Next Article in Journal
How to Shape the Employees’ Sustainable Work Attitude: The Moderating Effect of Supervisor Attitudes
Next Article in Special Issue
Using Group Model Building to Foster Learning for Strategic Sustainable Development
Previous Article in Journal
Utilization of Earth-to-Air Heat Exchanger to Pre-Cool/Heat Ventilation Air and Its Annual Energy Performance Evaluation: A Case Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Combining Indigenous Knowledge and Modern Education to Foster Sustainability Competencies: Towards a Set of Learning Design Principles
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Knowledge of Student Teachers on Sustainable Land Use Issues–Knowledge Types Relevant for Teacher Education

by
Lisa Richter-Beuschel
1,* and
Susanne Bögeholz
1,2,*
1
Department of Biology Education, University of Göttingen, 37073 Göttingen, Germany
2
Centre of Biodiversity and Sustainable Land Use (CBL), University of Göttingen, 37073 Göttingen, Germany
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2020, 12(20), 8332; https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208332
Submission received: 4 September 2020 / Revised: 6 October 2020 / Accepted: 8 October 2020 / Published: 10 October 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Competencies in Education for Sustainable Development II)

Abstract

:
For restructuring educational processes and institutions toward Sustainable Development, teachers’ knowledge and competences are crucial. Due to the high relevance of teachers’ content knowledge, this study aimed to (i) assess Sustainable Development-relevant knowledge by differentiating between situational, conceptual and procedural knowledge, (ii) find out via item response theory modelling how these theoretically distinguished knowledge types can be empirically supported, and (iii) link the knowledge dimension(s) to related constructs. We developed a paper-and-pencil test to assess these three knowledge types (N = 314). A two-dimensional structure that combines situational and conceptual knowledge and that distinguishes situational/conceptual knowledge from procedural knowledge, fits the data best (EAP/PV situational/conceptual: 0.63; EAP/PV procedural: 0.67). Student teachers at master level outperformed bachelor level students in situational/conceptual knowledge but master level students did not differ from students at bachelor level regarding procedural knowledge. We observed only slight correlations between the two knowledge dimensions and the content-related motivational orientations of professional action competence. Student teachers’ deficits in procedural knowledge can be attributed to the small number of Education for Sustainable Development-relevant courses attended. Systematically fostering procedural knowledge in teacher education could promote achieving cognitive learning objectives associated with Sustainable Development Goals in the long term.

1. Introduction

Promoting knowledge and competences of Education for Sustainable Development (ESD) enables people to act as informed citizens in creating a more sustainable world [1]. In 2015, the United Nations (UN) adopted 17 Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs) to foster Sustainable Development (SD) according to social, ecological, and economic interests [2]. Among other things, the SDGs aim at ending poverty, reducing inequality, and improving health and education [2]. ESD is one goal among the SDGs (Target 4.7), and it is a key enabler for the remaining 16 goals. The new framework for ESD beyond 2019 highlights the role of education, too [3]. The program covers the period from 2020 to 2030 and concentrates on enhancing “ESD’s contribution to the achievement of all 17 SDGs, focusing on policies, learning environments, teachers and educators, youth as well as communities” [3] (p. 1). Hence, the new framework highlights actions on teacher education as a priority [3].
Schools must face the challenges of society, for example, to enable learners to deal with land use change issues, biodiversity-related issues and climate change issues [4,5,6]. These exemplary Sustainable Development issues build a subset of Socioscientific Issues (SSIs) [7]. SSIs represent controversial and challenging to resolve real-world problems [8], which “tend to have multiple plausible solutions” [9] (p. 4). SSIs have a high social relevance and are related to science [8]. In science education, for instance, such complex issues can be used as contexts to better prepare students to “engage in discourses and decisions related to socially relevant issues associated with science” [9] (p. 4).
Teachers are key persons for changes in classroom practice [10]. To restructure educational processes and institutions toward SD, teachers’ knowledge and competences are crucial [1]. Generally, teachers require scientific knowledge to teach a subject [11]. Teaching Sustainable Development issues is challenging because it involves knowledge of multiple disciplines. In several countries, among them, Germany [12] and Sweden [13], ESD is not an individual subject but the task of several subjects. However, almost every subject can be linked to Sustainable Development issues [12]. One learning objective of teacher education courses is to “know about sustainable development, the different SDGs and the related topics and challenges” [1] (p. 52).
For SSIs, content knowledge is of high relevance because it is related to students’ quality of informal reasoning [14,15]. Thus, “individuals need to have an understanding of an issue in order to render informed decisions” [15] (p. 73f). Also for dealing with Sustainable Development issues in the context of ESD, content knowledge is a necessary skill [16,17,18]. Furthermore, a study in mathematics showed that teachers’ content knowledge is a predictor of students’ achievement [19]. Therefore, knowing about teachers’ prerequisites for dealing with Sustainable Development issues is essential for optimising ESD in teacher education and, thus, in schools. Sustainable Development issues are challenging because many foci like ecological, economic, social, and political must be considered. Knowledge of different disciplines and interdisciplinary knowledge in various fields, such assustainable land use or ecosystem services, is necessary.
Content knowledge as part of professional action competence [20] can be best investigated via Item Response Theory (IRT) models. For example, by analysing performance tests the number of correct answered items is related to a persons’ ability [21]. However, it cannot be assumed that all items of a performance test measure the ability in the same way. Instead, the items of a performance test show usually different difficulties [21]. Hence, it is troublesome to make conclusions about the ability of two persons with the same amount of correct answers without considering the item difficulties [21]. With IRT models, it is possible to consider person ability as well as item difficulty [21]. Thus, “it is possible to compute the probability that a student succeeds on an item” [22]. Therefore, IRT modelling allows one to gain valid insights into learners’ knowledge and competences and is frequently used in current educational research (e.g., [23,24,25]). However, apart from a few exceptions that have used IRT modelling [26,27,28], most studies evaluating knowledge about Sustainable Development issues did not use IRT-modelling for data analysis. This accounts for studies evaluating knowledge of the SDGs [29], climate change [30,31,32], or biodiversity [33]. Furthermore, most of these studies focused on only one type of knowledge [28,33,34], often without declaring which specific type (declarative, procedural, explicit/implicit knowledge).
For dealing with remarkably complex Sustainable Development issues, problem-solving is crucial. According to de Jong and Ferguson-Hessler [35], problem-solving requires situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge. Situational knowledge enables learners to sift relevant information out of a specific problem statement and, if necessary, complete it with additional information outside of the specific problem statement [35]. Conceptual knowledge comprises the principles, concepts and facts that apply within a certain domain [35]. These principles, concepts, and facts to solve problems are not included in the problem statement itself. Hence, the learners have to activate relevant prior knowledge to solve the problem. Procedural knowledge includes actions that are valid for certain types of problems within a domain [35]. Procedural knowledge helps the problem solver to transfer knowledge from one problem situation to another. So far, the knowledge model of de Jong and Ferguson-Hessler [35] was rarely used in education sciences to evaluate learning and teaching prerequisites [36,37]. However, especially regarding Sustainable Development issues like resource use issues in Indonesia [38] or land use issues in Madagascar [39]—where problem-solving is essential–the model turned out to be suitable [38,39,40].
To the best of our knowledge, none of the educational studies applying de Jong and Ferguson-Hessler’s model, e.g., [36,37,38] empirically checked for the independence of single knowledge types using multidimensional IRT modelling. For example, Koch et al. [38] used classical test theory and did not situate type-specific knowledge in relation to similar SD-related constructs. To close this gap, the present study aims to
i.
Assess sustainable land use-relevant knowledge in a differentiated way (situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge);
ii.
Find out via IRT modelling how the theoretically distinguished knowledge types can be empirically supported by the resulting knowledge dimension(s);
iii.
Link these knowledge dimension(s) to related constructs.
Therefore, the study focuses on the following three research questions:
  • Which evidence does IRT modelling provide for the distinction between situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge?
The authors assume that the three theoretical knowledge types represent separate latent constructs. In the last few decades, digitisation has triggered changes in reading habits [41]. Deep forms of reading shifted to more superficial reading practices [41]. Commonly, situational knowledge refers to information given in specific scenarios cf. [38]. If participants have difficulty processing the extensive information, we could also assume that situational and conceptual knowledge build one dimension of knowledge together.
Zwickle et al. [28] showed that sustainability knowledge increases with higher educational level. Thus, we make a similar assumption for the present study. Considering different levels of ESD integration among the subjects of geography, biology, and politics in schools [42], evidence for a similar pattern in teacher education is lacking so far. Student teachers (students at universities aiming to become teachers, in the following interchangeably used with "pre-service teacher”) of programs for different school forms, e.g., for primary or secondary schools, show varying prerequisites, such as interest in science [43]. Hence, an assumption of differing student teachers’ knowledge dependent on school form can be made. Thus, a further research question is:
2.
How do specifications in teacher education, such as academic progress, form of school, and subjects studied influence student teachers’ knowledge of sustainable land use in the knowledge dimension(s)?
In addition to cognitive components, motivational orientations belong to professional action competence; they influence teaching [44] and are essential for problem-solving [45]. Thus, we apply existing instruments to SD-related motivational orientations and set them in relation to our knowledge dimension(s) to check for an assumed divergent validity. The assumption derives from the different nature of the constructs, which are compared (i.e., knowledge and motivational orientations). Furthermore, we check for convergent validity to other knowledge constructs. This led to research question three:
3.
How do the knowledge dimension(s) resulting from IRT modelling differ from SD-related motivational constructs, such as self-efficacy beliefs of ESD teaching, responsibility toward climate change and biodiversity, attitudes toward SD, self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues, and sustainability knowledge?
We expect a higher degree of relatedness between the knowledge dimension(s) of our construct and other knowledge scales compared to scales assessing motivational orientations.

2. Method

2.1. Questionnaire Design

The three knowledge types were operationalised according to de Jong and Ferguson-Hessler [35]. The questionnaire development for SD-relevant knowledge followed Koch et al. [38]. Initially, we chose two contexts relevant to the challenges of biodiversity loss and climate change: “insects and pollination” and “use of peatlands”. Both are linked to the overarching topic of sustainable land use. For each of these contexts, we drafted a scenario based on scientific facts. We published short versions of the scenarios in Richter-Beuschel et al. in 2018 [46].
Based on the scenarios, we constructed multiple-choice items (1 attractor, 3 distractors) to test situational and conceptual knowledge (Figure 1). The items testing situational knowledge referred explicitly to the scenarios. Answering the items testing conceptual knowledge requires factual knowledge in the fields of biodiversity loss and climate change. In sum, we designed 47 items testing situational and conceptual knowledge. Two think-aloud studies served to improve the understanding of items and to adjust distractors that are too good or too bad [47]. An additional quantitative pre-study (N = 109) allowed us to revise or exclude items with extremely high or low difficulty. These steps resulted in a questionnaire with 18 situational and 15 conceptual knowledge items. The 33 items have an ecological, socio-economic, or institutional focus (11 items each).
We developed items to evaluate student teachers’ procedural knowledge through a two-round Delphi approach [46]. Student teachers had to evaluate resulting potential solution strategies regarding “insects and pollination” and “peatland use” for their effectiveness on a four-point Likert scale (1: ineffective to 4: very effective) (Figure 1). The student teachers rated the effectiveness of each solution strategy concerning the following three fields of action: (i) realisation of sustainable land use, (ii) provision of ecosystem services, and (iii) biodiversity conservation and climate protection (in the following abbreviated as “protection”). These are the investigated facets of sustainable land use issues. A combination of qualitative and quantitative steps in the two-rounded Delphi study with an intermediated think-aloud study with student teachers reduced the initial number of 123 items to 51 items [46].
In total, the final paper-and-pencil survey consisted of three main sections:
  • General information (e.g., gender, age, study program, semester, final school examination grade where 1 equals school grade A), ESD specific information (e.g., self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues, number of courses attended including ESD and the percentage of ESD in these courses) and sustainability knowledge (10 items of Zwickle et al. published in 2014 [28]);
  • Written scenarios and multiple-choice items to test situational and conceptual knowledge as well as Likert scale items to test procedural knowledge;
  • Further validation instruments, i.e., self-efficacy beliefs of ESD teaching, attitudes toward sustainable development, responsibility toward climate change and biodiversity, and self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues.

2.2. Validation Instruments

At the beginning of the questionnaire, we asked the participants to self-assess their knowledge. They indicated their knowledge regarding 13 SD-relevant topics which are related to the contexts of “insects and pollination” and “use of peatlands”: diversity of species, diversity of ecosystems, genetic diversity, ecosystem services, climate change, importance of peatlands, Sustainable Development, environmental policy, agricultural policy, bees and pollination, sustainable land use, sustainable consumption, and ESD. We applied a five-point Likert scale with 1: insufficient, 2: sufficient, 3: satisfactory, 4: good, and 5: very good.
Furthermore, we used an instrument to evaluate self-efficacy beliefs of ESD teaching [48]. The instrument comprised 35 four-point Likert scale items: 1: not right, 2: a little right, 3: rather right, and 4: exactly right [48]. Following the confirmatory factor analysis in Lünemann [49], we did not consider one factor with four items out of the eight factors due to correlations > 1 with another factor of the model and due to content dispensability of the removed factor.
A scale used by Biasutti and Frate [50] measured Attitudes toward Sustainable Development (ASD). Their instrument covered the factors of environment, society, economy, and education, and comprised 20 five-point Likert scale items with 1: strongly disagree, 2: disagree, 3: neutral, 4: agree, and 5: strongly agree [50]. To assess responsibility toward climate change, we applied three items suggested by the German Federal Ministry for the Environment, Nature Conservation and Nuclear Safety [51]. Additionally, we adapted them to create three items to assess responsibility toward biodiversity. The resulting items are presented in Figure 2. We used the same scale as for ASD above.

2.3. Data Collection and Sample Description

The cross-sectional study took place between October 2018 and May 2019. The survey was conducted in German. A total of 314 German student teachers from ten universities in seven federal states participated. They studied to become secondary school teachers in at least one of the subjects of biology, geography, or politics. The students were in Bachelor (54.5%), Master (36.6%), or State examination programs (8.3%) and from different semesters. About 80% of the participants were studying to become teachers in German high schools (Gymnasium) and integrated comprehensive schools (hereafter referred to as high schools). Another 20% of the student teachers were studying to become teachers in other school forms, such as vocational school. Most of the participants were female (71.3%; male 26.8%; diverse 1%). The average age of the student teachers was 23.1 years (SD: 3.0, range: 18 to 39).
The survey was conducted onsite at the universities but outside of courses. The mean processing time of the questionnaire was about 60 minutes. Participation was voluntary, anonymity was assured, and students received a financial reward.

2.4. Data Analysis

We applied the dichotomous Rasch model [52] to analyse the dimensionality of situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge using Acer ConQuest 4 [53]. The student teachers’ answers to the multiple-choice items concerning situational and conceptual knowledge were coded as either correct: 1 or incorrect: 0. For procedural knowledge, an averaged expert rating [46] served as an anchor to judge the “right” answer, even where there is factually uncertain knowledge. For dichotomous modelling, the experts’ answers of second Delphi round [46] were rounded to the nearest integer. We coded student teachers’ answers with 1: correct, when they had chosen the same level as the rounded averaged expert ratings, and with 0: incorrect in all other cases.
We applied all analyses case centered. First, we conducted One-Dimensional (1D) modelling of situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge. Items with misfit were excluded from further analysis. A misfit is given when discrimination is <0.20 [54,55] or Weighted Mean Squares (wMNSQ) is <0.8 and >1.2 [56]. After 1D-modelling, 16 items remained for situational, 11 for conceptual, and 32 for procedural knowledge.
Second, the items remaining for the three knowledge types were modelled 1D, Two- (2D), and Three-Dimensionally (3D). Two-dimensional modelling combines situational and conceptual knowledge into one dimension. We used information criteria AIC (Akaike’s Information Criterion) and BIC (Bayesian Information Criterion) as indicators for the model fit and χ2-test to test for significant differences of 1D, 2D, and 3D models. Furthermore, we reported the Expected A-Posteriori reliability/Plausible Values (EAP/PV reliability) and person separation reliabilities based on Weighted Likelihood Estimates (WLE). We checked for potentially biased items regarding gender, study program, and educational level by analysing Differential Item Functioning (DIF).
To answer research question two, the WLE person abilities were used. We first z-standardised WLEs with IBM SPSS Statistics 26. One person built a significant outlier (WLE beyond ± 3.29, [57]); we excluded the outlier from further analyses with person abilities. We applied one-way ANOVAs, t-tests, and linear regression analysis. Due to the homogeneity of variances (Levene Test) in all analyses, we applied Tukey’s HSD (honestly significant difference) post-hoc tests to evaluate which specific groups within an ANOVA differ from each other. Shapiro–Wilk test indicated that WLE person abilities are not normally distributed. Therefore, we used Spearman’s rho (rS) to analyse correlations. In all analyses, the pre-defined significance level is 0.05. To test for the impact of academic progress, participants were divided by their semester into two groups: bachelor level students comprise all participants from first to sixth semester. Master level students comprise all participants in their seventh semester or later. We multiplied the number of ESD-related courses by the percentage of ESD in these courses to create the variable “ESD in university”. The product represents the amount of ESD-relevant content taught in the attended lectures, seminars and other courses.
Concerning research question three, we analysed latent correlations between knowledge dimensions and self-efficacy beliefs, responsibility, attitudes, and self-assessed knowledge using Partial Credit Models [58]. In the first step, we modelled the validation instruments one-dimensional by applying Rating Scale Models [59]. To better characterise the content of the validation constructs of self-efficacy beliefs, responsibility, attitudes, and self-assessed knowledge, we used the reasonably less restricted limit of wMNSQ from 0.7 to 1.3 [56]. Remaining items with misfit were excluded.
The modelling of self-efficacy beliefs started with 31 items; two misfitting items on media use were not considered further. In 26 of the remaining 29 items, less than 5% of participants chose the answer in category one (not right). Thus, by combining categories one and two, the response options were reduced from four to three. For the scales regarding responsibility toward climate change and responsibility toward biodiversity, no misfit occurred. The instrument measuring Attitudes toward SD (ASD) was reduced from 20 to 18 items due to misfit. The response options for both the responsibility and attitudes scale were reduced from five to three options by collapsing categories one, two, and three.
Prior to 1D modelling of self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues, we aggregated the three biodiversity items, diversity of species, diversity of ecosystems, and genetic diversity, into one variable to avoid an overrepresentation of (or bias due to) biodiversity focused items in the scale. We used this new variable alongside the remaining ten variables (11 items in total). We kept the misfitting item “bees and pollination” (wMNSQ = 1.39), due to the high relevance of content.
Regarding sustainability knowledge, we calculated manifest correlations with person abilities in our knowledge dimension(s) because of low information depth of the ten dichotomous items.

3. Results

3.1. Dimensionality of Sustainable Development-Relevant Content Knowledge and Quality of the Instrument

For the 2D and 3D model, AIC and BIC fit nearly equally well (Table 1). The χ2 test for both models displayed significant results (Table 1), and both models are interpretable.
However, looking at the EAP/PV and WLE person separation reliabilities for each dimension of the 2D and 3D model (Table 2), the 2D model is the more reliable solution for situational/conceptual knowledge in the present study. The 2D model combines situational and conceptual knowledge in one dimension.
The latent correlation between situational and conceptual knowledge is highest in the 3D model with 0.76 (Table 3). In the 2D model, the latent correlation of 0.07 between situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge clearly indicates two distinct dimensions (data not shown).
Table 4 displays the test characteristics of the resulting dimensions of situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge. Both dimensions show acceptable EAP/PV reliabilities. The average item difficulty is lower for the situational/conceptual than for the procedural knowledge dimension. The Wright Map (Figure 3) illustrates the item difficulties of situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge items.
Situational/conceptual knowledge items with a socio-economic focus on sustainable land use issues were easiest (mean item difficulty: −1.36), followed by—in average—less difficult items with an institutional (−0.78) and ecological focus (−0.50). However, ANOVA showed no significance (F(2,24) = 2.29, p = 0.123) in average difficulty for the items of the socio-economic, institutional, and ecological discipline. All items developed for situational knowledge showed a mean difficulty of −1.01 and items developed for conceptual knowledge a mean difficulty of −0.72. The t-test revealed no difference in difficulty of situational and conceptual knowledge items (t(10) = −0.59, p = 0.571).
In terms of procedural knowledge, the fields of action on sustainable land use (mean item difficulty: 0.36), ecosystem services (0.36), and protection (0.45) show similar mean item difficulties. ANOVA showed no differences between the average difficulty of the items of the three fields of action (F(2,29) = 0.37, p = 0.692).
The 1D model of situational/conceptual knowledge showed no considerable DIF for gender (average logit difference: 0.06, with females (n = 224) higher than males (n = 84)) (<0.4, [60]), except for item 21 (logit difference: 0.50). For procedural knowledge, none of the items showed considerable DIF for gender (maximum logit difference: 0.30; average logit difference 0.08 higher for females).
Bachelor and master level students showed considerable DIF in item 21 on situational/conceptual dimension (logit difference: 0.71). No considerable DIF occurred in procedural knowledge (maximum logit difference: 0.27). For students who seek to be a high school teacher (n = 250) and students who want to become a teacher in other school forms (n = 63), only item 6 of situational/conceptual knowledge displayed considerable DIF (logit difference: 0.44). In procedural knowledge, only item 19 showed noteworthy DIF (logit difference: 0.61).

3.2. Validation

3.2.1. Influence of Specifications in Teacher Education on Knowledge in Sustainable Land Use Issues

Regarding WLE person abilities, master level students (n = 135) outperformed the bachelor level students (n = 174) in situational/conceptual knowledge (t(307) = −3.96, p < 0.001) but not in procedural knowledge (t(307) = −0.62, p = 0.537) (Figure 4). Master level students scored 0.38 logits higher in situational/conceptual knowledge and only 0.06 logits higher in procedural knowledge than bachelor level students.
Students aiming to become high school teachers (n = 250) displayed higher person ability for situational/conceptual knowledge of sustainable land use issues than students aiming to become teachers in other school forms (n = 62) (t(310) = 3.37, p = 0.001) (Figure 5). For procedural knowledge, no differences in person ability occurred (t(310) = 0.28, p = 0.783). Students seeking to become a high school teacher scored 0.35 logits higher in situational/conceptual knowledge and 0.08 logits higher in procedural knowledge than students who chose other school forms for their studies.
ANOVA did not reveal differences in person ability for different subjects (biology, geography, politics, and a combination of biology and geography) in the dimensions of situational/conceptual (F(4308) = 2.05, p = 0.087) and procedural knowledge (F(4308) = 1.16, p = 0.329). However, final school examination grades were significantly correlated with situational/conceptual (rS = −0.33, p < 0.001) but not with procedural knowledge (rS = −0.03, p = 0.597). In addition, “ESD in university” was significantly correlated with person ability for procedural knowledge (rS = 0.20, p < 0.001) but not with person ability for situational/conceptual knowledge (rS = 0.09, p = 0.118). Linear regressions showed that “ESD in university” predicts SD-relevant procedural knowledge with a small effect (r2 = 0.02, p = 0.016).

3.2.2. Knowledge Dimensions and Related Constructs

In 1D modelling, all validation instruments showed at least moderately acceptable EAP/PV reliabilities. Further test characteristics of 1D models are presented in Table 5.
The highest assumed correlation between knowledge dimensions and self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues can be confirmed only for situational/conceptual knowledge (Table 6). The knowledge dimensions were not (situational/conceptual) or were negatively correlated (procedural) with self-efficacy beliefs of ESD teaching. Furthermore, latent correlations between the knowledge dimensions and responsibility and attitudes were almost negligible. The highest correlations appear between the motivational constructs of self-efficacy beliefs, attitudes, and responsibility (Table 6).
The manifest correlation between the construct of sustainability knowledge and person abilities of the situational/conceptual knowledge dimension is moderate (rS = 0.372, p < 0.001), and no correlation occurs with procedural knowledge (rS = 0.018, p = 0.749).

4. Discussion

4.1. Arguments for a Two-Dimensional Structure of Sustainable Development-Related Knowledge

The study aimed at empirically supporting the structure of the three theoretically distinguished knowledge types of situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge, proposed by de Jong and Ferguson-Hessler [35]. The knowledge types were contextualised for sustainable land use. We tested our assumed 3D model against a 1D and 2D model. The global fit statistics suggest that either 2D- or 3D-modelling is applicable. We chose the 2D model due to its better EAP/PV and WLE person reliabilities for the combined situational/conceptual dimension compared to split dimensions of situational and conceptual knowledge in the 3D model (Table 2). The two resulting dimensions of 2D modelling, i.e., situational/conceptual knowledge and procedural knowledge, are interpretable.
Combining situational and conceptual knowledge in one dimension is plausible: To answer situational knowledge items, student teachers had to read two extensive and complex scenarios cf. [46]. We did not explicitly request the student teachers to read the scenarios carefully before working on the multiple-choice items of situational and conceptual knowledge. They did not know that they had to perform a knowledge test which checks for processing information from the scenarios. Changes in reading habits due to digitisation could have led to more superficial reading practices [41]. This could have affected the participants’ information processing of the scenarios and of the multiple-choice answer options. Thus, student teachers who did not carefully read the information given were perhaps not sufficiently able to integrate information provided in their answers to situational knowledge questions. Therefore, conceptual, rather than situational knowledge, would have been assessed by the multiple-choice items. This is in line with lacking difference in item difficulties between the developed situational and conceptual knowledge items. The phenomenon of working superficially on the multiple-choice options could have occurred for both knowledge types that were assessed by the multiple-choice items.
The latent correlation between situational and conceptual knowledge in the 3D model (Table 3) could have been positively influenced by the use of the same multiple-choice item test format. Furthermore, the use of the same test format could be one reason that—in our modelling—a single dimension for situational/conceptual knowledge was an obvious option considering their EAP/PV and WLE (Table 2). In contrast, Binder et al. [61] used four different methods to measure four types of prior knowledge of biology and physics university students. They interpreted that knowledge of facts, knowledge of meaning, integration of knowledge, and application of knowledge are distinct [61]. However, latent correlations between these knowledge types ranged between 0.24 and 0.86 [61]. Compared with our latent correlations of 0.76 between situational and conceptual knowledge, the 3D theory-driven structure could be chosen if concerns of reliability (WLE values for the 3D model) had not intervened.
Regarding the Wright Maps shown in Figure 3, situational/conceptual knowledge items range from −3.08 to 1.19 logits and procedural knowledge items from −0.14 to 1.00 logits. Figure 3 depicts the item distributions as well. It shows several areas of precise and some areas of less precise measurement. Especially items with low difficulty are rare in the procedural knowledge dimension. This could be an artefact of dichotomising the student teachers’ answers. We handled all student teachers’ answers deviating from the experts’ answers (rounded to the integer) by one unit or more as incorrect (score 0). This loss of differentiated information about the weaker student teachers may have resulted in missing items at the lower end of the Wright Map of procedural knowledge.
Solaz-Portolés and López [62] wrote an overview of knowledge types that are involved in problem-solving regarding science education. The authors stated that while “conceptually it is possible to distinguish knowledge types, in practice they are difficult to distinguish” [62] (p. 106). A reason for this difficulty may be the assessment methods, which were not adequately adjusted to knowledge types [62]. However, the low latent correlation in our study shows a clear distinction between the dimensions of situational/conceptual knowledge and procedural knowledge in the 2D model (0.07)—and even acceptable latent correlations for the theoretically underlying 3D model structure (Table 3). In sum, at least two different types of knowledge can be measured with the proposed instrument.

4.2. Arguments for the Suitability of the Measurement Instrument

Altogether, no Differential Item Functioning (DIF) considerably impaired the measurement of the situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge dimensions. When the participants were grouped by gender, only item 21 of the situational/conceptual dimension displayed DIF. This can be explained by the extremely low difficulty of item 21 (Figure 3) and therefore, the comparably imprecise measurement. For the groups of bachelor and master level students, DIF occurred again only in item 21 of the situational/conceptual dimension.
When students were grouped by the different school forms they wished to work in, DIF existed for item 19 in the procedural dimension—explainable by its content. For the situational/conceptual dimension, the DIF displayed in item 6 can be traced back to the same reasons as item 21 (imprecise estimation).
The DIF of items 6 and 21 in situational/conceptual dimension and item 19 for procedural knowledge can, additionally, be plausibly explained by the content they covered. Items 6 and 21 cover the genetic diversity of honeybees. In item 6, genetic diversity functions as a force for vulnerability toward diseases and parasites, and in item 21, genetic diversity influences the threat of extinction in general. Therefore, the DIF in item 21 indicates that bachelor level students have difficulty in understanding biodiversity at the level of genes. This is in line with the findings of Jiwa and Esa [33]. They report that definitions of the term biodiversity of student teachers lack explicitly stated aspects of genetic diversity. Additionally, Fiebelkorn and Menzel [63] showed that German biology student teachers were not sufficiently conscious of the genetic level of biodiversity. The same explanation can be applied to the DIF of item 6, resulting of the subsample compositions: the subsample of student teachers who wish to teach in high school is balanced (bachelor level: 49.6%, master level: 49.2%), whereas the subsample of student teachers for other school formss is somewhat unbalanced (bachelor level: 81%, master level: 19%).
Item 19 in the procedural knowledge dimension requested participants to evaluate the effectiveness of intensifying the investigation of regenerative peat substitutes, regarding their contribution to climate protection. High school student teachers showed higher ability to answer this item than student teachers for other school forms. High school teachers are trained more specifically in the sciences than teachers for other school forms, and they have the task to qualify their students in a research-oriented way to prepare them for university studies [64]. Additionally, the higher ability of high school student teachers to answer item 19 may be aligned with their generally higher person ability. High school and integrated comprehensive school student teachers show higher professional and scientific interest than student teachers for other school types [43]. Persons with lower final school examination grades prefer to choose school forms other than high school and integrated comprehensive school [43,65].
As shown above, the developed instrument is suitable for the following groups of student teachers: (i) females and males, (ii) bachelor and master level students, and (iii) students aiming to become high school teachers and teachers at other school forms.

4.3. Arguments for Validity

On average, items measuring procedural knowledge were more difficult than items measuring situational/conceptual knowledge. This is explainable due to the interdisciplinary knowledge needed to solve procedural knowledge items. The higher abilities of master level students to answer situational/conceptual items, compared to bachelor level students (Figure 4), could be partly explained by their knowledge acquired during teacher education. Another reason to consider is that a small number of student teachers at the master level may have changed into a Master of Education program after their bachelor’s degrees in science. Probably, more intensive studies of conceptual knowledge in the science bachelor studies than in the bachelor qualifying for becoming a teacher might create a slight bias in the results of master level student teachers. The higher situational/conceptual knowledge of master than bachelor level students is in line with Koch et al. [38]. They reported no difference in situational knowledge of Indonesian university students from third to seventh semester, but in two of three domains in conceptual knowledge [38]. In addition, Zwickle et al. [28], who measured sustainability knowledge—which is comparable with knowledge defined as conceptual in the present research—found higher levels of sustainability knowledge in upperclassmen than in underclassmen. The lack of difference in person ability between bachelor and master level students for the dimension of procedural knowledge in our study (Figure 4) is also in line with Koch et al. [38]. They detected that third- and seventh-semester Indonesian university students both showed low procedural knowledge, with high deviations from expert judgments of solutions’ effectiveness [38]. The missing correlation between final school examination grades and procedural knowledge in our study is also understandable: up to now, solution strategies for interdisciplinary problems are rarely part of upper secondary education, with its subject-specific curricula and examinations [64].
Despite the higher person ability of master level students in situational/conceptual knowledge, the results of the present study revealed no correlation between “ESD in university” and situational/conceptual knowledge. The missing correlation matches German teacher education curricular requirements of studying pedagogical content knowledge regarding two school subjects and studying corresponding natural or social sciences. Natural or social science courses probably foster situational/conceptual knowledge, but maybe they are not represented in the “ESD in university” indicator. Furthermore, other sources of knowledge can be considered: for example, participants who indicated having dealt with ESD through “private research in the internet” showed significantly higher abilities in situational/conceptual knowledge (rS = 0.13, p = 0.028). This finding is in line with Burmeister and Eilks [66], who report that German chemistry pre-service teachers gained their knowledge of sustainability and ESD from media and the internet instead of their formal education. In contrast to situational/conceptual knowledge, for the procedural knowledge dimension, higher “ESD in university” was accompanied by higher person abilities. This leads to the assumption that a higher proportion of ESD in teacher education courses may foster procedural knowledge. Thus, interdisciplinary knowledge could equip student teachers with the necessary procedural knowledge (e.g., for making land use more sustainable), and enables the teaching of such knowledge to students—the next generation of decision-makers.
Our results do not contradict the results of a prior study with only a subsample of student teachers, which revealed no correlation between the number of attended ESD-relevant courses and procedural knowledge [40]. This result was attributed to the extremely low number of courses with ESD relevance [40]. In the prior study, no correction by the percentage of ESD in the courses has been applied. However, without this correction, a significant but weak correlation between procedural knowledge and the amount of ESD taught is revealed (rS = 0.19, p = 0.001). Furthermore, the differentiated, more diverse sample composition of the present study—comprising not only students becoming a teacher at high schools and integrated comprehensive schools as in Richter-Beuschel and Bögeholz [40]—may contribute to these differing results. Two further reasons—perhaps most important—are: first, the IRT analyses of the present study consider only 32 of the 51 items used in the prior study with analyses only considering classical test theory. Second, the present study analysed procedural knowledge in a dichotomous format (rounded to integer), whereas the prior study used the precise deviation between experts’ and students’ effectiveness estimations to build the specific value (decimal without rounding) for procedural knowledge [40]. Thus, different information was processed in the final analyses of the present and prior study. Overall, considering the more restrictive prerequisites for data analyses and the more sophisticated requirements connected with IRT modelling, the results of the present study are more valid.
In the following, we give first insights into the distribution of the items on the Wright Maps. Item 5 measuring situational/conceptual knowledge (Figure 1) was one of the most difficult items (Figure 3). The distractor “cities … offer little food for wild bees” was derived from a common misconception and was chosen by many participants—indeed, urban areas provide important resources for insects [67]. Item 26 (Figure 1) revealed a low difficulty level (Figure 3). Through formulations like “always” or “only” in distractors, the item was already easy because of its structure, apart from its content. Within the dimension of procedural knowledge, item 10 (Figure 1), for example, was relatively easy to answer (Figure 3). This seems plausible because the solution strategy, “Support pollinator-friendly agriculture by purchasing ecologically produced products”, is an omnipresent option to take if one wants to. This strategy does not include any visible barrier and does not require any personal action. In contrast, item 25 (Figure 1) was of highest difficulty (Figure 3). The realisation of “Cultivate peatlands without fertilisers and pesticides” depends on diverse external conditions. For example, a change of personal routines and common working behaviours of relevant stakeholders influences income depending on sensitive and complex cultivation and farm management. Such a complex situation is hard to estimate for student teachers, and even for other non-experts without the corresponding background.
The present study used multidimensional IRT modelling to investigate divergent validity of knowledge dimensions and motivational factors and to locate situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge in the nomological net of professional action competence. In sum, both, the situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge dimensions, showed little to no correlation with the constructs of self-efficacy beliefs of ESD teaching, responsibility toward climate change and biodiversity, and attitudes toward sustainable development. Due to findings in science education [68], one might think that content knowledge is related to self-efficacy beliefs. In our study on dimensions of situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge contextualised for sustainable land use, this is not the case. This could be explained by the subject matter addressed by self-efficacy beliefs of ESD teaching. The content of the construct content knowledge about sustainable land use cannot be equated with the content of the perceived competences in ESD teaching.
Neuhaus and Vogt [69] identified three types of German biology student teachers: the pedagogical-innovative, the scientific-innovative, and the scientific-conventional teacher types. In the present study, due to voluntary participation outside of courses, we cannot exclude an overrepresentation of the two scientific-oriented types. The participants’ scientific focus may have led to lower self-efficacy beliefs of ESD teaching and, thus, to missing or negative correlations with the dimension of situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge.
The absent or slight correlations between situational/conceptual knowledge and responsibility or attitudes, as well as between procedural knowledge and responsibility or attitudes, are in line with previous research. Pre-service teachers in chemistry showed to have a positive attitude toward ESD despite lacking theory-based knowledge about sustainability and ESD [66]. Similarly, Kagawa [70] reported that Plymouth University students’ knowledge about concepts of sustainability and SD and their attitude were not correlated. In addition, pre-service teachers’ knowledge of the impact of mitigative climate actions has been shown not to correlate with their willingness to undertake these actions [71]. The authors attributed the lacking correlation to the insufficient knowledge of the participants [71]. Our missing correlations between situational/conceptual, procedural knowledge and motivational orientations may further result from measuring responsibility and attitudes more general instead of specific to the used contexts. According to Ajzen and Fishbein (1980, in [72]), satisfactory correlations between behaviour and attitude require the measurement of attitudes toward a specific action. Probably, the requirement of specificity also accounts for the operationalisation of knowledge and attitudes.
For our self-assessed knowledge scale, the specificity was given, and the highest correlation between situational/conceptual knowledge and self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues (Table 6) compared to other validation constructs occurred. However, despite the same construct being investigated (knowledge of specific land use context), the results show low correlation (0.26) between situational/conceptual knowledge and self-assessed knowledge. The reason may be that self-reported knowledge cannot be equated with performance in situational/conceptual knowledge dimension. The lack of correlation between self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues and procedural knowledge may be attributed to the characteristics of the knowledge types: in answering the self-assessed knowledge scale, knowledge about facts and principles and, thus, situational/conceptual knowledge will be considered rather than unsolved, uncertain solution strategies for Sustainable Development issues of procedural knowledge. The highest correlations between self-efficacy beliefs, responsibility, and attitudes (Table 6) are plausible, because they all assess motivational orientations instead of knowledge.

5. Conclusions

This research presents an instrument for assessing disciplinary and interdisciplinary knowledge of student teachers about sustainable land use issues. We operationalised situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge types according to de Jong and Ferguson-Hessler [35]. Their knowledge model forms a reasonable basis for evaluating knowledge in Sustainable Development issues because it focuses on problem-solving. One strength of the presented study lies in the application of IRT analysis, which considers person abilities as well as item difficulties. Differentiated knowledge assessments, including an empirical support of theoretically underlying knowledge types, were rare in educational research so far. Thus, we aimed to obtain empirical support for a 3D model that reflects the three theory-driven types of situational, conceptual and procedural knowledge. Following the EAP/PV and WLE person reliabilities, in the current state, we consider situational and conceptual knowledge together as one dimension. Furthermore, the study provides first insights into the situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge of student teachers. The results show that it is difficult for prospective teachers to deal with global challenges, which have no simple or unambiguous solution. This could be one reason for an inadequate implementation of ESD in German school education cf. [73].
The measurement instrument was constructed using three different foci of situational and conceptual knowledge (ecological, socio-economic, and institutional) and different fields of action in procedural knowledge (sustainable land use, ecosystem services, and biodiversity conservation/climate protection). The similarity of item difficulty between the SD-relevant foci and the fields of action shows that operationalising an instrument with comparable difficulty levels, covering multiple disciplinary and interdisciplinary knowledge and SD-associated objectives, was successful.
By interpreting the results of the study, it must be considered that we conducted a one-time survey. For conclusions about knowledge acquisition during teacher education, it would be much more adequate to conduct a longitudinal study. When applying the questionnaire in future, we will examine dimensionality more closely. We will use the full item set again and request the participants to read the scenarios very carefully before working on the questionnaire, as the scenarios’ contents are essential for the following knowledge questions. In this way, it may be possible to prove whether reading habits and information processing influence the lacking differentiation between situational and conceptual knowledge.
To analyse student teachers’ procedural knowledge in a more differentiated way than dichotomous evaluation, we will use student teachers’ deviation from the rounded averaged expert answers. Thus, we will not equate one-unit deviation and more together as incorrect (score 0); we will consider partial credit reflecting the factual units of deviation to the rounded averaged expert answers to the next integer. With these changes, we assume to produce even better and more sophisticated modelling, through more precise measurement and better coverage of the low difficulty area of procedural knowledge.
The developed instrument shows several strengths: One strength is the broad content coverage of SD-related knowledge. This includes SD-related foci and SD-related fields of action concerning highly relevant Sustainable Development issues regarding the challenges of biodiversity loss and climate change. A second strength of the instrument is the evaluation of disciplinary (situational/conceptual) knowledge and interdisciplinary (procedural) knowledge. A third strength is the broad coverage of knowledge types or rather empirically supported dimensions relevant to solve SD-related problems regarding sustainable land use issues. All strengths together, the content coverage, the disciplinary and interdisciplinary knowledge considered, and the problem-solving relevant knowledge dimensions, clearly address important prerequisites for teaching according to the objectives of education for SDGs [1]. As ESD is one important task of teacher education, we can conclude the following from our study: Student teachers attended only a small number of ESD-relevant courses during their university studies. The small effect of ESD-relevant courses on procedural knowledge in sustainable land use issues gives a first hint that procedural knowledge can be fostered during teacher education. However, enhancing procedural knowledge in the context of ESD to solve Sustainable Development issues remains expandable. Up to now, the disciplinary knowledge is fostered by German teacher education [64]. Therefore, further developments should especially consider interdisciplinary ESD teaching in teacher education—integrated in the subject-specific courses and in additional qualifications, (e.g., [74]).
In addition to skills, attitudes, values, motivation, and commitment, knowledge is one of the key factors to improve SD-related competences worldwide [1]. Qualified teachers could promote ESD at school. Therefore, equipping teachers as change agents with situational/conceptual and procedural knowledge in SD-related challenges of biodiversity and climate change, could be a decisive contribution toward sustainable land use and achieving core SDGs.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, L.R.-B. and S.B.; formal analysis, L.R.-B. and S.B.; funding acquisition, S.B.; methodology, L.R.-B. and S.B.; project administration, S.B.; supervision, S.B.; writing—original draft, L.R.-B.; writing—review and editing, S.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project is part of the “Qualitätsoffensive Lehrerbildung”, a joint initiative of the Federal Government and the Länder, which aims to improve the quality of teacher training. The program is funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (reference number: 01JA1617). The authors are responsible for the content of this publication. The Article Processing Charge (APC) was funded by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Goettingen.

Acknowledgments

We thank Tobias C. Stubbe for statistical advice.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. UNESCO (United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization). Education for Sustainable Development Goals. Learning Objectives; UNESCO: Paris, France, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  2. United Nations (UN), General Assembly. Transforming Our World: The 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development; United Nations (UN), General Assembly: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  3. UNESCO (United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization). Framework for the Implementation of Education for Sustainable Development (ESD) beyond 2019; UNESCO: Paris, France, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  4. McREL. Topic: Environmental Issues. Available online: http://www2.mcrel.org/compendium/topicsDetail.asp?topicsID=69&subjectID=2 (accessed on 20 August 2020).
  5. NGSS Lead States. Next Generation Science Standards: For States, by States; The National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. OECD. The Future of Education and Skills: Education 2030; OECD: Paris, France, 2018. Available online: https://www.oecd.org/education/2030/E2030%20Position%20Paper%20(05.04.2018).pdf (accessed on 20 August 2020).
  7. Böhm, M.; Barkmann, J.; Eggert, S.; Carstensen, C.H.; Bögeholz, S. Quantitative Modelling and Perspective Taking: Two Competencies of Decision Making for Sustainable Development. Sustainability 2020, 12, 6980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sadler, T.D.; Barab, S.A.; Scott, B. What do students gain by engaging in socioscientific inquiry? Res. Sci. Educ. 2007, 37, 371–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Sadler, T.D. Situating Socio-scientific Issues in Classrooms as a Means of Achieving Goals in Science Education. In Socio-Scientific Issues in the Classroom: Teaching, Learning and Research; Sadler, T.D., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2011; pp. 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  10. Anderson, R.D.; Helms, J.V. The ideal of standards and the reality of schools: Needed research. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2001, 38, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Liakopoulou, M. The Professional Competence of Teachers: Which qualities, attitudes, skills and knowledge contribute to a teacher’s effectiveness? Int. J. Hum. Soc. Sci. 2011, 1, 66–78. [Google Scholar]
  12. Schreiber, J.-R. Competencies, Themes, Standards, Design of Lessons and Curricula. In Curriculum Framework: Education for Sustainable Development, 2nd ed.; Schreiber, J.-R., Siege, H., Eds.; Engagement Global gGmbH: Bonn, Germany, 2016; pp. 86–110. ISBN 978-3-06-230062-2. [Google Scholar]
  13. Sund, P.; Gericke, N. Teaching contributions from secondary school subject areas to education for sustainable development—A comparative study of science, social science and language teachers. Environ. Educ. Res. 2020, 26, 772–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Sadler, T.D.; Fowler, S.R. A Threshold Model of Content Knowledge Transfer for Socioscientific Argumentation. Sci. Educ. 2006, 90, 986–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Sadler, T.D.; Zeidler, D.L. The Significance of Content Knowledge for Informal Reasoning Regarding Socioscientific Issues: Applying Genetics Knowledge to Genetic Engineering Issues. Sci. Educ. 2005, 89, 71–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bertschy, F.; Künzli, C.; Lehmann, M. Teachers’ Competencies for the Implementation of Educational Offers in the Field of Education for Sustainable Development. Sustainability 2013, 5, 5067–5080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Hellberg-Rode, G.; Schrüfer, G. Welche spezifischen professionellen Handlungskompetenzen benötigen Lehrkräfte für die Umsetzung von Bildung für Nachhaltige Entwicklung (BNE)? Ergebnisse einer explorativen Studie. Z. Didakt. Biol. (ZDB) 2016, 20, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Sleurs, W. Competencies for ESD (Education for Sustainable Development) Teachers. A framework to Integrate ESD in the Curriculum of Teacher Training Institutes. Brussels, 2008. Available online: https://www.unece.org/fileadmin/DAM/env/esd/inf.meeting.docs/EGonInd/8mtg/CSCT%20Handbook_Extract.pdf (accessed on 21 August 2020).
  19. Hill, H.C.; Rowan, B.; Loewenberg Ball, D. Effects of Teachers’ Mathematical Knowledge for Teaching on Student Achievement. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2005, 42, 371–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Baumert, J.; Kunter, M. Stichwort: Professionelle Kompetenz von Lehrkräften. Z. Erzieh. 2006, 9, 469–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Neuman, K. Rasch-Analyse naturwissenschaftsbezogener Leistungstest. In Methoden in der Naturwissenschaftsdidaktischen Forschung; Krüger, D., Parchmann, I., Schecker, H., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2014; pp. 355–369. ISBN 978-3-642-37826-3. [Google Scholar]
  22. OECD. PISA Data Analysis Manual: SAS, 2nd ed.; OECD Publishing: Paris, France, 2009; ISBN 978-92-64-05624-4. [Google Scholar]
  23. OECD. PISA 2015 Technical Report; OECD Publishing: Paris, France, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  24. Camilli, G.; Dossey, J.A. IRT scoring procedures for TIMSS data. MethodsX 2019, 6, 1506–1511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA). Methods and Procedures in Pirls; TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Lynch School of Education, Boston College and International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA): Chestnut Hill, PA, USA, 2017; ISBN 978-1-889938-44-8. [Google Scholar]
  26. Frick, J.; Kaiser, F.G.; Wilson, M. Environmental knowledge and conservation behavior: Exploring prevalence and structure in a representative sample. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2004, 37, 1597–1613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kaiser, F.G.; Frick, J. Development of an environmental knowledge measure: An application of the MRCML model. Diagnostica 2002, 48, 181–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Zwickle, A.; Koontz, T.M.; Slagle, K.M.; Bruskotter, J.T. Assessing sustainability knowledge of a student population. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 2014, 15, 375–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Zamora-Polo, F.; Sánchez-Martín, J.; Corrales-Serrano, M.; Espejo-Antúnez, L. What Do University Students Know about Sustainable Development Goals? A Realistic Approach to the Reception of this UN Program Amongst the Youth Population. Sustainability 2019, 11, 3533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Herman, B.C.; Feldman, A.; Vernaza-Hernandez, V. Florida and Puerto Rico Secondary Science Teachers’ Knowledge and Teaching of Climate Change Science. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2017, 15, 451–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Boon, H. Pre-Service Teachers and Climate Change: A Stalemate? Aust. J. Teach. Educ. 2016, 41, 39–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Plutzer, E.; McCaffrey, M.; Hannah, A.L.; Rosenau, J.; Berbeco, M.; Reid, A.H. Climate Confusion among U.S. Teachers. Science 2016, 351, 664–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Jiwa, R.A.M.; Esa, N. Student Teachers’ Knowledge of Biodiversity. Int. J. Sci. Res. Publ. 2015, 5, 193–196. [Google Scholar]
  34. Heeren, A.J.; Singh, A.; Zwickle, A.; Koontz, T.M.; Slagle, K.M.; McCreery, A.C. Is sustainability knowledge half the battle? Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 2016, 17, 613–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. De Jong, T.; Ferguson-Hessler, M. Types and Qualities of Knowledge. Educ. Psychol. 1996, 31, 105–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Wagner, K.; Bergner, M.; Krause, U.-M.; Stark, R. Förderung wissenschaftlichen Denkens im Lehramtsstudium: Lernen aus eigenen und fremden Fehlern in multiplen und uniformen Kontexten. Z. Pädagog. Psychol. 2018, 32, 5–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Klein, M.; Wagner, K.; Klopp, E.; Stark, R. Fostering of Applicable Educational Knowledge in Student Teachers: Effects of an Error-based Seminar Concept and Instructional Support during Testing on Qualities of Applicable Knowledge. J. Educ. Res. Online 2017, 9, 88–114. [Google Scholar]
  38. Koch, S.; Barkmann, J.; Strack, M.; Sundawati, L.; Bögeholz, S. Knowledge of Indonesian University Students on the Sustainable Management of Natural Resources. Sustainability 2013, 5, 1443–1460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Niens, J.; Richter-Beuschel, L.; Bögeholz, S. Land-Use and Health Issues in Malagasy Primary Education—A Delphi Study. Sustainability 2020, 12, 6212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Richter-Beuschel, L.; Bögeholz, S. Student Teachers’ Knowledge to Enable Problem-Solving for Sustainable Development. Sustainability 2020, 12, 79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Mangen, A.; van der Weel, A. The evolution of reading in the age of digitisation: An integrative framework for reading research. Literacy 2016, 50, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bagoly-Simó, P.; Hemmer, I. Bildung für Nachhaltige Entwicklung in den Sekundarschulen: Ziele, Einblicke in Die Realität, Perspektiven. 2017. Available online: https://www.ku.de/fileadmin/150305/Professur_fuer_Didaktik_der_Geographie/Forschung/Literatur/Bildung_f%c3%bcr_nachhaltige_Entwicklung_in_den_Sekundarschulen_%e2%80%93_Ziele__Einblicke_in_die_Realit%c3%a4t__Perspektiven_-_Bagoly-Simo___Hemmer.pdf (accessed on 20 September 2019).
  43. Retelsdorf, J.; Möller, J. Grundschule oder Gymnasium? Zur Motivation ein Lehramt zu studieren. Z. Pädagog. Psychol. 2012, 26, 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Weinert, F.E. Concept of Competence: A Conceptual Clarification. In Defining and Selecting Key Competencies; Rychen, D.S., Salganik, L.H., Eds.; Horgrefe & Huber: Seattle, WA, USA, 2001; pp. 45–65. [Google Scholar]
  45. Mayer, R.E. Cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational aspects of problem solving. Instr. Sci. 1998, 26, 49–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Richter-Beuschel, L.; Grass, I.; Bögeholz, S. How to Measure Procedural Knowledge for Solving Biodiversity and Climate Change Challenges. Educ. Sci. 2018, 8, 190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Richter-Beuschel, L.; Derksen, C.; Bögeholz, S. Konzeptuelles Wissen angehender Lehrkräfte für Bildung für Nachhaltige Entwicklung. In BfN-Skripten: Treffpunkt Biologische Vielfalt XVI. Interdisziplinärer Forschungsaustausch im Rahmen des Übereinkommens über die Biologische Vielfalt; Korn, H., Dünnfelder, H., Schliep, R., Eds.; Bundesamt für Naturschutz: Bonn-Bad Godesberg, Germany, 2018; Volume 487, pp. 88–95. [Google Scholar]
  48. Rosenbaum, S.; Handtke, K.; Richter-Beuschel, L.; Grieger, M.; Bögeholz, S. Fragebogen zur Befähigung für ein Unterrichten von BNE; University of Goettingen: Goettingen, Germany, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lünemann, I.K. Selbstwirksamkeitserwartungen von Lehramtsstudierenden der Fächer Biologie, Erdkunde und Politik zum Unterrichten von Bildung für Nachhaltige Entwicklung. Master’s Thesis, Georg-August University, Goettingen, Germany, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  50. Biasutti, M.; Frate, S. A Validity and Reliability Study of the Attitudes toward Sustainable Development scale. Environ. Educ. Res. 2017, 23, 214–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Federal Ministry for the Environment, Nature Conservation and Nuclear Safety. Umweltbewusstsein in Deutschland 2008: Ergebnisse einer Repräsentativen Bevölkerungsumfrage; BMU: Berlin, Germany, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  52. Rasch, G. Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1960. [Google Scholar]
  53. Wu, M.L.; Adams, R.J.; Wilson, M.R.; Haldane, S.A. Acer ConQuest Version 2.0: Generalised Item Response Modelling Software; Acer: Camberwell, VIC, Australia, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  54. Ebel, R.L.; Frisbie, D.A. Essentials of Educational Measurement; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1986; ISBN 978-0132860062. [Google Scholar]
  55. Rush, B.R.; Rankin, D.C.; White, B.J. The impact of item-writing flaws and item complexity on examination item difficulty and discrimination value. BMC Med. Educ. 2016, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Boone, W.J.; Staver, J.R.; Yale, M.S. Rasch Analysis in the Human Sciences; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014; ISBN 978-94-007-6857-4. [Google Scholar]
  57. Field, A.P.; Miles, J.; Field, Z. Discovering Statistics Using R; Sage Publ.: London, UK, 2014; ISBN 978-1446200469. [Google Scholar]
  58. Masters, G. A Rasch model for partial credit scoring. Psychometrika 1982, 47, 149–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Andrich, D. A rating formulation for ordered response categories. Psychometrika 1978, 43, 561–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Pohl, S.; Carstensen, C.H. NEPS Technical Report—Scaling the Data of the Competence Tests (NEPS Working Paper No. 14). Bamberg: Otto-Friedrich-Universität, Nationales Bildungspanel. [National Educational Panel Study]. 2012. Available online: https://www.neps-data.de/Portals/0/Working%20Papers/WP_XIV.pdf (accessed on 28 April 2020).
  61. Binder, T.; Schmiemann, P.; Theyssen, H. Knowledge Acquisition of Biology and Physics University Students—The Role of Prior Knowledge. Educ. Sci. 2019, 9, 281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Solaz-Portolés, J.J.; Sanjosé López, V. Types of knowledge and their relations to problem solving in science: Directions for practice. Educ. Sci. J. 2008, 6, 105–112. [Google Scholar]
  63. Fiebelkorn, F.; Menzel, S. Student Teachers’ Understanding of the Terminology, Distribution, and Loss of Biodiversity: Perspectives from a Biodiversity Hotspot and an Industrialized Country. Res. Sci. Educ. 2013, 43, 1593–1615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. KMK (Kultusministerkonferenz der Länder). Ländergemeinsame Inhaltliche Anforderungen für die Fachwissenschaften und Fachdidaktiken in der Lehrerbildung. Beschluss der Kultusministerkonferenz vom 16.10.2008 i. d. F. vom 16.05.2019; KMK: Hannover, Germany, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  65. Neugebauer, M. Wer entscheidet sich für ein Lehramtsstudium–und warum? Eine empirische Überprüfung der These von der Negativselektion in den Lehrerberuf. Z. Erzieh. 2013, 16, 157–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Burmeister, M.; Eilks, I. An understanding of sustainability and education for sustainable development among German student teachers and trainee teachers of chemistry. Sci. Educ. Int. 2013, 24, 167–194. [Google Scholar]
  67. European Academies’ Science Advisory Council (EASAC). Ecosystem Services, Agriculture and Neonicotinoids; EASAC policy report 26; EASAC Secretariat Deutsche Akademie der Naturforscher Leopoldina: Halle, Germany, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  68. Schoon, K.J.; Boone, W.J. Self-Efficacy and Alternative Conceptions of Science of Preservice Elementary Teachers. Sci. Educ. 1998, 82, 553–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Neuhaus, B.; Vogt, H. Dimensionen zur Beschreibung verschiedener Biologielehrertypen auf Grundlage ihrer Einstellung zum Biologieunterricht. Z. Didakt. Nat. 2005, 11, 73–84. [Google Scholar]
  70. Kagawa, F. Dissonance in students’ perceptions of sustainable development and sustainability. Implications for curriculum change. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 2007, 8, 317–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Tolppanen, S.; Claudelin, A.; Kang, J. Pre-service Teachers’ Knowledge and Perceptions of the Impact of Mitigative Climate Actions and Their Willingness to Act. Res. Sci. Educ. 2020, 7, 233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Mairesse, O.; Macharis, C.; Lebeau, K.; Turcksin, L. Understanding the attitude-action gap: Functional integration of environmental aspects in car purchase intentions. Psicológica 2012, 33, 547–574. [Google Scholar]
  73. Singer-Brodowski, M.; Etzkorn, N.; von Seggern, J. One Transformation Path Does Not Fit All—Insights into the Diffusion Processes of Education for Sustainable Development in Different Educational Areas in Germany. Sustainability 2019, 11, 269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Eggert, S.; Bögeholz, S.; Oberle, M.; Sauer, M.; Schneider, S.; Surkamp, C. Herausforderung Interdisziplinäres Unterrichten in der Lehrerbildung—Das Göttinger Zertifikatsmodell. J. Lehr. 2018, 18, 51–55. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Exemplary items for assessing situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge (correct answers of situational and conceptual knowledge in italics; mean experts’ estimations rounded to the integer as benchmark for the evaluation of student teachers’ procedural knowledge are marked by a circle).
Figure 1. Exemplary items for assessing situational, conceptual, and procedural knowledge (correct answers of situational and conceptual knowledge in italics; mean experts’ estimations rounded to the integer as benchmark for the evaluation of student teachers’ procedural knowledge are marked by a circle).
Sustainability 12 08332 g001
Figure 2. Items for assessing responsibility toward biodiversity.
Figure 2. Items for assessing responsibility toward biodiversity.
Sustainability 12 08332 g002
Figure 3. Wright Maps of Situational/Conceptual Knowledge and Procedural Knowledge (each ’X’ represents 1.1 cases) ( Sustainability 12 08332 i001: item difficulty is lower than presented) (N = 314).
Figure 3. Wright Maps of Situational/Conceptual Knowledge and Procedural Knowledge (each ’X’ represents 1.1 cases) ( Sustainability 12 08332 i001: item difficulty is lower than presented) (N = 314).
Sustainability 12 08332 g003
Figure 4. Person abilities in Situational/Conceptual Knowledge (SCK) and Procedural Knowledge (PK) from bachelor and master level students (***: p < 0.001, ns = not significant) (n = 309). Error bars indicate standard errors.
Figure 4. Person abilities in Situational/Conceptual Knowledge (SCK) and Procedural Knowledge (PK) from bachelor and master level students (***: p < 0.001, ns = not significant) (n = 309). Error bars indicate standard errors.
Sustainability 12 08332 g004
Figure 5. Person abilities in Situational/Conceptual Knowledge (SCK) and Procedural Knowledge (PK) from student teachers of high school and other school forms (***: p = 0.001, ns = not significant) (n = 312). Error bars indicate standard errors.
Figure 5. Person abilities in Situational/Conceptual Knowledge (SCK) and Procedural Knowledge (PK) from student teachers of high school and other school forms (***: p = 0.001, ns = not significant) (n = 312). Error bars indicate standard errors.
Sustainability 12 08332 g005
Table 1. Comparison of fit statistics between One-, Two-, and Three-Dimensional (1D, 2D, 3D) modelling of knowledge types with Item Response Theory (IRT) (N = 314).
Table 1. Comparison of fit statistics between One-, Two-, and Three-Dimensional (1D, 2D, 3D) modelling of knowledge types with Item Response Theory (IRT) (N = 314).
ModelDevianceNumber of ParametersChange in DevianceChange in dfpAICBIC
1D22,493.1360 22,61322,838.09
2D22,281.05622122<0.00122,40522,637.51
3D22,273.716583<0.0522,40322,647.42
Table 2. EAP/PV and WLE person separation reliabilities for each knowledge dimension of Two- and Three-Dimensional (2D, 3D) modelling (N = 314).
Table 2. EAP/PV and WLE person separation reliabilities for each knowledge dimension of Two- and Three-Dimensional (2D, 3D) modelling (N = 314).
2D Model3D Model
Situational/Conceptual KnowledgeProcedural
Knowledge
Situational
Knowledge
Conceptual KnowledgeProcedural Knowledge
EAP/PV0.630.680.600.560.66
WLE0.600.670.470.390.67
Table 3. Latent correlations between the situational, conceptual and procedural knowledge of Three-Dimensional (3D) modelling.
Table 3. Latent correlations between the situational, conceptual and procedural knowledge of Three-Dimensional (3D) modelling.
Situational KnowledgeConceptual Knowledge
Situational Knowledge
Conceptual Knowledge0.76
Procedural Knowledge0.060.08
Table 4. Test characteristics for the dimensions of Situational/Conceptual Knowledge (SCK) and Procedural Knowledge (PK) (N = 314) (SE: Standard Error; wMNSQ: weighted (infit) mean-square).
Table 4. Test characteristics for the dimensions of Situational/Conceptual Knowledge (SCK) and Procedural Knowledge (PK) (N = 314) (SE: Standard Error; wMNSQ: weighted (infit) mean-square).
SCK
(27 Items)
PK
(32 Items)
Item Separation Reliability0.980.83
Variance (SE)0.35 (0.05)0.29 (0.04)
EAP/PV Reliability0.630.67
WLE Person Separation Reliability0.600.67
Average Item Difficulty (SE)−0.89 (0.05)0.45 (0.02)
Item Difficulty range−3.08–1.19−0.14–1.00
wMNSQ range0.96–1.050.96–1.04
Discrimination range0.22–0.420.22–0.38
Table 5. Test characteristics for the validation instruments of one-dimensional rating scale models (SD: Sustainable Development; ESD: Education for Sustainable Development; SE: Standard Error) (N = 314).
Table 5. Test characteristics for the validation instruments of one-dimensional rating scale models (SD: Sustainable Development; ESD: Education for Sustainable Development; SE: Standard Error) (N = 314).
Validation Instrument
(Number of Items per Scale; Number of Scoring Categories)
Self-Efficacy Beliefs of ESD Teaching
(29; 3)
Responsibility toward
Climate Change and Biodiversity
(6; 3)
Attitudes toward SD
(18; 3)
Self-Assessed Knowledge Regarding SD
(11; 5)
Item Separation Reliability0.970.960.980.99
Variance (SE)0.64 (0.06)1.46 (0.21)0.47 (0.06)0.77 (0.09)
EAP/PV Reliability0.880.720.750.80
WLE Person Separation Reliability0.880.680.750.84
Average Item Difficulty (SE)−0.33 (0.03)−0.65 (0.03)−0.43 (0.04)−0.06 (0.04)
Item Difficulty range−2.45–0.77−1.55–0.17−1.68–0.78−1.25–1.23
wMNSQ range0.75–1.220.87–1.120.77–1.250.76–1.39
Discrimination range0.26–0.610.54–0.720.22–0.540.46–0.71
Table 6. Latent correlations of two Five-Dimensional modellings of (a) Situational/Conceptual Knowledge with the validation instruments of self-efficacy beliefs of Education for Sustainable Development teaching, responsibility toward climate change and biodiversity, attitudes toward Sustainable Development, and self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues, and of (b) Procedural Knowledge with the beforehand mentioned validation instruments, respectively (N = 314).
Table 6. Latent correlations of two Five-Dimensional modellings of (a) Situational/Conceptual Knowledge with the validation instruments of self-efficacy beliefs of Education for Sustainable Development teaching, responsibility toward climate change and biodiversity, attitudes toward Sustainable Development, and self-assessed knowledge of SD-relevant issues, and of (b) Procedural Knowledge with the beforehand mentioned validation instruments, respectively (N = 314).
Dimension(a) Situational/Conceptual Knowledge(b) Procedural KnowledgeSelf-Efficacy Beliefs 1 Responsibility 1 Attitudes 1
Self-Efficacy Beliefs0.002−0.14
Responsibility0.040.180.35
Attitudes−0.010.080.340.55
Self-Assessed Knowledge0.260.10−0.030.11−0.02
1 The latent correlations between the validation instruments itself were calculated as mean of latent correlations of both Five-Dimensional modellings with (a) Situational/Conceptual Knowledge and (b) Procedural Knowledge.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Richter-Beuschel, L.; Bögeholz, S. Knowledge of Student Teachers on Sustainable Land Use Issues–Knowledge Types Relevant for Teacher Education. Sustainability 2020, 12, 8332. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208332

AMA Style

Richter-Beuschel L, Bögeholz S. Knowledge of Student Teachers on Sustainable Land Use Issues–Knowledge Types Relevant for Teacher Education. Sustainability. 2020; 12(20):8332. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208332

Chicago/Turabian Style

Richter-Beuschel, Lisa, and Susanne Bögeholz. 2020. "Knowledge of Student Teachers on Sustainable Land Use Issues–Knowledge Types Relevant for Teacher Education" Sustainability 12, no. 20: 8332. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208332

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop