Next Article in Journal
New Insights on Flood Mapping Procedure: Two Case Studies in Poland
Next Article in Special Issue
Overview of the Legal Design Basis for Passive Fire Protection in Korea and the Direction of Further Development
Previous Article in Journal
Spatial Econometric Analysis of the Relationship between Urban Land and Regional Economic Development in the Beijing–Tianjin–Hebei Coordinated Development Region
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Consideration of the Door Opening Process in Pedestrian Flow: Experiments on Door Opening Direction, Door Handle Type, and Limited Visibility

1
School of Architectural, Civil, Environmental and Energy Engineering, Kyungpook National University, Daegu 41566, Korea
2
Department of Fire and Disaster Prevention Engineering, Changshin University, Changwon 51352, Korea
3
Department of Architectural and Fire Protection Engineering, Pukyong National University, Busan 48513, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2020, 12(20), 8453; https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208453
Submission received: 24 September 2020 / Accepted: 10 October 2020 / Published: 14 October 2020

Abstract

:
The door is a section prone to bottlenecks and is an important element in the study of pedestrian flow. Therefore, characteristics of doors (e.g., width, location, and the distance between doors) have been taken into consideration in the existing literature related to doors. According to several previous studies, it appears likely that the door opening process (DOP) influences pedestrian flow. However, the number of studies examining the DOP remains small. Therefore, to enhance understanding of pedestrian flow, we examined two door characteristics that could affect the DOP (opening direction (swing door: push or pull) and handle type (knob, lever, and panic bar)) and limited visibility. We conducted a walking experiment to take all variables (10 cases; 10 participants per case) into account. Statistical analysis was performed on the difference in movement times, and the results were as follows: (1) inclusion of the DOP affected pedestrian flow; (2) when visibility was limited, movement times with DOP inclusion increased significantly regardless of the door opening direction and handle type; (3) when the door opening direction was ‘push’, regardless of limited visibility and door handle type, movement times with DOP inclusion were significantly lower; and (4) the door handle type did not result in any significant difference in movement times with DOP inclusion. In addition, we calculated the delay time based on the experiment results, to include the DOP in pedestrian flow (push 1.96–2.88 s, pull 3.91–4.43 s; limited visibility: push 7.38–12.56 s, and pull 12.88–16.35 s). The results of this study could be used as basic data for the development of codes/regulations, engineering guidance, and egress models for doors.

1. Introduction

Understanding pedestrian flow during a fire in a building is a key problem and should be considered in improving the accuracy of an evacuation model [1,2,3,4]. Studies related to pedestrian flow were conducted in relation to building conditions (e.g., building use, size, number of floors, number of rooms, size and number of doors, and presence or absence of escape stairs) [5,6,7], pedestrian characteristics (e.g., number of pedestrians, age and gender distribution, interaction, physical characteristics, familiarity with the building, and roles) [4,8,9], and evacuation environment (e.g., day/night, season, smoke, heat, and presence of toxic gas) [10,11,12,13,14]. Among them, building conditions not only determine the evacuation capacity of the building at the design stage but also affect pedestrian flow [15]. In addition, building conditions are associated with the egress route that pedestrians take in evacuation and have therefore been considered in many studies. These previous studies have examined the relationship between pedestrian flow and building conditions, including building use (e.g., high-rise building and underground) [16,17,18], stairs [5,19,20], corridors [21,22,23,24], and doors [25,26,27,28,29]. A door is one of the important building conditions as it is an exit for pedestrian evacuation. In addition, as bottlenecks are likely to occur, various studies related to doors have been carried out. Some studies have shown that wider door width was associated with faster pedestrian walking speed [8,25]. However, other studies have shown that when door width was increased to a certain level, walking speed no longer increased [9,27]. Moreover, door location exerted an effect on pedestrian flow and was an important factor in actual fire situations [28,30]. In addition, when multiple doors were present inside a building, short distances between doors led to longer movement time [29].
Therefore, the existing literature showed that a number of studies have been conducted on door characteristics. The above studies were conducted on the premise that the door was open. However, studies on some of the factors that can be included in the door opening process (DOP) support that DOP can affect evacuation. For example, depending on dynamic width (width depending on the degree of door opening), available exit width was smaller relative to the actual door width. In addition, the need for greater force to open doors has been associated with greater interference with evacuation [31]. Considering that the majority of people keep doors closed in daily life [32], there is a high probability that doors will be closed in an evacuation situation. In addition, the results of studies examining the extended degree of the door leaf [21] and door type [33] support the claim that closed doors must be taken into consideration in evacuation situations. In addition, depending on the handle type (between knob and lever), the degree of recognition that people perceive varies significantly [34], and the force required and usability vary [35]. In such situations, people generally evacuate by passing through the door of the room, and there is a high probability that the final or emergency exit will be made through a door. Therefore, the DOP is an important factor in understanding pedestrian flow through doors, but no studies have examined door opening direction (e.g., push and pull) or handle type (e.g., knob, lever, and panic bar).
Therefore, the purpose of the study was to improve the understanding of pedestrian flow, by analyzing the relationships between two DOP-related variables (opening direction and handle type), on pedestrians’ movement time. In addition, an analysis was conducted on the condition of limited visibility of pedestrians in consideration of the fire situation. In this study, an experimental set was developed to analyze the relationship between the three variables, and a walking experiment was conducted by separating the experimental cases. In addition, the difference in movement time by the experiment case was analyzed statistically. Finally, the delay time due to the DOP was presented.

2. Method and Procedure

The concept shown in Figure 1 was used to analyze the effects of the DOP. The process of a pedestrian passing through a door is as follows: (1) walking before the door passage (Figure 1a); (2) walking from the time the pedestrian holds the door handle to the time he/she opens and exits the door (Figure 1b); and (3) walking after the door passage (Figure 1c). Figure 1b shows the DOP. When doors are open, pedestrians can skip the process in Figure 1b during evacuation. When doors are closed, evacuation could be delayed or walking speed reduced by the process shown in Figure 1b.

2.1. Experimental Setup

In this study, experiment cases were set up with three variables to achieve the purpose of the study. The experiment was planned as one-person walking, excluding the influence of other pedestrians, to specifically observe the results of the conditions of the variables. All of the experiments were video-recorded, and the movement time was measured accordingly. The experimental process is shown in Figure 2.
The experiment was conducted to analyze movement time, with the door opening direction, door handle type as independent variables. Door handles were divided into three types, as shown in Figure 2a: (1) knob (typical handle), (2) lever (typical handle), and (3) panic bar (handle used for emergency exit). The doors were swing doors, and opening directions were divided into two types: (1) push and (2) pull. As mentioned in the introduction, in addition to building conditions, various factors influence the pedestrian flow. Moreover, the DOP is influenced by correlations with other factors that affect pedestrian flow [36,37,38]. Among them, limited visibility was chosen because it could reduce pedestrians’ walking speed and impedes evacuation [10,11,12,13,38]. Limited visibility caused by smoke was simulated using non-transparent eyepatches (extinction coefficient 1.0, visible distance 1–2 m), which have been used in various experiments [11,39]. Although an eyepatch is different from actual smoke, it was chosen because it was safe and participants could have similar experiences.
The cases were constructed by considering all factors of opening direction, handle type, and limited visibility. However, only one opening direction was considered for the panic bar, because it could only be pushed open. Therefore, the study included 10 cases to account for all variables, and a detailed description is provided in Table 1. Cases 1-1 to 3 were normal conditions and Cases 4-1 to 6 were limited visibility conditions.
Experiments were conducted in a custom-developed experimental set, to control for variables excluded from Table 1. The experimental set was constructed with wooden beams and column, and the walls were constructed using thick paper to create a corridor with a door. The size and shape of the experimental set are shown in Figure 3. The entire area was 7.2 m × 3.6 m in size, and the width of the door installed was 0.9 m (minimum door width for Korea Standard; Ministry of Land, Infrastructure and Transport). In addition, the starting and arrival points were marked 3 m from the experimental set, to allow pedestrians to enter the corridor at normal walking speed.
The participants were recruited via an advertisement at a university in Korea, a website, and social network services. The participants aged between 20 and 40 years, and the total number of participants was 100 (43 males, 57 females). All participants were college students or faculty members without physical disabilities and did not have problems with walking. All procedures of our experiment were approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the university. The experiments were conducted according to the following procedure.
(1)
Participants who agreed to participate in the experiment recorded personal information and were photographed for identification.
(2)
With the door open, participants’ normal walking speed was measured and recorded. Participants measured their speed individually. In addition, participants walked from the starting point to the arrival point, and the speed was calculated on the basis of the distance moved in the experimental set (7.2 m; mean normal walking speed: 1.21 m/s, SD = 0.22 m/s). Participants’ movement time was measured from the time the front foot entered the measurement area to the moment the back foot exited the measurement area.
(3)
Each participant departed from the starting point, passed through the door, and walked to the arrival point, as shown in Figure 3. Staff members were stationed to ensure participant safety in cases in which non-transparent eyepatches were used (Cases 4-1 to 6).
(4)
Experiments were conducted for 1 participant at a time, and each case included 10 participants.

2.2. Data Analysis

To analyze changes in participants’ movement times resulting from the DOP, 6 closed-circuit televisions (CCTVs) were installed (at the starting point, arrival point, on both sides of the door, and between each point and the door) to record participants’ behavior, as shown in Figure 4. The CCTVs were secured to the cross beams in the experimental set, and the camera angle was set vertically to capture images of the area directly below. In addition, lines were marked 1 m apart on the floor, because the exact locations of the participants were difficult to determine in the video recordings because of the height and curvature of the lens (yellow line in Figure 5). The video recordings were analyzed at 0.25 s intervals, and the CCTV images are shown in Figure 5.
In addition, participants’ movement times were measured for the whole section and DOP section, as shown in Figure 4. The measuring section for the movement time in the whole section was defined as the points at which the participants entered and exited the experimental set (i.e., it was not measured from the starting point to the arrival point). In addition, the measuring section for the movement time in the DOP section was defined as the points at which the participants held the door handle and opened and exited through the door.
To determine whether participants’ movement times changed depending on the inclusion of the DOP, the movement time for each case was compared with the movement time measured at normal walking speed (with the door open). We performed a statistical analysis to determine whether inclusion of the DOP exerted a significant effect on movement time. We determined statistical significance using a non-parametric statistical method (Mann–Whitney U and Kruskal–Wallis tests), as the groups did not satisfy normality. The statistical tests were performed at a significance level of 0.05. However, four hypotheses (movement time difference (a) with and without the DOP, (b) with and without limited visibility, (c) between two opening directions, and (d) among three handle types) were tested in this study. Therefore, individual statistical tests were tested at a significance probability of 0.01 to control familywise type I errors below 5% (Bonferroni correction, 0.05/4).
The analysis was conducted in the following order: (1) it was analyzed whether the increase in movement time with DOP inclusion is significant compared to the movement time when it is not; (2) differences in movement time depending on the visibility condition (with or without non-transparent eyepatches) were analyzed with the same door opening direction and handle type (e.g., Case 1-1 and 4-1); (3) difference in movement time of participants according to the opening direction was analyzed (e.g., Case 1-1 and Case 1-2); and (4) difference in movement time of participants according to handle type was analyzed (e.g., Case 1-1, Case 2-1, Case 3). Then, the above differences were analyzed using the movement time of the DOP section. This is because eyepatches can affect the walking speed of participants in sections other than the DOP section.

3. Results

3.1. Movement Times and Walking Speeds According to the Experimental Case

Participants’ movement times and walking speeds while passing through the DOP and whole sections for each case are shown in Table 2 and Figure 6. For this purpose, the average movement time and walking speed in the whole section were analyzed to compare the normal walking speed of the participants. The average movement time in the DOP section was analyzed to find the duration the participants spent on the DOP.
Overall, the participants’ mean walking speed was slower relative to their mean normal walking speed (1.21 m/s), which was measured with the door open, in each case. In addition, participants’ walking speeds differed according to the door opening direction (Cases 1-1 and 1-2/Cases 2-1 and 2-2/Cases 4-1 and 4-2/Cases 5-1 and 5-2) and handle type (Cases 1-1, 2-1, and 3/Cases 4-1, 5-1, and 6). Moreover, walking speeds for cases involving limited visibility conditions, in which participants wore non-transparent eyepatches (Cases 4-1 to 6), were significantly slower relative to those for cases involving normal conditions (Cases 1-1 to 3). These differences were observed in the movement times of both the DOP section and whole section without the DOP section (e.g., Case 1-1 (7.30 s = 9.00 s − 1.70 s) < Case 4-1 (13.52 s = 16.05 s − 2.53 s). However, differences in movement times in cases involving limited visibility conditions were greater relative to those observed for cases involving normal conditions (Figure 6).

3.2. Analysis of Differences in Movement Times between Cases with and without the DOP

The results showed that movement times for all cases with the DOP were significantly longer (increases ranged from 32.0% to 267.2%) relative to those for cases without the DOP (Table 2). The results of the statistical analysis performed to determine whether DOP inclusion exerted a significant effect on movement times are shown in Table 3. In addition, movement times for the whole section without the DOP increased in the cases involving limited visibility conditions (Cases 4-1 to 6) and were therefore excluded. For the remaining cases, the results showed significant differences in movement times between cases with and without DOP (Case 1-1: p < 0.001; Case 1-2: p < 0.001; Case 2-1: p < 0.001; Case 2-2: p < 0.001; Case 3: p < 0.001).

3.3. Analysis of Differences in Movement Times According to the Presence or Absence of Limited Visibility

As shown in Table 3, participants’ movement times in the cases involving limited visibility conditions (Cases 4-1 to 6) were significantly longer relative to those in cases involving normal conditions (Cases 1-1 to 3). The results of the analysis of movement times with DOP inclusion according to the presence or absence of limited visibility (with or without non-transparent eyepatches) showed significant differences in movement times between cases involving normal and limited visibility conditions (knob, push, p = 0.005; knob, pull, p < 0.001; lever, push, p = 0.002; lever, pull, p < 0.001; and panic bar, push, p < 0.001; Table 4). This indicated that the presence or absence of limited visibility exerted an effect on movement times with DOP inclusion.

3.4. Analysis of Differences in Movement Times According to the Door Opening Direction

The results of the analysis of handle types between cases involving normal (Cases 1-1 to 2-2) and limited visibility (Cases 4-1 to 5-2) conditions showed that for the knob-type handle (Cases 1-1 and 1-2), movement times were fastest when the door opening direction was push (1.70 s), and for the lever-handle (Cases 2-1 and 2-2), movement times were fastest when door opening direction was push (1.35 s). There were significant differences in movement times according to the door opening direction (knob: p < 0.001; lever: p < 0.001; Table 5). These results indicated that movement times were shortest when the push door opening direction was used, regardless of the type of door handle.
The results of the analysis of cases involving limited visibility conditions showed that for the knob-type handle (Cases 4-1 and 4-2), movement times were fastest when the door opening direction was push (2.53 s), and for the lever-type handle (Cases 5-1 and 5-2), movement times were fastest when the door opening direction was push (2.13 s). There were significant differences in movement times according to the door opening direction (knob: p < 0.001; lever: p < 0.001; Table 6). These results indicated that movement times were shortest when the push door opening direction was used, regardless of the handle type, in cases involving limited visibility conditions. Therefore, movement times were shortest when the push door opening direction was used in cases involving both normal and limited visibility conditions.

3.5. Analysis of Differences in Movement Times According to the Handle Type

Since limited visibility and door opening direction resulted in significant differences in evaluation times, all cases were analyzed for both variables. Therefore, the cases were split into normal condition-push cases (Case 1-1, 2-1, and 3), limited visibility condition-push cases (Cases 4-1, 5-1, and 6), normal condition-pull cases (Cases 1-2 and 2-2), and limited visibility condition-pull cases (Cases 4-2 and 5-2) in the analysis.
Movement times for the normal condition-push cases were shortest for the panic bar-type handle (1.18 s) and longest for the knob-type handle (1.70 s). No significant differences in movement times were observed between door handle types (p = 0.108; Table 7). This result indicated that the door handle type did not exert an effect on movement times with DOP inclusion when the push door opening direction was used in cases involving normal conditions.
Movement times for the limited visibility condition-push cases were shortest for the panic bar-type handle (2.13 s) and longest for the knob-type handle (2.52 s). No significant difference in movement times was observed between door handle types (p = 0.224; Table 8). This result indicated that door handle type did not exert an effect on movement times with DOP inclusion when the push door opening direction was used in cases involving limited visibility conditions.
Movement times for the normal condition-pull cases (Cases 1-2 and 2-2) were shortest for the lever-type handle (2.93 s). No significant difference in movement times was observed between door handle types (p = 0.908; Table 9). This result indicated that the door handle type did not exert an effect on movement times with DOP inclusion when the pull door opening direction was used in cases involving normal conditions.
Movement times for limited visibility condition-pull cases (Cases 4-2 and 5-2) were shortest for the lever-type handle (5.15 s). No significant difference in movement times was observed between door handle types (p = 0.197; Table 10). This result indicated that door handle type did not exert an effect on movement times with DOP inclusion when the pull door opening direction was used in cases involving limited visibility conditions.
The results of the analysis of handle type showed that handle type did not exert a significant effect on movement times with DOP inclusion in any cases (normal condition-push, limited visibility condition-push, normal condition-pull, and limited visibility condition-pull).

4. Discussion

This study examined the effects of the DOP on pedestrian flow. The purpose was to investigate the relationships between three variables (opening direction, handle type, and limited visibility) and participants’ walking speed. The results suggested that the effects of the DOP on the evacuation process should be considered when calculating movement time. The findings could have implications for the development of codes/regulations, engineering guidance, and egress models related to doors. In addition, future research examining correlations between the DOP and other factors that could influence pedestrian flow through doors could help to clarify current understanding of this flow.
In all cases with the DOP, movement times were longer relative to those observed for cases without the DOP. The results showed significant differences between cases with and without the DOP (Case 1-1: p < 0.001; Case 1-2: p < 0.001; Case 2-1: p < 0.001; Case 2-2: p < 0.001; and Case 3: p < 0.001). When a large number of pedestrians pass through a door, the movement time may vary with changes in door width [8,25] or crowd density [40,41,42,43]. However, this experiment considered one-person walking, and the door width was fixed at 0.9 m. Therefore, the DOP exerted a significant effect on pedestrian flow.
Limited visibility significantly increased movement times for all cases. Table 2 shows that the movement time for the whole section increased. In other words, walking speed decreased (knob/push: 0.80–0.45 m/s; knob/pull: 0.68–0.32 m/s; lever/push: 0.89–0.53 m/s; lever/pull: 0.72–0.38 m/s; and panic bar/push: 0.83–0.39 m/s). In addition, movement times for the DOP section increased. This could be attributed to two factors. First, participants would not have known the location of the door because of the use of non-transparent eyepatches. Smoke is a major factor resulting in delays in pedestrian flow [10,11,12,13,38], which is supported by research results indicating that pedestrian flow through doors was impeded by smoke even when doors were open [12]. Second, participants could have experienced difficulty in operating the door handles because of the use of non-transparent eyepatches. Participants wearing non-transparent eyepatches did not knowing which handle type was installed, which was evident in the longer movement times recorded for participants wearing non-transparent eyepatches (panic bar/limited visibility: 2.50 s), relative to those observed for participants who were not wearing non-transparent eyepatches (panic bar: 1.18 s). In this study, there were limitations to using eyepatches in place of actual smoke. However, an eyepatch can fully reproduce pedestrians’ visibility depending on the concentration of smoke [11]. Additionally, the use of only one characteristic eyepatch (extinction coefficient 1.0, visible distance 1–2 m) was intended to prevent participants from recognizing the door opening direction and handle type before arriving at the door. Therefore, the eyepatches used in this experiment sufficiently simulated the limited visibility situation.
With respect to the door opening direction, movement times were fastest when the push door opening direction was used. As with door handle type, all cases were considered when comparing movement times, to control for the effects of other variables, and the push opening direction showed significantly shorter movement times relative to those for the pull opening direction. Based on experience, it is optimal for doors to open in the direction of an evacuation. This idea is supported by building and fire safety codes in various countries (e.g., NFPA101; National Fire Protection Association). However, considering exceptions based on installation standards and fire situations, it is necessary to consider cases involving the pull opening direction, as individuals could pass through various pull-direction doors depending on the building and fire situation. Therefore, analysis of pull-direction doors is necessary for the inclusion of this type of door in an evacuation model.
In contrast to our expectations, no significant difference in movement times was observed between handle types. To control for the effects of door opening direction and limited visibility, differences between handle types were compared with various combinations of these two factors, but no significant differences were observed. It is noteworthy that movement times were fastest for the panic bar handle in situations without limited visibility (panic bar: 1.18 s and lever: 1.35 s), whereas movement times were fastest for the lever handle in situations involving limited visibility (lever: 2.13 s, panic bar: 2.50 s; and opening direction: push). There are two possible explanations for this result. First, participants did not know the location of the door because they were wearing non-transparent eyepatches. This explanation is supported by previous studies indicating that smoke made it difficult to perceive one’s route [10,11,12,13,38]. Second, participants were unaware of the handle type because they were wearing non-transparent eyepatches and were unable to perceive the panic bar, which is not commonly used in Korea (although panic bars are installed and used in specific buildings, such as hospitals, their use is limited in general buildings in Korea). Therefore, participants were unable to perceive the panic bar when their visual fields were impaired, because they were unfamiliar with this type of handle. However, this is the authors’ opinion and should be examined further in future research.
Based on the current results, we aimed to present the difference in movement times between cases with and without the DOP as delay time. The delay time was defined as the DOP delay time and was calculated as follows.
T = t − t0,
T indicates the delay time for the DOP, t indicates the movement time for the whole section for each case (Table 2), and t0 indicates the movement time for the whole section when the door was open (see Table 2). The results calculated using Equation (1) are shown in Table 11. However, the delay time in cases involving limited visibility conditions (Cases 4-1 to 6) could change depending on the concentration of smoke or fire. In addition, this was a simulation experiment; therefore, application of the data from Table 11 should be performed with caution depending on the population and region (participants were aged between 20 and 40 years and had no physical abnormality).
One factor that should be considered in future research is the effect of the DOP on movement times with multiple pedestrians. When multiple pedestrians are passing through a door, which is affected by door width and pedestrian speed and density [40,41,42,43]. The flow rate (product of walking speed and crowd density) for pedestrians can be used in engineering calculations and evacuation models. However, the results observed in real situations and simulation experiments show values below the maximum planned flow rate [44]. This could be attributed to familiarity between pedestrians when exiting the door [45], competition between pedestrians (or selfish behavior) [3,9,46], and the degree to which the door is open [21,26]. For example, flow rate increases as competition increases, but if this exceeds a certain level, evacuation efficiency decreases [3]. In addition, depending on pedestrian density, the location of following pedestrians could change, and the door might not close completely, which could impede evacuation [21,26]. Therefore, to develop an accurate evacuation model, other factors excluded from the flow rate equation (product of walking speed and crowd density) for pedestrians should also be considered. Examination of the effects of the DOP on movement times with multiple pedestrians would contribute greatly to the development of an accurate evacuation model. Moreover, the comparison of results obtained with the DOP and existing research results related to flow rates would be a useful topic for future research.

5. Conclusions

In the current study, we analyzed changes in pedestrians’ movement times according to two door characteristics (opening direction and handle type) and limited visibility, to enhance understanding of pedestrian flow in cases with the DOP. To date, a number of studies have involved door passage to understand pedestrian flow, but few have considered the DOP. However, in general, people evacuate by passing through the door of the room in evacuation situations, and there is a strong probability that the final or emergency exit will be a door. Taking these facts into consideration, we determined that there is sufficient need to take the DOP into account. The following conclusions were drawn from the study.
(1)
There was a statistically significant difference between movement times with and without the DOP. Based on this finding, it is appropriate to include the DOP in the calculation of pedestrians’ movement times.
(2)
When visibility was limited, movement times with DOP inclusion increased significantly regardless of the door opening direction and handle type.
(3)
When the door opening direction was ‘push’, regardless of limited visibility and door handle type, movement times with DOP inclusion were significantly lower.
(4)
The handle type did not show a significant difference in movement times with DOP inclusion.
(5)
Based on the experimental results, the DOP delay time was calculated as follows: push 1.96–2.88 s and pull 3.91–4.43 s/(visibility is limited) push 7.38–12.56 s and pull 12.88–16.35 s.
DOP delay time could be applied to an evacuation model for the calculation of movement times for pedestrians passing through a door. However, the current results lack representativeness and are limited in that only a few factors were considered. Nonetheless, this was a basic study on the DOP, which has not been considered in evacuation models previously. The results of the current study could be useful for improving the accuracy of evacuation models designed to assess building safety.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.-Y.S.; Methodology, Y.-C.K.; Data curation, Y.-H.B.; Validation J.-Y.S. and Y.-H.B.; Formal analysis, Y.-H.B.; Investigation, R.-S.O.; Resources, W.-H.H. and J.-H.C.; Writing–original draft, J.-Y.S.; Writing—review and editing, Y.-C.K., R.-S.O., Y.-H.B., W.-H.H. and J.-H.C.; Visualization, J.-Y.S.; Software J.-Y.S.; Supervision, W.-H.H. and J.-H.C.; Project administration, J.-H.C.; Funding acquisition, J.-H.C.. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant (No. NRF-2018R1A2B3005951), which was funded by the Korean government (MSIT).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Gwynne, S.; Galea, E.R.; Owen, M.; Lawrence, P.J.; Filippidis, L. A review of the methodologies used in the computer simulation of evacuation from the built environment. Build. Environ. 1999, 34, 741–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Helbing, D.; Johansson, A. Pedestrian, Crowd, and Evacuation Dynamics. Encycl. Complex. Syst. Sci. 2013, 16, 6476–6495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Nicolas, A.; Bouzat, S.; Kuperman, M.N. Pedestrian flows through a narrow doorway: Effect of individual behaviours on the global flow and microscopic dynamics. Transp. Res. Part B Methodol. 2017, 99, 30–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Yanagisawa, D.; Kimura, A.; Tomoeda, A.; Nishi, R.; Suma, Y.; Ohtsuka, K.; Nishinari, K. Introduction of frictional and turning function for pedestrian outflow with an obstacle. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlinear Soft Matter Phys. 2009, 80, 36110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Huo, F.; Song, W.; Chen, L.; Liu, C.; Liew, K.M. Experimental study on characteristics of pedestrian evacuation on stairs in a high-rise building. Saf. Sci. 2016, 86, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Lovreglio, R.; Ronchi, E.; Nilsson, D. A model of the decision-making process during pre-evacuation. Fire Saf. J. 2015, 78, 168–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Wang, S.; Yue, H.; Zhang, B.; Li, J. Setting the Width of Emergency Exit in Pedestrian Walking Facilities. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 138, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Tian, W.; Song, W.; Ma, J.; Fang, Z.; Seyfried, A.; Liddle, J. Experimental study of pedestrian behaviors in a corridor based on digital image processing. Fire Saf. J. 2012, 47, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Song, X.; Ma, L.; Ma, Y.; Yang, C.; Ji, H. Selfishness- and Selflessness-based models of pedestrian room evacuation. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 2016, 447, 455–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Isobe, M.; Helbing, D.; Nagatani, T. Experiment, theory, and simulation of the evacuation of a room without visibility. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlinear Soft Matter Phys. 2004, 69, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Jeon, G.Y.; Hong, W.H. An experimental study on how phosphorescent guidance equipment influences on evacuation in impaired visibility. J. Loss Prev. Process. Ind. 2009, 22, 934–942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kobes, M.; Helsloot, I.; de Vries, B.; Post, J.G.; Oberijé, N.; Groenewegen, K. Way finding during fire evacuation; an analysis of unannounced fire drills in a hotel at night. Build. Environ. 2010, 45, 537–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Fridolf, K.; Andrée, K.; Nilsson, D.; Frantzich, H. The impact of smoke on walking speed. Fire Mater. 2014, 38, 744–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Zhang, G.; Zhu, G.; Yuan, G.; Wang, Y. Quantitative risk assessment methods of evacuation safety for collapse of large steel structure gymnasium caused by localized fire. Saf. Sci. 2016, 87, 234–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Helbing, D.; Buzna, L.; Johansson, A.; Werner, T. Self-organized pedestrian crowd dynamics: Experiments, simulations, and design solutions. Transp. Sci. 2005, 39, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Pauls, J. Calculating evacuation times for tall buildings. Fire Saf. J. 1987, 12, 213–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Aleksandrov, M.; Cheng, C.; Rajabifard, A.; Kalantari, M. Modelling and finding optimal evacuation strategy for tall buildings. Saf. Sci. 2019, 115, 247–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ronchi, E.; Arias, S.; La Mendola, S.; Johansson, N. A fire safety assessment approach for evacuation analysis in underground physics research facilities. Fire Saf. J. 2019, 108, 102839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Qu, Y.; Gao, Z.; Xiao, Y.; Li, X. Modeling the pedestrian’s movement and simulating evacuation dynamics on stairs. Saf. Sci. 2014, 70, 189–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sano, T.; Ronchi, E.; Minegishi, Y.; Nilsson, D. A pedestrian merging flow model for stair evacuation. Fire Saf. J. 2017, 89, 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Gwynne, S.M.V.; Kuligowski, E.D.; Kratchman, J.; Milke, J.A. Questioning the linear relationship between doorway width and achievable flow rate. Fire Saf. J. 2009, 44, 80–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Shiwakoti, N.; Gong, Y.; Shi, X.; Ye, Z. Examining influence of merging architectural features on pedestrian crowd movement. Saf. Sci. 2015, 75, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Chen, S.; Fu, L.; Fang, J.; Yang, P. The effect of obstacle layouts on pedestrian flow in corridors: An experimental study. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 2019, 534, 122333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Bae, Y.-H.; Kim, Y.-C.; Oh, R.-S.; Son, J.-Y.; Hong, W.-H.; Choi, J.-H. Walking speed reduction rates at intersections while wayfinding indoors: An experimental study. Fire Mater. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Nagai, R.; Fukamachi, M.; Nagatani, T. Evacuation of crawlers and walkers from corridor through an exit. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 2006, 367, 449–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Daamen, W.; Hoogendoorn, S. Capacity of doors during evacuation conditions. Procedia Eng. 2010, 3, 53–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Liao, W.; Tordeux, A.; Seyfried, A.; Chraibi, M.; Drzycimski, K.; Zheng, X.; Zhao, Y. Measuring the steady state of pedestrian flow in bottleneck experiments. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 2016, 461, 248–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Tavares, R.M. Design for horizontal escape in buildings: The use of the relative distance between exits as an alternative approach to the maximum travel distance. Saf. Sci. 2010, 48, 1242–1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Zhao, D.L.; Li, J.; Zhu, Y.; Zou, L. The application of a two-dimensional cellular automata random model to the performance-based design of building exit. Build. Environ. 2008, 43, 518–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Khamis, N.; Selamat, H.; Ismail, F.S.; Lutfy, O.F.; Haniff, M.F.; Nordin, I.N.A.M. Optimized exit door locations for a safer emergency evacuation using crowd evacuation model and artificial bee colony optimization. Chaos Solitons Fractals 2020, 131, 109505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. McDermott, H.; Haslam, R.; Gibb, A. Occupant interactions with self-closing fire doors in private dwellings. Saf. Sci. 2010, 48, 1345–1350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Hopkin, C.; Spearpoint, M.; Wang, Y. Internal door closing habits in domestic premises: Results of a survey and the potential implications on fire safety. Saf. Sci. 2019, 120, 44–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Fruin, J.J. Service Pedestrian Planning and Design; Metropolitan Association of Urban Designers and Environmental Planners: New York, NY, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  34. Paschoarelli, L.C.; Santos, R.; Bruno, P. Influence of door handles design in effort perception: Accessibility and usability. Work 2012, 41, 4825–4829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Paschoarelli, L.C.; Santos, R. Usability Evaluation of Different Door Handles; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2010; pp. 291–299. [Google Scholar]
  36. Boyce, K.; McConnell, N.; Shields, J. Evacuation response behaviour in unannounced evacuation of licensed premises. Fire Mater. 2017, 41, 454–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Shen, Y.; Wang, Q.S.; Yan, W.G.; Sun, J.H.; Zhu, K. Evacuation Processes of Different Genders in Different Visibility Conditions—An Experimental Study, Proceedings of the Procedia Engineering; Elsevier Ltd.: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; Volume 71, pp. 65–74. [Google Scholar]
  38. Seike, M.; Kawabata, N.; Hasegawa, M. Experiments of evacuation speed in smoke-filled tunnel. Tunn. Undergr. Space Technol. 2016, 53, 61–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Xie, W.; Lee, E.W.M.; Cheng, Y.; Shi, M.; Cao, R.; Zhang, Y. Evacuation performance of individuals and social groups under different visibility conditions: Experiments and surveys. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2020, 47, 101527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Helbing, D.; Farkas, I.; Vicsek, T. Simulating dynamical features of escape panic. Nature 2000, 407, 487–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Lee, R.S.C.; Hughes, R.L. Prediction of human crowd pressures. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2006, 38, 712–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Seyfried, A.; Steffen, B.; Klingsch, W.; Lippert, T.; Boltes, M. The Fundamental Diagram of Pedestrian Movement Revisited—Empirical Results and Modelling, Proceedings of the Traffic and Granular Flow’05; Schadschneider, A., Pöschel, T., Kühne, R., Schreckenberg, M., Wolf, D.E., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2007; pp. 305–314. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kretz, T.; Grünebohm, A.; Schreckenberg, M. Experimental study of pedestrian flow through a bottleneck. J. Stat. Mech. Theory Exp. 2006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Graat, E.; Midden, C.; Bockholts, P. Complex evacuation; effects of motivation level and slope of stairs on emergency egress time in a sports stadium. Saf. Sci. 1999, 31, 127–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Fu, L.; Cao, S.; Shi, Y.; Chen, S.; Yang, P.; Fang, J. Walking behavior of pedestrian social groups on stairs: A field study. Saf. Sci. 2019, 117, 447–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Haghani, M.; Sarvi, M.; Shahhoseini, Z. Evacuation behaviour of crowds under high and low levels of urgency: Experiments of reaction time, exit choice and exit-choice adaptation. Saf. Sci. 2020, 126, 104679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Concept of the door opening process: (a) walking before the door passage; (b) walking from the time the pedestrian holds the door handle to the time he/she opens and exits the door (DOP); (c) walking after the door passage.
Figure 1. Concept of the door opening process: (a) walking before the door passage; (b) walking from the time the pedestrian holds the door handle to the time he/she opens and exits the door (DOP); (c) walking after the door passage.
Sustainability 12 08453 g001
Figure 2. Experimental process: (a) handle types (knob, lever, and panic bar), eyepatch, and preparation process for the experimental set; (b) analysis of effects of the door opening process using closed-circuit televisions; (c) analysis of experimental data.
Figure 2. Experimental process: (a) handle types (knob, lever, and panic bar), eyepatch, and preparation process for the experimental set; (b) analysis of effects of the door opening process using closed-circuit televisions; (c) analysis of experimental data.
Sustainability 12 08453 g002
Figure 3. Size of the experimental set. After departing from the starting point, participants performed the door opening process and walked to the arrival point.
Figure 3. Size of the experimental set. After departing from the starting point, participants performed the door opening process and walked to the arrival point.
Sustainability 12 08453 g003
Figure 4. Movement time measurement in the whole and door opening process sections.
Figure 4. Movement time measurement in the whole and door opening process sections.
Sustainability 12 08453 g004
Figure 5. Closed-circuit television images. Lines were marked 1 m apart on the floor to analyze video recordings.
Figure 5. Closed-circuit television images. Lines were marked 1 m apart on the floor to analyze video recordings.
Sustainability 12 08453 g005
Figure 6. Movement time distribution by case. DOP (door opening process) refers to ‘movement time for DOP section’, and Whole refers to ‘movement time for whole section’.
Figure 6. Movement time distribution by case. DOP (door opening process) refers to ‘movement time for DOP section’, and Whole refers to ‘movement time for whole section’.
Sustainability 12 08453 g006
Table 1. Experimental cases.
Table 1. Experimental cases.
Case No.VisibilityHandle TypeOpening Direction
Case 1-1NormalKnobPush
Case 1-2NormalKnobPull
Case 2-1NormalLeverPush
Case 2-2NormalLeverPull
Case 3NormalPanic BarPush
Case 4-1LimitedKnobPush
Case 4-2LimitedKnobPull
Case 5-1LimitedLeverPush
Case 5-2LimitedLeverPull
Case 6LimitedPanic BarPush
Table 2. Results according to the case.
Table 2. Results according to the case.
Case No.Average Movement Time in DOP Section (s)Average Movement Time in Whole Section (s)Average Walking Speed in Whole Section (m/s)Percentage of Increase in Movement Time with and without DOP
Case 1-11.709.000.8047.1%
Case 1-22.9710.550.6872.4%
Case 2-11.358.080.8932.0%
Case 2-22.9210.030.7263.9%
Case 31.188.680.8341.8%
Case 4-12.5316.050.45162.3%
Case 4-25.6822.470.32267.2%
Case 5-12.1313.500.53120.6%
Case 5-25.1519.000.38210.5%
Case 6 2.5018.680.39205.1%
Average movement time at normal walking speed = 6.12 s; DOP = door opening process.
Table 3. Mann–Whitney U test results for movement times with and without DOP (* p < 0.01). The statistical analysis was performed on the difference between the normal walking speed and average movement time of each experimental case.
Table 3. Mann–Whitney U test results for movement times with and without DOP (* p < 0.01). The statistical analysis was performed on the difference between the normal walking speed and average movement time of each experimental case.
Case NoNRangeMin.Max.Average Movement Time (s)SDMann–Whitney Up-Value (N = 110)
Normal walking1004.004.008.006.120.10--
Case 1-1103.756.7510.509.001.2730.000.000 *
Case 1-2103.508.0011.5010.551.027.500.000 *
Case 2-1103.006.509.508.081.0675.000.000 *
Case 2-2103.008.0011.0010.031.017.500.000 *
Case 3104.256.5010.758.681.756.000.000 *
Abbreviations: DOP = door opening process; Min. = minimum; Max. = maximum; SD = standard deviation.
Table 4. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining movement times in the DOP section with and without limited visibility (* p < 0.01).
Table 4. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining movement times in the DOP section with and without limited visibility (* p < 0.01).
VisibilityNRangeMin.Max.Average Movement Time (s)SDMann–Whitney Up-Value (N = 20)
Case 1-1
(Normal)
101.251.002.251.700.4487.000.005 *
Case 4-1
(Limited)
101.501.753.252.530.53
Case 1-2
(Normal)
101.502.253.752.980.6199.500.000 *
Case 4-2
(Limited)
103.503.757.255.681.05
Case 2-1
(Normal)
101.000.751.751.350.3690.500.002 *
Case 5-1
(Limited)
101.251.502.752.12500.44
Case 2-2
(Normal)
101.252.253.502.930.49100.000.000 *
Case 5-2
(Limited)
102.004.256.255.150.78
Case 3
(Normal)
102.250.252.501.180.7492.500.000 *
Case 6
(Limited)
102.001.753.752.500.65
Abbreviations: DOP = door opening process; Min. = minimum; Max. = maximum; SD = standard deviation.
Table 5. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining opening direction in cases involving normal conditions (* p < 0.01).
Table 5. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining opening direction in cases involving normal conditions (* p < 0.01).
Opening DirectionNRangeMin.Max.Average Movement Time (s)SDMann–Whitney Up-Value (N = 20)
Case 1-1
(Push, Knob)
101.251.002.251.700.443.000.000 *
Case 1-2
(Pull, Knob)
101.502.253.752.980.61
Case 2-1
(Push, Lever)
101.000.751.751.350.360.000.000 *
Case 2-2
(Pull, Lever)
101.252.253.502.930.49
Abbreviations: Min. = minimum; Max. = maximum; SD = standard deviation.
Table 6. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining opening direction in cases involving limited visibility conditions (* p < 0.01).
Table 6. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining opening direction in cases involving limited visibility conditions (* p < 0.01).
Opening DirectionNRangeMin.Max.Average Movement Time (s)SDMann–Whitney Up-Value (N = 20)
Case 4-1
(Push, Knob)
101.501.753.252.530.530.000.000 *
Case 4-2
(Pull, Knob)
103.503.757.255.681.05
Case 4-1
(Push, Lever)
101.251.502.752.130.440.000.000 *
Case 4-2
(Pull, Lever)
102.004.256.255.150.78
Abbreviations: Min. = minimum; Max. = maximum; SD = standard deviation.
Table 7. Results of Kruskal–Wallis tests examining differences in movement times according to handle type (normal condition, push).
Table 7. Results of Kruskal–Wallis tests examining differences in movement times according to handle type (normal condition, push).
Handle TypeNRangeMinMaxAverage Movement Time (s)SDMean RankChi-Squarep-Value (N = 30)
Case 1-1
(Knob, Push)
101.251.002.251.700.4420.104.4460.108
Case 2-1
(Lever, Push)
101.000.751.751.350.3614.15
Case 3
(Panic Bar, Push)
102.250.252.501.180.7412.25
Abbreviations: Min. = minimum; Max. = maximum; SD = standard deviation.
Table 8. Results of Kruskal–Wallis tests examining differences in movement times according to handle type (limited visibility condition, push).
Table 8. Results of Kruskal–Wallis tests examining differences in movement times according to handle type (limited visibility condition, push).
Handle TypeNRangeMinMaxAverage Movement Time (s)SDMean RankChi-Squarep-Value (N = 30)
Case 4-1
(Knob, Push)
101.501.753.252.530.5318.102.9960.224
Case 5-1
(Lever, Push)
101.251.502.752.130.4411.70
Case 6
(Panic Bar, Push)
102.001.753.752.500.6516.70
Abbreviations: Min. = minimum; Max = maximum; SD = standard deviation.
Table 9. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining differences in movement times according to the handle type (normal condition, pull).
Table 9. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining differences in movement times according to the handle type (normal condition, pull).
Handle TypeNRangeMin.Max.Average Movement Time (s)SDMann–Whitney Up-Value (N = 20)
Case 1-2
(Knob, Pull)
101.502.253.752.980.6148.500.908
Case 2-2
(Lever, Pull)
101.252.253.502.930.49
Abbreviations: Min. = minimum; Max = maximum; SD = standard deviation.
Table 10. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining movement times according to handle type (limited visibility condition, pull).
Table 10. Results of Mann–Whitney U tests examining movement times according to handle type (limited visibility condition, pull).
Handle TypeNRangeMin.Max.Average Movement Time (s)SDMann–Whitney Up-Value (N = 20)
Case 4-2
(Knob, Pull)
103.503.757.255.681.0533.000.197
Case 5-2
(Lever, Pull)
102.004.256.255.150.78
Abbreviations: Min = minimum; Max = maximum; SD = standard deviation.
Table 11. Delay time for DOP.
Table 11. Delay time for DOP.
CategoryHandle TypeOpening DirectionVisibilityDOP Delay Time (s)
Case 1-1KnobPushNormal2.88
Case 1-2KnobPullNormal4.43
Case 2-1LeverPushNormal1.96
Case 2-2LeverPullNormal3.91
Case 3Panic barPushNormal2.56
Case 4-1KnobPushLimited9.93
Case 4-2KnobPullLimited16.35
Case 5-1LeverPushLimited7.38
Case 5-2LeverPullLimited12.88
Case 6Panic barPushLimited12.56
Abbreviations: DOP = door opening process.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Son, J.-Y.; Bae, Y.-H.; Kim, Y.-C.; Oh, R.-S.; Hong, W.-H.; Choi, J.-H. Consideration of the Door Opening Process in Pedestrian Flow: Experiments on Door Opening Direction, Door Handle Type, and Limited Visibility. Sustainability 2020, 12, 8453. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208453

AMA Style

Son J-Y, Bae Y-H, Kim Y-C, Oh R-S, Hong W-H, Choi J-H. Consideration of the Door Opening Process in Pedestrian Flow: Experiments on Door Opening Direction, Door Handle Type, and Limited Visibility. Sustainability. 2020; 12(20):8453. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208453

Chicago/Turabian Style

Son, Jong-Yeong, Young-Hoon Bae, Young-Chan Kim, Ryun-Seok Oh, Won-Hwa Hong, and Jun-Ho Choi. 2020. "Consideration of the Door Opening Process in Pedestrian Flow: Experiments on Door Opening Direction, Door Handle Type, and Limited Visibility" Sustainability 12, no. 20: 8453. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208453

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop