightwadism lifestyle and its impact on environmental sustainability.

5.1. Environmental Impact of Anti-Consumption Lifestyles

According to the results of the current investigation, the negative relationship observed between tightwadism and environmental impact is in line with past research [20]. This finding provides additional evidence that limiting consumption in the manner of tightwadism succeeds in reducing harmful impacts on the environment. As hypothesized, the pain of spending experienced by tightwads may lead to a greater reduction in environmental impact than voluntary simplicity, frugality, environmental concern, or ethically-minded consumption. It appears the negative emotions that arise from the prospect of spending play an important role in consumers’ ability to self-regulate [58]. Tightwads’ aversion toward spending and strong desire to avoid it drives them to significantly reduce their overall consumption which, in turn, may explain why they would have a significantly lower environmental impact than other consumers.
Confirming a past study stating that frugality was unrelated to ecological impact [20], no significant relationship was found between frugality and environmental impact in this current investigation. Past literature suggests that individuals who adopt a frugal lifestyle view consumption as trivial and superficial [100] and undertake to reduce their consumption in the short term [35]. However, a decline in their consumption could be offset by an increase in their long-term expenditures, as some individuals refuse to submit to their short-term whims in order to achieve a goal of greater value, potentially of material nature [35]. Thus, even if frugal people adopt a way of life in which they seek to limit their consumption, they could commit themselves to higher expenditures in the long run, or even increase their spending on durable goods, as long as these purchases allow them to avoid waste and superficial costs [100].
In the case of voluntary simplicity, we observed no statistically significant relationship with environmental impact. Since consumption reduction is not a goal in itself for voluntary simplicity but rather a way to lead a fulfilling life, individuals who adopt this anti-consumption lifestyle might still spend on goods or services that fit their self-concept [56,57]. For instance, they might be spending money on experience (i.e., travel overseas), rather than material possessions.

5.2. Environmental Concern, Ethically Minded Consumption, and Emissions Impact Knowledge

This study also reveals that environmentally concerned consumers do not have a lower environmental impact. This reinforces the view that attitude–behavior inconsistencies exist, at least in part, because positive attitudes toward the environment will not necessarily lead to a reduction in environmental impact. A similar conclusion can be drawn from the statistically significant and positive relationship between ethically minded consumption and environmental impact. The more individuals claim to consume based on ethical criteria, the higher their environmental impact. These results suggest that displaying pro-environmental or ethical attitudes might not be enough to reduce the negative impact of consumption on the environment.
Although environmental concern and ethical consumption can be perceived as models of sustainable consumption to achieve, our results demonstrate that their ecological contribution is limited, especially when compared to that of tightwadism. It would seem, then, that even high environmental or ethical concern may be outweighed by other needs and desires, and that many conflicting factors may compete to shape their daily decisions and actions of consumers [101]. For example, the desire to travel to visit one’s family abroad could take precedence over the environmentally concerned individuals’ responsibility to reduce air travel to minimize their contribution to global warming. Therefore, we point to the need for other elements with the potential to bring people to act in an ecological way, without environmental or ethical concern being the central solution.
Another possible explanation for the findings with environmental concern and ethically minded consumption relates to the “negative footprint illusion” effect, as described by Kim and Schudt [102]. This bias in judgment is such that when consumers purchase an environmentally friendly product, they become insensitive to the quantity of usage or purchase of such product. In other words, though consumers have purchased a more environmentally friendly option, they end-up using it or purchasing it more, which ultimately increases their environmental impact.
The moderation analysis allowed us to conclude that environmental impact is more negatively associated with tightwadism when emissions impact knowledge is high. This result suggests that knowing the most effective emission-reduction actions is likely to encourage consumers who adopt a tightwad lifestyle to restrict their consumption to a greater degree. This finding is important, as it supports previous studies suggesting that understanding how to solve environmental problems increases the likelihood to take action to protect the environment [89,101,103].

5.3. Research Limitations

The results of this research should be evaluated considering a few methodological limitations. First, one should keep in mind this study’s limitations related to sample size, composition, and location. As in any study with similar methods, the results presented in this paper should be interpreted with care and may not be generalizable to other contexts. Future research should attempt to recruit larger samples and more diverse profiles of respondents. Please refer to Table 5 for a comparison of the demographics of the sampled population and the general population in the United States. Of note, the current sample is older, has considerably more women, and has lower household income. Despite these limitations, it is important to note that some of the results presented in this article are consistent with Kropfeld et al. [20], which was conducted using a different sample (Germans) and method (a more limited carbon footprint calculator). This provides further evidence that the adoption of an anti-consumption lifestyle (i.e., tightwadism) may lead to a significant reduction in one’s carbon footprint.
Second, participants’ answers may partly reflect their desire to project a positive image of themselves. The social desirability or self-presentation bias is inherent in all surveys that concern consumers’ willingness to adopt socially responsible consumption behaviors [43,109]. To alleviate this issue, we reiterated that the data collection was anonymous, and participants’ privacy was safeguarded.
Third, although the carbon footprint calculator used in this study [98] is reliable and efficient in terms of broad inclusion of carbon emission sources and depth measurement [25], the level of detail gathered from the participants has some limitations. For example, participants had to provide details about their travel itineraries by plane over the last 12 months, specifying the city of departure and destination, the stops made, and class type they travelled in (e.g., economy, business, etc.). This allowed us to quantify each plane trip individually. However, such a level of specificity was not possible for other categories, such as home energy. Expenses related to home energy were reported by energy type (e.g., electricity, natural gas, coal, etc.) for a typical month in the past year. Assumptions had to be made; the amount of total energy consumed by a household in a year is the sum of 12 identical consumption months, and the amount of energy consumed by an individual is equivalent to the total energy consumed by its household divided by the number of people that live together. Needless to say, this provided, at best, a rough measure of one’s carbon footprint.
Fourth, the carbon footprint calculator made no distinction between a green or ethical product and a regular one. The environmental footprint could differ greatly depending on the raw materials that make up the product, or its recyclability degree, for example. A product’s carbon footprint is calculated using the amount spent on that product, without considerations for ethical or green aspects. Therefore, an environmentally sensitive individual may have spent more on local products or products with fully recycled materials and, as a result, generate a higher environmental impact. This is especially true for the clothing and food categories. This limitation affects the accuracy of the calculation of the carbon footprint of environmentally and ethically conscious consumers, whose higher expenses incurred would be penalized by the calculator in question. Further studies should differentiate between more environmentally friendly products and regular products, which would allow researchers to test whether the consumption of environmentally friendly products leads to a significantly lower environmental impact for environmentally concerned consumers.
Fifth, there could be considerable differences between participants’ reported and observed behavior. Since the results depend on the use of retrospective information, respondents may not remember their past expenditures very accurately. Because they had to count their expenditures over a certain period, for numerous types of products and services (electricity, food, transportation, etc.), the amount spent could represent only an approximate reconstruction of their actual expenditures. In order to reduce participants’ memory bias, the expenditure period was adapted to the type of expenditure (e.g., home energy expenses per month, education expenses per year, dining out expenses per month, etc.). Furthermore, a self-report measure was used to estimate consumption patterns. Such measures are greatly influenced by memory. Future research would profit from measuring real buying behavior.

5.4. Research Implications

By examining the environmental impact of anti-consumption lifestyles, environmental concern and ethically minded consumption, this study provides additional empirical evidence that tightwadism is associated with lower carbon footprint, thus confirming existing theories that anti-consumption lifestyles may play a role in achieving more sustainable societies [1,2,19,20,21]. Few studies have considered various lifestyles in a single framework, as well as explored how emissions impact knowledge moderates the effect of anti-consumption on environmental impact. Our findings demonstrate that emissions impact knowledge increases the negative association between tightwadism and environmental impact. This finding should motivate researchers to investigate additional conditions and personality traits that may further increase one’s motivation to reduce consumption. The results of this study also indicate that environmental and ethical concerns fail to generate a positive impact on the environment, thereby reinforcing the “green gap” raised by previous studies. Surprisingly, we found that ethically minded consumption is positively associated with environmental impact. Future research should investigate this finding, as it seems to suggest that one’s ethical concerns seems to conflict with other needs and desires, leading to a failure in reducing one’s environmental impact. While this research provides a foundation for future studies on anti-consumption lifestyles and their effects on the environment, it also paves the way for taking further measures to create a more sustainable society. Until recently, the ecological modernization approach to consumption and the dominant topic in sustainable development consisted in creating technological or efficiency-driven solution for consumers. While some resources can benefit from these improvements, others can only be protected by restricting final consumption. This research reiterates the importance of turning away from traditional material-driven consumption patterns, given that environmental and ethical concerns appear to have a limited effect on environmental impact reduction, toward more anti-consumption-based lifestyles such as tightwadism. We propose that adopting a tightwad lifestyle can substantially reduce annual personal emissions and environmental impact to build a strong foundation for living a sustainable life. In this context, it seems essential to move away from this emphasis on technology and efficiency improvements, which give the delusive promise that environmental protection and materialistic lifestyles can coexist.
Public policy makers and companies concerned with achieving significant progress towards sustainable development have a role to play in this transition to environmentally sustainable consumption. Instead of focusing on environmentally and ethically conscious messages in their practices and campaigns, with much smaller potential emissions reductions, our findings should encourage them to promote and endorse values such as tightwadism, sufficiency, and consumption reduction. Furthermore, our findings suggest that policy makers should enhance knowledge around the impact of consumption reduction on the environment and raise the question of consumption reduction in the public debate to make it acceptable for consumers and gradually integrate it into the dominant consumption ideology. This call for sufficiency is not limited to policy makers, but also to commercial marketers [110].

Author Contributions

L.T. worked on the conceptualization, methodology, data collection, formal analyses, writing of the original manuscript and project administration. M.V.N. worked on the conceptualization, methodology, formal analyses, data curation supervision and writing of the original draft, review and editing of the text. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. García-de-Frutos, N.; Ortega-Egea, J.M.; Martínez-del-Río, J. Anti-consumption for environmental sustainability: Conceptualization, review, and multilevel research directions. J. Bus. Ethics 2018, 148, 411–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Black, I.R.; Cherrier, H. Anti-consumption as part of living a sustainable lifestyle: Daily practices, contextual motivations and subjective values. J. Consum. Behav. 2010, 9, 437–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Chatzidakis, A.; Lee, M.S. Anti-consumption as the study of reasons against. J. Macromark. 2013, 33, 190–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Corral-Verdugo, V.; Bechtel, R.B.; Fraijo-Sing, B. Environmental beliefs and water conservation: An empirical study. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 247–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Howard-Grenville, J.; Buckle, S.J.; Hoskins, B.J.; George, G. Climate change and management. Acad. Manag. J. 2014, 57, 615–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Jackson, T. Live better by consuming less? Is there a “double dividend” in sustainable consumption? J. Ind. Ecol. 2005, 9, 19–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Peattie, K.; Peattie, S. Social marketing: A pathway to consumption reduction? J. Bus. Res. 2009, 62, 260–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Connolly, J.; Prothero, A. Sustainable consumption: Consumption, consumers and the commodity discourse. Consum. Mark. Cult. 2003, 6, 275–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Kilbourne, W.E.; Carlson, L. The dominant social paradigm, consumption, and environmental attitudes: Can macromarketing education help? J. Macromark. 2008, 28, 106–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kjellberg, H. Market practices and over-consumption. Consum. Mark. Cult. 2008, 11, 151–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Varey, R.J. Marketing means and ends for a sustainable society: A welfare agenda for transformative change. J. Macromark. 2010, 30, 112–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Carfora, V.; Catellani, P.; Caso, D.; Conner, M. How to reduce red and processed meat consumption by daily text messages targeting environment or health benefits. J. Environ. Psychol. 2019, 65, 101319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Palomo-Vélez, G.; Tybur, J.M.; van Vugt, M. Unsustainable, unhealthy, or disgusting? Comparing different persuasive messages against meat consumption. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 58, 63–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Taufik, D. Prospective “warm-glow” of reducing meat consumption in China: Emotional associations with intentions for meat consumption curtailment and consumption of meat substitutes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 60, 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Cowen, L.; Gatersleben, B. Testing for the size heuristic in householders’ perceptions of energy consumption. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 54, 103–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bamberg, S. Applying the stage model of self-regulated behavioral change in a car use reduction intervention. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 33, 68–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Chng, S.; Abraham, C.; White, M.P.; Hoffmann, C.; Skippon, S. Psychological theories of car use: An integrative review and conceptual framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 55, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Nayum, A.; Klöckner, C.A. A comprehensive socio-psychological approach to car type choice. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 401–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Nepomuceno, M.V.; Laroche, M. When materialists intend to resist consumption: The moderating role of self-control and long-term orientation. J. Bus. Ethics. 2017, 143, 467–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Kropfeld, M.I.; Nepomuceno, M.V.; Dantas, D.C. The Ecological Impact of Anticonsumption Lifestyles and Environmental Concern. J. Public Policy Mark. 2018, 37, 245–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Nasr, N. The beauty and the beast of consumption: A review of the consequences of consumption. J. Consum. Mark. 2019, 36, 911–925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lee, M.; Roux, D.; Cherrier, H.; Cova, B. Anti-consumption and consumer resistance: Concepts, concerns, conflicts and convergence. Eur. J. Mark. 2011, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Lee, M.S.; Fernandez, K.V.; Hyman, M.R. Anti-consumption: An overview and research agenda. J. Bus. Res. 2009, 62, 145–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Sheth, J.N.; Sethia, N.K.; Srinivas, S. Mindful consumption: A customer-centric approach to sustainability. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2011, 39, 21–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Mulrow, J.; Machaj, K.; Deanes, J.; Derrible, S. The state of carbon footprint calculators: An evaluation of calculator design and user interaction features. Sustain. Prod. Consum. 2018, 18, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Carrigan, M.; Szmigin, I.; Wright, J. Shopping for a better world? An interpretive study of the potential for ethical consumption within the older market. J. Consum. Mark. 2004, 21, 401–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Auger, P.; Devinney, T.M. Do what consumers say matter? The misalignment of preferences with unconstrained ethical intentions. J. Bus. Ethics. 2007, 76, 361–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Devinney, T.M.; Auger, P.; Eckhardt, G.; Birtchnell, T. The other CSR: Making consumers socially responsible. Stanf. Soc. Innov. Rev. 2006, 30–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Fan, Y. Ethical branding and corporate reputation. Corp. Commun. Int. J. 2005, 10, 341–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Roberts, J.A. Green consumers in the 1990s: Profile and implications for advertising. J. Bus. Res. 1996, 36, 217–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Uusitalo, L. Are environmental attitudes and behaviour inconsistent? Findings from a Finnish study. Scand. Political Stud. 1990, 13, 211–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Mostafa, M.M. Shades of green: A psychographic segmentation of the green consumer in Kuwait using self-organizing maps. Expert Syst. Appl. 2009, 36, 11030–11038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Oğuz, D.; Kavas, S. Environmental awareness of university students in Ankara, Turkey. Afr. J. Agric. Res. 2010, 5, 2629–2636. [Google Scholar]
  34. Suki, N.M. Young consumer ecological behaviour: The effects of environmental knowledge, healthy food, and healthy way of life with the moderation of gender and age. Manag. Environ. Qual. Int. J. 2013, 24, 726–737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lastovicka, J.L.; Bettencourt, L.A.; Hughner, R.S.; Kuntze, R.J. Lifestyle of the tight and frugal: Theory and measurement. J. Consum. Res. 1999, 26, 85–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Rick, S.I.; Cryder, C.E.; Loewenstein, G. Tightwads and spendthrifts. J. Consum. Res. 2007, 34, 767–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Iwata, O. Attitudinal and behavioral correlates of voluntary simplicity lifestyles. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 1997, 25, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Rich, S.A.; Wright, B.J.; Bennett, P.C. Development of the Voluntary Simplicity Engagement Scale: Measuring Low-Consumption Lifestyles. J. Consum. Policy 2019, 43, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Boulstridge, E.; Carrigan, M. Do consumers really care about corporate responsibility? Highlighting the attitude—Behaviour gap. J. Commun. Manag. 2000, 4, 355–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Carrigan, M.; Attalla, A. The myth of the ethical consumer–do ethics matter in purchase behaviour? J. Consum. Mark. 2001, 18, 560–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Chatzidakis, A.; Hibbert, S.; Smith, A.P. Why people don’t take their concerns about fair trade to the supermarket: The role of neutralisation. J. Bus. Ethics. 2007, 74, 89–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Carrigan, M.; Moraes, C.; Leek, S. Fostering responsible communities: A community social marketing approach to sustainable living. J. Bus. Ethics 2011, 100, 515–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Moraes, C.; Carrigan, M.; Szmigin, I. The coherence of inconsistencies: Attitude–behaviour gaps and new consumption communities. J. Mark. Manag. 2012, 28, 103–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ellen, P.S.; Wiener, J.L.; Cobb-Walgren, C. The role of perceived consumer effectiveness in motivating environmentally conscious behaviors. J. Public Policy Mark. 1991, 10, 102–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Wynes, S.; Nicholas, K.A. The climate mitigation gap: Education and government recommendations miss the most effective individual actions. Environ. Res. Lett. 2017, 12, 074024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Vandermerwe, S.; Oliff, M.D. Customers drive corporations. Long Range Plan. 1990, 23, 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Worcester, R.M. Public and elite attitudes to environmental issues. Int. J. Public Opin. Res. 1993, 5, 315–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Zavestoski, S. The social-psychological bases of anticonsumption attitudes. Psychol. Mark. 2002, 19, 149–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cherrier, H.; Black, I.R.; Lee, M. Intentional non-consumption for sustainability: Consumer resistance and/or anti-consumption? Eur. J. Mark. 2011, 45, 1757–1767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Nepomuceno, M.V.; Rohani, M.; Grégoire, Y. Consumer resistance: From anti-consumption to revenge. In Consumer Perception of Product Risks and Benefits; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  51. Grégoire, Y.; Fisher, R.J. Customer betrayal and retaliation: When your best customers become your worst enemies. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2008, 36, 247–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Grégoire, Y.; Laufer, D.; Tripp, M.T. A comprehensive model of customer direct and indirect revenge: Understanding the effects of perceived greed and customer power. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2010, 38, 738–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Eckhardt, G.M.; Belk, R.; Devinney, T.M. Why don’t consumers consume ethically? J. Consum. Behav. 2010, 9, 426–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Iyer, R.; Muncy, J.A. Purpose and object of anti-consumption. J. Bus. Res. 2009, 62, 160–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Leonard-Barton, D. Voluntary simplicity lifestyles and energy conservation. J. Consum. Res. 1981, 8, 243–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Craig-Lees, M.; Hill, C. Understanding voluntary simplifiers. Psychol. Mark. 2002, 19, 187–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Elgin, D. Voluntary Simplicity, 2nd ed.; Harper Collins: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  58. Prelec, D.; Loewenstein, G. The red and the black: Mental accounting of savings and debt. Mark. Sci. 1998, 17, 4–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Schor, J.B. The Overspent American: Why We Want What We don’t Need; Harper Perennial: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  60. Fujii, S. Environmental concern, attitude toward frugality, and ease of behavior as determinants of pro-environmental behavior intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 262–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Gärling, T.; Fujii, S.; Gärling, A.; Jakobsson, C. Moderating effects of social value orientation on determinants of proenvironmental behavior intention. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kim, H.S.; Damhorst, M.L. Environmental attitude and commitment in relation to ad message credibility. J. Fash. Mark. Manag. Int. J. 1999, 3, 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Antil, J.H. Socially responsible consumers: Profile and implications for public policy. J. Macromark. 1984, 4, 18–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Bord, R.J.; O’connor, R.E.; Fisher, A. In what sense does the public need to understand global climate change? Public Underst. Sci. 2000, 9, 205–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Moisander, J. Motivational complexity of green consumerism. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2007, 31, 404–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wiener, J.L.; Doescher, T.A. A framework for promoting cooperation. J. Mark. 1991, 55, 38–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Auld, J.W. Consumers, cars and communities: The challenge of sustainability. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2001, 25, 228–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Casimir, G.; Dutilh, C. Sustainability: A gender studies perspective. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2003, 27, 316–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Black, I. Sustainability through anti-consumption. J. Consum. Behav. 2010, 9, 403–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Bird, K.; Hughes, D.R. Ethical consumerism: The case of “Fairly–Traded” coffee. Bus. Ethics A Eur. Rev. 1997, 6, 159–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Carrigan, M.; Marinova, S.; Szmigin, I.; De Pelsmacker, P.; Janssens, W.; Sterckx, E.; Mielants, C. Consumer preferences for the marketing of ethically labelled coffee. Int. Mark. Rev. 2005, 22, 512–530. [Google Scholar]
  72. Crane, A. Unpacking the ethical product. J. Bus. Ethics. 2001, 30, 361–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Strong, C. Features contributing to the growth of ethical consumerism-a preliminary investigation. Mark. Intell. Plan. 1996, 14, 5–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Cowe, R.; Williams, S. Who are the Ethical Consumers? Co-Operative Bank/Mori Survey; The Cooperative Bank: Manchester, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  75. Gelb, B.D. More boycotts ahead? Some implications. Bus. Horiz. 1995, 38, 70–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Blend, J.R.; Van Ravenswaay, E.O. Measuring consumer demand for ecolabeled apples. Am. J. Agric. Econ. 1999, 81, 1072–1077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Loureiro, M.L.; McCluskey, J.J.; Mittelhammer, R.C. Will consumers pay a premium for eco-labeled apples? J. Consum. Aff. 2002, 36, 203–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Maietta, O.W. The hedonic price of fair trade coffee for the Italian consumer. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Agricultural Policy Reform and the WTO: Where Are We Heading? Capri, Italy, 23–26 June 2003. [Google Scholar]
  79. Trudel, R.; Cotte, J. Does being ethical pay? Wall Str. J. 2008, 12, R1. [Google Scholar]
  80. Billock, T.N. Shifting Consumer Attitudes in the US towards Fair Trade; Fair Trade Resources Network: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2004; Available online: http://fairtraderesource.org/resources.html (accessed on 19 May 2019).
  81. Dawkins, J. The Public’s Views of Corporate Responsibility 2003; Ipsos Mori: London, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  82. De Pelsmacker, P.; Driesen, L.; Rayp, G. Do consumers care about ethics? Willingness to pay for fair-trade coffee. J. Consum. Aff. 2005, 39, 363–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Doane, D. Taking flight: The rapid growth of ethical consumerism. Lond. New Econ. Found. 2001, 1, 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  84. Transfair U.S.A. Fair Trade Coffee Facts and Figures. 2005. Available online: http://www.transfairusa.org/content/Downloads/2005Q2FactsandFigures.pdf (accessed on 26 November 2020).
  85. Vermeir, I.; Verbeke, W. Sustainable food consumption: Exploring the consumer attitude-behaviour gap. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 2004, 4, 268. [Google Scholar]
  86. Bray, J.; Johns, N.; Kilburn, D. An exploratory study into the factors impeding ethical consumption. J. Bus. Ethics. 2011, 98, 597–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. KPMG/Synovate. KPMG and Synovate ‘Retailers Are Not Influenced by Ethical Consumers; It’s Simply that a Socially Responsible Retailer is More Profitable’ Retail Think Tank White. 2007. Available online: http://www.retailthinktank.co.uk/news/news_releases/white_paper_-_july_2007/ (accessed on 26 November 2020).
  88. Olli, E.; Grendstad, G.; Wollebaek, D. Correlates of environmental behaviors: Bringing back social context. Environ. Behav. 2001, 33, 181–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Lozano, R. Incorporation and institutionalization of SD into universities: Breaking through barriers to change. J. Clean. Prod. 2006, 14, 787–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Kaiser, F.G.; Fuhrer, U. Ecological behavior’s dependency on different forms of knowledge. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 52, 598–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Attari, S.Z.; DeKay, M.L.; Davidson, C.I.; De Bruin, W.B. Public perceptions of energy consumption and savings. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2010, 107, 16054–16059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  92. Oppenheimer, D.M.; Meyvis, T.; Davidenko, N. Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 45, 867–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Iwata, O. An evaluation of consumerism and lifestyle as correlates of a voluntary simplicity lifestyle. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2006, 34, 557–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Nepomuceno, M.V.; Laroche, M. The impact of materialism and anti-consumption lifestyles on personal debt and account balances. J. Bus. Res. 2015, 68, 654–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Dunlap, R.E.; Van Liere, K.D.; Mertig, A.G.; Jones, R.E. New trends in measuring environmental attitudes: Measuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: A revised NEP scale. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 425–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Sudbury-Riley, L.; Kohlbacher, F. Ethically minded consumer behavior: Scale review, development, and validation. J. Bus. Res. 2016, 69, 2697–2710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Carbon Footprint Ltd. Carbon Calculator. 2017. Available online: https://www.carbonfootprint.com/calculator.aspx (accessed on 26 November 2020).
  98. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis Second Edition: A Regression-Based Approach; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  100. Evans, D. Thrifty, green or frugal: Reflections on sustainable consumption in a changing economic climate. Geoforum 2011, 42, 550–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2002, 8, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Kim, B.; Schuldt, J.P. Judging the environmental impact of green consumption: Evidence of quantity insensitivity. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 60, 122–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Mobley, C.; Vagias, W.M.; DeWard, S.L. Exploring additional determinants of environmentally responsible behavior: The influence of environmental literature and environmental attitudes. Environ. Behav. 2010, 42, 420–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Randall, D.M.; Fernandes, M.F. The social desirability response bias in ethics research. J. Bus. Ethics. 1991, 10, 805–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Gossen, M.; Ziesemer, F.; Schrader, U. Why and how commercial marketing should promote sufficient consumption: A systematic literature review. J. Macromark. 2019, 39, 252–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Statista. Educational Attainment Distribution in the United States from 1960 to 2019. 2020. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/184260/educational-attainment-in-the-us/ (accessed on 26 November 2020).
  107. Statista. Total Population in the United States by Gender from 2010 to 2025. 2020. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/737923/us-population-by-gender/ (accessed on 26 November 2020).
  108. Statista. Median Age of the Resident Population of the United States from 1960 to 2019. 2020. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/241494/median-age-of-the-us-population/ (accessed on 26 November 2020).
  109. Statista. Marital Status of the U.S. Population in 2019, by Sex. 2020. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/242030/marital-status-of-the-us-population-by-sex/ (accessed on 26 November 2020).
  110. Statista. Percentage Distribution of Household Income in the U.S. in 2019. 2020. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/203183/percentage-distribution-of-household-income-in-the-us/ (accessed on 26 November 2020).
Table 1. Real score attributed to individual actions according to their potential in reducing CO2 emissions.
Table 1. Real score attributed to individual actions according to their potential in reducing CO2 emissions.
Individual ActiontCO2/Year Reduction PotentialReal Score Attributed (on a Scale of 1 to 7)
High impact actionsMore than 0.8 tCO2/year
Have one fewer child58.67
Live car free2.406
Avoid one transatlantic flight (i.e., have the means to take a transatlantic flight, but still avoid it)1.605
Buy green energy (e.g., install solar panels in your home; buy an Energy Star HVAC system)1.405
Buy a more efficient car (i.e., a car with at least 5 miles per gallon above the American average)1.194
Adopt a plant-based diet (i.e., avoid the intake of animal products such as meat, milk, eggs, etc.)0.804
Moderate impact actionsBetween 0.2 and 0.8 tCO2/year
Replace gasoline car by hybrid *0.503
Wash clothes in cold water0.252
Recycle0.212
Hand-dry clothes0.212
Low impact actionsLess than 0.2 tCO2/year
Upgrade lightbulbs0.101
Plant a tree0.031
* Increasing fuel economy might be more effective in terms of tCO2 reduction than switching to a hybrid car (Wynes and Nicholas 2017).
Table 2. Calculation of CO2 emissions per expense category.
Table 2. Calculation of CO2 emissions per expense category.
Expenditure CategoriesSurvey IndicationsSub-Categories1 Metric ton of CO2e is Equivalent to:
Household EnergyParticipants had to estimate their household consumption ($USD) of each type of energy for the past 12 months.Electricity$2894
Natural gas$9
Heating oil$40
Coal$12
Liquefied petroleum gas (LPG)$405
Propane$388
Wood pellets$14
TransportationParticipants had to provide details about their travel itineraries by plane over the last 12 months, specifying the city of departure and destination, the stops made, and class type they traveled in (e.g., economy, business, etc.). FlightsAmount of $USD for 1 metric ton of CO2e depends on many variables, such as the class type, the itinerary, etc.
Participants had to report the number of miles driven with their vehicle(s) in the last 12 months, providing accurate information on the vehicle: manufacturer, model, year of manufacture, the fuel type, whether it is manual or automatic, its trim level and fuel efficiency (miles/gallon).CarAmount of $USD for 1 metric ton of CO2e depends on many variables, such as the fuel type and efficiency.
If participants have a motorcycle, they had to provide the motorcycle type they own as well as the number of miles driven.MotorcycleAmount of $USD for 1 metric ton of CO2e depends on the motorcycle type and fuel efficiency.
If the participants used public transport in the last 12 months, they had to indicate the mileage travelled (per week, month, or year) by commuter train, long-distance train, local bus, coach bus, subway, tram, and taxi. Commuter train14,286 miles
Long-distance train100,000 miles
Local bus5882 miles
Coach bus25,000 miles
Subway20,000 miles
Tramway16,667 miles
Taxi4167 miles
Other Expenses (products and services)This is a measure of the emissions caused through the manufacture, delivery and disposal of products and services participants buy. Participants had to estimate the amount spent (per week, month, or year) on different categories of goods and services. Food and beverages *$1250
Pharmaceuticals$2500
Clothing, textiles and shoes$3750
Telephone, mobile/cellphone call costs$2041
Hotel, restaurants and pubs$2439
Banking and financial sector (mortgage and interest payments)$6667
Insurance$4000
Paper-based products (e.g., books and magazines)$1886
Computer and equipment$1923
Television, radio and phone (equipment)$3226
Motor vehicles (excluding fuel costs)$1613
Furniture and other manufactured good$2500
Education$5000
* Indicates CO2 emissions that were divided by the number of people in the household. For electricity, 1 metric ton of CO2e at 0.053 kgCO2e/kWh is equal to 18,867 KWh. The amount in dollars is 15.34¢/kWh. For natural gas, 1 metric ton of CO2e is equal to 5000 KWh. The amount in dollars is 0.18¢/kWh. For heating oil 1 metric ton of CO2e is equal to 104 US gallons. The amount in dollars is 2.60/US gallon. For coal, 1 metric ton of CO2e is equal to $33.72/ton. For LPG, 1 metric ton of CO2e is equal to 174 US gallons. The amount in dollars is 2.33/US gallon. For propane, 1 metric ton of CO2e is equal to 174 US gallons. The amount in dollars is 2.33/US gallon. For wood pellets, 1 metric ton of CO2e is equal to 0.07 ton of wood. The amount in dollars is $14/ton. The sources used for the calculations were: https://www.bls.gov; https://www.eia.gov; https://www.globalpetrolprices.com/; https://www.walmart.com.
Table 3. Items used to measure each scale and respective statistical parameters.
Table 3. Items used to measure each scale and respective statistical parameters.
Factor LoadingMeanStandard Deviation
Frugality ScaleIf you take good care of your possessions, you will definitely save money in the long run.0.7856.1791.553
There are many things that are normally thrown away that are still quite useful.0.6855.5011.638
Making better use of my resources makes me feel good.0.8515.9441.555
If you can re-use an item you already have, there’s no sense in buying something new.0.8205.8181.619
I believe in being careful in how I spend my money.0.8585.8431.609
I discipline myself to get the most from my money.0.8185.4791.626
I am willing to wait on a purchase I want so that I can save money.0.8405.5691.598
There are things I resist buying today so I can save for tomorrow.0.8225.5101.657
Voluntary Simplicity ScaleI fully adhere to a simple lifestyle and only buy necessities.0.7933.2771.146
I never buy impulsively.0.6292.8881.182
I only shop after seriously considering whether or not an item is absolutely necessary.0.7193.3751.086
I am more concerned with personal growth and fulfilment than with material possessions.0.5683.7981.091
Even when I have money, I never buy things unexpectedly.0.7103.0701.205
I want to grow my own food in the future.0.5362.9861.474
In the future, I want to produce my own goods (such as clothes and tools).0.4712.2611.295
I would adopt a simple lifestyle even if I were able to live extravagantly.0.7163.2831.176
A simple lifestyle makes you financially independent from others.0.6053.7651.074
Environmental ConcernWe are approaching the limit of the number of people the earth can support.0.5083.8911.952
Humans have the right to modify the natural environment to suit their needs. (R) *---
When humans interfere with nature it often produces disastrous consequences.0.7394.8861.959
Human ingenuity will insure that we do NOT make the earth unliveable. (R) *---
Humans are severely abusing the environment.0.8045.0461.972
The earth has plenty of natural resources if we just learn how to develop them. (R) *---
Plants and animals have as much rights as humans to exist.0.6405.1741.994
The balance of nature is strong enough to cope with the impacts of modern industrial nations. (R) *---
Despite our special abilities humans are still subject to the laws of nature.0.5805.3341.796
The so-called ‘‘ecological crisis’’ facing humankind has been greatly exaggerated. (R) *--−1
The earth is like a spaceship with very limited room and resources.0.6283.7861.846
Humans were meant to rule over the rest of nature. (R) *---
The balance of nature is very delicate and easily upset. 0.7044.4031.906
Humans will eventually learn enough about how nature works to be able to control it. (R) *---
If things continue on their present course, we will soon experience a major ecological catastrophe.0.7574.6772.034
Tightwadism ScaleWhich of the following descriptions fits you better? Tightwad - Spendthrift (R)0.8275.5532.039
Do you have trouble limiting your spending? (R)0.7722.6800.925
Do you have trouble spending money?0.6472.7840.944
Who are you more similar to, Mr. A or Mr. B?0.6943.2531.244
Ethical ConsumptionWhen there is a choice, I always choose the product that contributes to the least amount of environmental damage.0.8133.2331.208
I have switched products for environmental reasons.0.8233.2531.295
If I understand the potential damage to the environment that some products can cause, I do not purchase those products.0.8023.5341.211
I do not buy household products that harm the environment.0.7573.2191.197
Whenever possible, I buy products packaged in reusable or recyclable containers.0.7633.7501.173
I make every effort to buy paper products (toilet paper, tissues, etc.) made from recycled paper.0.7563.1571.291
I will not buy a product if I know that the company that sells it is socially irresponsible.0.7973.3511.293
I do not buy products from companies that I know use sweatshop labor, child labor, or other poor working conditions.0.6323.7441.311
I have paid more for environmentally friendly products when there is a cheaper alternative.0.8013.2891.284
I have paid more for socially responsible products when there is a cheaper alternative.0.7863.2581.292
(R) = Denotes reversed items. * = Denote items removed from the analyses because the factor loading obtained was below 0.30 or because the item loaded in multiple factors. The statistics of the remaining items were calculated after the elimination of these items.
Table 4. Correlations between Anticonsumption Lifestyles, Environmental Concern, and Ethically Minded Consumption with Environmental Impact and Expenditures Categories.
Table 4. Correlations between Anticonsumption Lifestyles, Environmental Concern, and Ethically Minded Consumption with Environmental Impact and Expenditures Categories.
MSDTightwadismVoluntary SimplicityFrugalityEnvironmental ConcernEthically Minded Consumption
Overall Environmental Impact9.578.08−0.12 *−0.070.010.030.11 *
Environmental Impact of other expenses4.444.09−0.14 **−0.14 **0.030.050.10
- Food and beverages5.464.55−0.20 **−0.10 0.0180.0700.09
- Pharmaceuticals0.020.003−0.32 **−0.01−0.0560.1000.105
- Clothing, textiles and shoes0.250.38−0.33 **−0.11 −0.039−0.0200.008
- Telephone, mobile/cellphone call costs0.730.72−0.15 **−0.057−0.074−0.080−0.020
- Hotel, restaurants and pubs0.571.08−0.10 −0.15 *0.0080.102−0.048
- Banking and financial sector (mortgage and interest payments)1.263.080.033−0.0470.061−0.136 −0.017
- Insurance0.671.32−0.13 *0.0530.019−0.0310.039
- Paper-based products (e.g., books and magazines)0.040.07−0.14 *−0.11 −0.084−0.0000.010
- Computer and equipment0.120.25−0.081−0.17 *−0.004−0.065−0.075
- Television, radio and phone (equipment)0.140.27−0.1520.0330.095−0.077−0.053
- Motor vehicles (excluding fuel costs)0.501.060.009−0.092−0.049−0.137 0.019
- Furniture and other manufactured good0.232.15−0.18 *−0.18 *0.0380.017−0.016
- Education0.281.140.027−0.074−0.0500.145−0.115
Environmental impact of household energy2.323.31−0.10 0.010.03−0.04−0.003
Environmental impact of flights0.882.63−0.09−0.17 *−0.08−0.010.003
Environmental impact of private transportation (car and motorbike usage)1.451.97−0.060.010.10†0.005−0.04
Environmental impact of public transportation usage 0.443.54−0.02−0.06−0.15−0.01−0.07
p < 0.10. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01.
Table 5. Demographics of recruited participants and national average of the United States [104,105,106,107,108].
Table 5. Demographics of recruited participants and national average of the United States [104,105,106,107,108].
Current SampleNational Average (USA)
EducationEducation
Less than High School Degree1.43%Less than High School Degree9.90%
High School Graduate14.57%At least High School Degree, but not more than college degree54.10%
Some College/Associate’s Degree41.43%
Undergraduate University Degree (Bachelor)27.43%College Graduate or More36.00%
Graduate University Degree (Master/MBA)12.57%
PhD, Law, or Medical Degree2.29%
I don’t want to answer this question0.28%
SexSex
Male17.71%Male48.90%
Female81.29%Female51.10%
Average Age45.69Average Age38.4
Marital StatusMarital Status
Single27.51%Single32.40%
Married44.41%Married; Domestic partnership/Civil union52.29%
Domestic partnership/Civil union7.45%
Divorced/Separated17.19%Divorced/Separated9.66%
Widow2.87%Widow5.65%
I don’t want to answer this question0.57%
Income Income
I have no income0.86%Under $15,0009.10%
Between $1 and $30,00023.71%Between $15,000 and $24,9998.00%
Between $30,001 and $60,00027.43%Between $25,000 and $34,9998.30%
Between $60,001 and $90,00018.00%Between $35,000 and $49,99911.70%
Between $90,001 and $120,00013.14%Between $50,000 and $74,99916.50%
More than $120,00012.57%Between $75,000 and $99,99912.30%
I don’t want to answer this question4.29%Between $100,000 and $149,99915.50%
Between $150,000 and $199,9998.30%
More than $200,00010.3%
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Touchette, L.; Nepomuceno, M.V. The Environmental Impact of Consumption Lifestyles: Ethically Minded Consumption vs. Tightwads. Sustainability 2020, 12, 9954. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12239954

AMA Style

Touchette L, Nepomuceno MV. The Environmental Impact of Consumption Lifestyles: Ethically Minded Consumption vs. Tightwads. Sustainability. 2020; 12(23):9954. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12239954

Chicago/Turabian Style

Touchette, Laurie, and Marcelo Vinhal Nepomuceno. 2020. "The Environmental Impact of Consumption Lifestyles: Ethically Minded Consumption vs. Tightwads" Sustainability 12, no. 23: 9954. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12239954

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop