Next Article in Journal
Holistic Sexual-Reproductive Healthcare Services and Needs for Queer Individuals: Healthcare Providers’ Perspectives
Next Article in Special Issue
Online Video-Mediated Compassion Training Program for Mental Health and Well-Being of University Students
Previous Article in Journal
Physician Workforce in Lithuania: Changes during Thirty Years of Independence
Previous Article in Special Issue
Youth Are the Experts! Youth Participatory Action Research to Address the Adolescent Mental Health Crisis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Healthy Lifestyle Related to Executive Functions in Chilean University Students: A Pilot Study

by
Felipe Caamaño-Navarrete
1,2,
Carlos Arriagada-Hernández
1,2,
Gerardo Fuentes-Vilugrón
1,2,
Lorena Jara-Tomckowiack
2,3,
Alvaro Levin-Catrilao
4,
Pablo del Val Martín
5,
Flavio Muñoz-Troncoso
6,7 and
Pedro Delgado-Floody
8,*
1
Physical Education Career, Faculty of Education, Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Temuco 4780000, Chile
2
Collaborative Research Group for School Development (GICDE), Temuco 4780000, Chile
3
Faculty of Education, Universidad Católica de Temuco, Temuco 4780000, Chile
4
Doctoral Programme in Physical Activity Sciences, Faculty of Education Sciences, Universidad Católica del Maule, Talca 3460000, Chile
5
Chilean Observatory of Physical Education and School Sport, Faculty of Education and Social Sciences, Universidad Andres Bello, Las Condes, Santiago 7550000, Chile
6
Faculty of Social Sciences and Arts, Universidad Mayor, Temuco 4780000, Chile
7
Department of Psychology and Anthropology, Faculty of Education and Psychology, Universidad de Extremadura, 06071 Badajoz, Spain
8
Department of Physical Education, Sport and Recreation, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco 4811230, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Healthcare 2024, 12(10), 1022; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12101022
Submission received: 16 April 2024 / Revised: 9 May 2024 / Accepted: 10 May 2024 / Published: 15 May 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Psychological Well-Being for Adolescents and Youths)

Abstract

:
Background: A negative lifestyle is reported to be related to cognitive problems. However, there is little information about this in relation to university students. The objective of the present study was to investigate the association between executive functions (EFs) and lifestyle parameters (i.e., physical activity (PA), sleep duration, screen time (ST), and food habits) among Chilean university students. Methods: This cross-sectional study included a total of 150 university students (94 females and 56 males, aged 21.28 ± 3.15 and 22.18 ± 2.90 years, respectively). Cognitive outcomes were measured using the CogniFit assessment battery. Lifestyle was measured through validated questionnaires. Results: Across the total sample, attention exhibited a positive association with PA h/week (β: 24.34 95% CI: 12.46 to 36.22, p = 0.001). Additionally, coordination was positively associated with PA h/week (β: 15.06 95% CI: 0.62 to 29.50, p < 0.041). PA h/week was positively linked with reasoning (β: 20.34 95% CI: 4.52 to 36.17, p = 0.012) and perception (β: 13.81 95% CI: 4.14 to 23.49, p = 0.005). Moreover, PA h/week was significantly linked to memory (β: 23.01 95% CI: 7.62 to 38.40, p = 0.004). In terms of the EFs, PA h/week showed a positive association with cognitive flexibility (β: 45.60 95% CI: 23.22 to 67.69, p = 0.001). Conclusions: In conclusion, lifestyle (PA h/week) was positively associated with EFs. Therefore, an increase in PA levels among these students should be a target for community- and university-based interventions in order to promote cognitive development such as attention, coordination, reasoning, perception, memory, and cognitive flexibility.

1. Introduction

Executive functions (EFs) refer to a set of higher-order cognitive abilities that enable the assessment and achievement of a goal [1]. In addition, EFs are fundamental for self-regulation, problem solving and decision making [2]. Furthermore, it has been suggested that EFs are essential for developing adaptive behaviours that involve diverse ways of processing information, different sensory modalities, and executing responses, including aspects related to memory and emotional regulation [3]. Moreover, higher EFs among university students have been associated with various benefits [2,4,5]. Complementary to the above, EFs exhibit varying profiles depending on the developmental stage of humans. These functions begin to emerge in infancy with basic skills (up to the age of 3), and more specific skills develop during early childhood. They continue to develop at their own pace in adolescence [6], and cognitive performance peaks during young adulthood [6,7]. However, they decline in old age, mainly due to structural and functional changes in the prefrontal cortex [8].
Existing evidence suggests that university is a crucial stage for the development of EFs [9]. In this sense, in a university context, EFs have a significant impact on student success and achievement [10]. In this sense, previous evidence has shown that EFs are linked to academic success, indicating the importance of promoting learning and achievement [5,11,12]. However, EFs not only contribute to the academic performance of students but are also related to mental health, physical health, health-related quality of life (HRQoL), and job success [2]. Complementary to the above, recent studies have shown that EFs could be related to health levels, lifestyle parameters (i.e., physical activity [PA] levels, diet quality, and sleep quality), and emotional regulation [5,13,14,15,16]. Complementary to the above, EFs may play a potential role in social functioning in university students [17]. On the other hand, university students who show poorer levels of EFs can be expected to have problems in the study process [18]. Therefore, addressing poor EFs as a variable for measurement among university students could be considered a priority.
Unhealthy lifestyles have become a public health concern [19] and are associated with increased cardiovascular morbidity and mortality [20]. In addition, university students form part of the population most at risk of developing unhealthy lifestyle behaviours [21]. Moreover, it has been indicated that the university context is a critical stage during which students begin making their own decisions [22]. When individuals enter university, they experience changes associated with increased autonomy and exposure to a new environment. This can lead to higher levels of stress, which may result in an increase in unhealthy patterns [19,23,24,25]. In this context, it has been demonstrated that university students often have poor health habits [26]. Evidence has also shown that university students are more prone to unhealthy lifestyle choices (i.e., physical inactivity, sedentary behaviour, unhealthy eating habits, smoking, and alcohol consumption) [27,28]. Likewise, healthy lifestyle patterns can improve overall health, as well as preventing disease [29]. In line with the above, university students may be more prone to weight gain due to extended periods of screen time (ST) on devices such as mobile phones and computers. This can make it challenging for them to find the motivation to engage in activities that promote healthy living [23].
People’s lifestyles have commonly been investigated in the context of health; however, it has been shown that lifestyle factors are intertwined with EFs in university students [30]. For instance, research has shown that engaging in physical activities, such as sports, has a positive impact on brain health, improves cognitive functions, and reduces the risk of dementia in old age [9]. Complementarily, healthy lifestyle habits such as healthy eating habits and practising PA could offer protection against cognitive decline [31]. Similarly, ST activities may be negatively linked with EFs [32]. This statement demonstrates a clear connection between leading a healthy lifestyle and the cognitive abilities of humans. Likewise, previous data regarding young people have indicated that unhealthy lifestyles (i.e., PA levels and poor sleep quality) were linked with poorer EFs [33]. Moreover, a previous study conducted with Mexican university students indicated that EFs were positively related with healthy lifestyle factors such as eating habits [34]. Furthermore, it has been shown that better PA levels are linked with higher executive inhibitory control especially in female university students [9]. Complementary to the above, it has been reported that more frequent moderate-to-vigorous or light PA was related to better EFs in young adults [35]. Indeed, existing evidence shows a general consensus that developing healthy lifestyle habits such as PA is helpful for cognitive functioning [36]. In a complementary way, a recent study conducted among college students reported that increased daily participation in PA could be beneficial for their EFs and, in addition, PA was negatively associated with negative emotion [37]. Therefore, the evidence contributes to consolidating the positive association between PA and brain function [38]. In this context, another investigation showed that PA and exercise may have positive effects on cognitive processes [39]. On the other hand, an unhealthy lifestyle may negatively impact EFs. In this context, it has been indicated that poor and unhealthy habits (i.e., skipping breakfast) could impact negatively on cognitive functions [40]. Intriguingly, a pilot study conducted in university students reported that sedentary behavior negatively predicted cognitive inhibition [41], and it has been shown that better EFs are linked with less sedentary behavior in university students [42]. The above is important to consider since it has been reported that university promotes behaviors such as sitting [43].
Against this background and to the best of our knowledge, no other study has explored the association between EFs and lifestyle in Chilean university students. The objective of this study was, therefore, to investigate the association between EFs and lifestyle parameters (i.e., PA, sleep duration, ST, and food habits) among Chilean university students and to determine the differences in EFs and lifestyle parameters according to sex.

2. Materials and Methods

A descriptive study with a cross-sectional design was developed. A total of 150 university students (94 females and 56 males, aged 21.28 ± 3.15 and 22.18 ± 2.90 years, respectively) from the Universidad Autónoma de Chile in Temuco, Chile participated in the study. A total of 33 subjects were excluded (women not meeting the inclusion criteria or for other reasons (n = 20); men not meeting the inclusion criteria or for other reasons (n = 13). The sample was intentional and non-probabilistic by convenience.
The inclusion criteria encompassed the following conditions: (i) obtaining informed consent from the participants and (ii) being university students. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (i) any musculoskeletal injuries or medical contraindications that would prevent subjects from performing averagely in the assessments and (ii) not being present at the time of the evaluations or failing to provide informed consent. The investigation complied with the Declaration of Helsinki (2013) and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Chile (N° CEC 18-23 Act). All students gave their written consent on the day of the assessment.

2.1. Measures

2.1.1. Cognitive Battery

To determine the cognitive domains and EFs, the 40 min CogniFit (San Francisco, CA, USA) neurocognitive assessment battery was used [31,32]. In this study, apart from the cognitive dimension score (i.e., attention, perception, reasoning, coordination, and memory), EFs (i.e., inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) were examined. Correspondingly, this battery has been reported to have good reliability [32]. In addition, this cognitive battery was previously performed by adult subjects [33].
The application of this evaluation is simple and intuitive, so that any professional can apply it without difficulty. In addition, it has been designed so that it can be used either face to face in a consultation or laboratory or remotely from the homes of patients or participants. This neuropsychological test was administered online, with an approximate duration of 30–40 min. At the end of the evaluation, a complete results report was automatically obtained with the user’s neurocognitive profile. In addition, this testing method provides valuable information that, as professionals, can help us detect whether there is a risk of any disorder or problem, recognize its severity, and identify the most appropriate support strategies for each case. It is recommended that this neuropsychological assessment is performed when the researcher wants to know the brain function or cognitive, physical, psychological, or social well-being of the patient or participant. This evaluation battery should be used complementary to the professional diagnosis and not as a substitute for a clinical interview. Each of the neuropsychological tasks contained in the cognitive assessment battery (CAB) for professionals has been validated following the scientific method. This ensures appropriate psychometric characteristics for an effective evaluation of the patient’s or participant’s cognitive status. The cognitive profile of the neuropsychological report has high reliability, consistency, and stability. Through cross-sectional research designs, psychometric statistics have been obtained with values close to 0.9, such as Cronbach’s alpha coefficient. The test–retest tests have obtained values close to 1, which demonstrates high reliability and precision [32,33].

2.1.2. Lifestyle

To evaluate the quality of the university students’ diet, students completed a diet questionnaire that had previously been used with Chilean university students [44]. The instrument consisted of 15 dichotomous-response questions (yes–no) about eating habits. The scores were categorized as follows:
≥13 points = healthy eating; between 10 and 12 points = you are on the right track, but you should improve; between 7 and 9 points = unhealthy eating; and ≤6 points = very unhealthy eating [45].
The original survey classified respondents into “healthy eating” (≥13 points), “you are on the right track, but you need to improve” (between 10 and 12 points), “unhealthy eating” (between 7 and 9 points), and “unhealthy diet” (≤6 points); to facilitate data analysis, the first two categories were merged. The final instrument was subjected to validation through expert judgment, carried out by two nutritionists. In addition to the survey, the place of residence and daily mealtimes on campus were considered.
PA was determined by using a short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire [46]. This instrument has been used and validated in Chilean adults [47]. The questions ask about the time spent being physically active in the past 7 days. It requests the following from the participants: Please answer each question even if you do not consider yourself an active person. Please think about the activities you do in study time or work, as part of your home or garden tasks, moving from one place to another, or in your free time for recreation, exercise, or sport. Think about all the intense activities you did in the past 7 days. Intense physical activities refer to those that involve intense physical effort and make you breathe much harder than normal. Think only about those physical activities that you did for at least 10 min straight.
Screen time and sleep duration were determined through the following questions that had been previously used in different studies [48,49]: “How many hours a week do you watch videos?”, “How many hours a week do you play video or computer games?”, and “How many hours of sleep do you usually get per day and/or night?”. The ST was computed from the sum of the two questions and analyzed in hours/day. Complementary to the above, a study conducted with a nationwide sample of youth indicated that the questions on ST and sleep are applicable to a young population [50].
The questionnaires and the CogniFit neurocognitive assessment battery were completed individually in the presence of assistant researchers (who respected data confidentiality and clarified any potential doubts or questions). All the evaluations took place in a computer lab during the morning.

2.2. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 21.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and Levene’s test were used to assess the normal distribution of data and homogeneity of variances. Continuous variables were expressed as means and confidence intervals. Differences in the comparison between the sexes were established using an analysis of variance Student’s t-test. To determine the association between EFs and lifestyle parameters, a simple linear regression was used. The significance level was set at p < 0.05.

3. Results

Table 1 displays a comparison of the study variables according to sex. Significant differences in attention (p = 0.011), working memory (p = 0.037), diet quality score (p = 0.037), PA h/week (p = 0.001), ST h/day (p = 0.012), and sleep duration h/day (p = 0.036) were observed.
In the total sample, attention exhibited a positive association with PA h/week (β: 24.34 95% CI: 12.46 to 36.22, p < 0.001). Additionally, coordination was positively associated with PA h/week (β: 15.06 95% CI: 0.62 to 29.50, p = 0.041) (Table 2).
Conversely, PA h/week was positively linked with reasoning (β: 20.34 95% CI: 4.52 to 36.17, p = 0.012) and perception (β: 13.81 95% CI: 4.14 to 23.49, p = 0.005) (Table 3).
Moreover, PA h/week was significantly linked to memory (β: 23.01 95% CI: 7.62 to 38.40, p = 0.004) (Table 4).
In terms of executive functions, PA h/week showed a positive association with cognitive flexibility (β: 45.60 95% CI: 23.22 to 67.69, p = 0.001) (Table 5).

4. Discussion

The objective of this study was to investigate the association between EFs and lifestyle parameters (i.e., PA, sleep duration, ST, and food habits) among Chilean university students and to determine the differences in EFs and lifestyle parameters according to sex. The main findings of this study are as follows: (i) men had better results in attention, memory, working memory, diet, and PA h/week. Women had less ST h/day and more hours of sleep; (ii) PA h/week was positively related to attention, coordination, reasoning, perception, memory, and cognitive flexibility; and (iii) ST, food habits, and sleep were not associated with EFs.
These findings highlight the importance of considering gender differences when analyzing EFs and lifestyle habits among university students. In this regard, another study among university students showed that there were differences in self-regulatory EFs according to sex [51]. Moreover, a cross-sectional study reported that EFs were linked with the orbitomedial cortex and moderated by sex in university students [52]. A recent study indicated that there were no differences in EFs between men and women [53]. Furthermore, a previous study among university students showed that men had higher PA scores than women [54]. In addition, it has been reported that female students spend more time studying [55]. In line with the above, previous evidence showed that women presented a higher proportion of physical inactivity than men among university students [56]. Other evidence has suggested that males had betters results on working memory tasks than females, while females had better results in reading comprehension than their counterparts [57]. Likewise, a previous investigation indicated that the executive functioning was related to individual differences such as sex [58]. Moreover, a systematic review with meta-analysis showed sex differences in verbal working memory [59]. Complementary to the above, specific gender differences in cognitive tasks have been reported [60]. Likewise, another study found that there were sex differences in selective attention [61]. In addition, it was found that there were sex differences in inhibitory control among university students [62]. Complementary to the above, another study reported sex differences in EFs in university students [63].
In this study, we found that PA h/week was positively related to cognitive flexibility. This evidence contributes to consolidating the positive association between PA and EFs in university students. In this sense, the positive links between PA and EFs observed in this study are consistent with extensive sections of the literature [64,65,66,67,68]. In this context, a systematic review reported that PA can be a way to improve cognitive outcomes and language skills in adolescents and young adults [69]. Another study indicated that EFs and PA could influence academic performance [70]. Similarly, a cross-sectional study among university students reported that PA was positively related to EFs [9]. In agreement with the above, a study indicated that practicing regular PA could have a beneficial and multifaceted impact on executive functioning, which encompasses various cognitive areas that are crucial for academic performance and daily functioning [71]. Likewise, PA has been shown to improve general cognitive functioning, including attention, memory, cognitive flexibility, and problem-solving skills [72]. In this regard, a study conducted with university students found that vigorous PA was linked with inhibition and working memory [73]. Another study conducted in adult subjects showed a positive link between objectively measured PA (i.e., moderate-to-vigorous and light physical activity) and EFs [35]. In addition, it has been shown that regular PA has a positive selective impact on EFs in adult subjects [74]. Moreover, data from university students showed that PA was associated with subjective EFs [75]. Therefore, increasing university students’ daily PA could positively influence EFs.
In this context, another study among university students found that healthy lifestyles (including PA) were positive for inhibitory control performance [76]. In addition, it has been reported that acute aerobic PA positively impacts inhibitory control [77]. Data from female college students showed that PA was positively related to working memory [78]. This previous evidence contributes to solidifying the positive association between PA and EFs in university students. Complementary to the above, a recent study concluded that increasing PA could improve working memory performance in college students [79]. Building on previously reported findings, it is suggested that during university, exercise may be linked to better cognitive flexibility [80]. Similarly, another study indicated that PA was positively associated with EFs, specifically in relation to executive inhibitory control [9]. Likewise, there is strong evidence about the positive relation between being active and EFs in university students [35]. Consistent with the above, another study has shown that PA is linked to better EFs and increased EFs may improve academic performance in university students [81].
In this study, we found that PA h/week was positively related to attention, coordination, reasoning, perception, and memory. In this line, it has been shown that practice PA and exercise may have positive effects on cognitive processes [39]. For example, a study reported a positive association between healthy lifestyle that included PA and cognitive function such as selective attention and concentration [82]. In this context, previous evidence indicated that PA is linked with better attention and memory [83]. Likewise, it has been indicated that PA promotes the release of growth factors and reduces brain inflammation; therefore, PA could prevent cognitive decline [68]. In addition, another study reported that healthy lifestyle habits were related to better cognitive function [84]. In addition, previous evidence has shown that PA is an effective method to stimulate brain plasticity [85]. In this context, a study among university students reported that PA was positively related to cognitive functions such as creativity [86]. Moreover, it has been reported that PA may improve attention and psychomotor vigilance in undergraduate students [87]. For example, a recent study reported that a single session of aerobic exercise increased BDNF serum levels [88]. Complementary to the above, PA interventions have been shown to improve EFs in university students [89]. However, that study did not find associations between ST, eating habits, sleep, and EFs. In this context, data from three empirical studies reported no relation between sleep quality and EFs in young adults [90]. Contrary to our results, another study reported that better EFs were related to less follow-up sedentary behavior [42]. Additionally, it has been reported that healthy eating habits are related to EFs among university students [30]. Previous evidence has shown that a healthy lifestyle is beneficial for EFs [91]. In addition, another study among university students reported that healthy eating habits were positively associated with academic performance [92]. Moreover, it has been indicated that a healthy lifestyle may decrease the risk of cognitive decline [93].
In the present study, the main limitation is the cross-sectional design. In addition, we used a convenience sample. Among the study’s strengths, we can highlight (i) the simplicity of the assessments (which would allow their use and application in healthy lifestyle interventions focused on university students) and (ii) the fact that cognitive measures were obtained using a computer neurocognitive assessment battery.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, firstly, attention exhibited a positive association with PA h/week. This suggests that engaging in regular PA is beneficial for maintaining attentional processes among university students, potentially enhancing their ability to focus and concentrate during academic tasks and activities. Secondly, coordination was also positively associated with PA h/week. This finding implies that physical activities improve coordination skills. Interestingly, this study also found that PA h/week was positively linked with reasoning and perception. This suggests that engaging in PA may benefit not only physical health but also cognitive processes related to logical thinking, problem-solving, and sensory perception. This is particularly important in a university setting where students are often required to engage in complex cognitive tasks and academic challenges. Moreover, the association between PA h/week and memory highlights the potential cognitive benefits of PA. Regular engagement in physical activities may support memory functions, including encoding storage and retrieval of information. This has implications for academic performance, as effective memory abilities are crucial for learning and retaining new information. In terms of EFs, this study found a positive association between PA h/week and cognitive flexibility. EFs play a critical role in higher-order cognitive processes such as planning, decision making, and adaptability to changing situations. The positive association with cognitive flexibility suggests that PA may enhance students’ ability to switch between different tasks, strategies, or mental sets, which are essential skills for academic and real-life success. Likewise, this study underscores the importance of lifestyle factors such as PA in promoting cognitive performance, specifically EFs, among university students. These findings highlight the multidimensional benefits of PA beyond physical health, extending to cognitive functions that are crucial for academic achievement and overall well-being. Therefore, promoting and encouraging regular PA among university students can serve as a valuable strategy to enhance cognitive performance and support academic success. Future research and interventions may further explore the mechanisms underlying the relationship between physical activity and cognitive functions to develop targeted strategies for optimizing cognitive outcomes in educational settings. This conclusion is drawn from the positive associations observed between PA h/week and attention, coordination, reasoning, perception, memory, and cognitive flexibility. Therefore, an increase in PA levels among these students should be a target for community- and university-based interventions in order to promote cognitive development such as attention, coordination, reasoning, perception, memory, and cognitive flexibility.

Author Contributions

F.C.-N., P.D.-F., C.A.-H. and L.J.-T. contributed to the conception, organization, and oversight of the study, the drafting of the analysis plan, the writing of the original manuscript draft, and final approval of the version to be published. G.F.-V., A.L.-C., P.d.V.M. and F.M.-T. contributed to critical manuscript revision and final approval of the version to be published. F.C.-N. and P.D.-F. contributed to data analysis and interpretation, critical manuscript revision, and final approval of the version to be published. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the internal DIUA Project 266-2023 of the Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Chile.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The investigation complies with the Declaration of Helsinki (2013) and has been approved by an Ethics Committee of Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Chile (N° CEC 18-23 Act, approved on May 2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are contained within the article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Cristofori, I.; Cohen-Zimerman, S.; Grafman, J. Chapter 11—Executive functions. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology; D’Esposito, M., Grafman, J.H., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019; Volume 163, pp. 197–219. [Google Scholar]
  2. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Salehinejad, M.A.; Ghanavati, E.; Rashid, M.H.A.; Nitsche, M.A. Hot and cold executive functions in the brain: A prefrontal-cingular network. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 2021, 5, 23982128211007769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Ramos-Galarza, C.; Acosta-Rodas, P.; Bolaños-Pasquel, M.; Lepe-Martínez, N. The role of executive functions in academic performance and behaviour of university students. J. Appl. Res. High. Educ. 2020, 12, 444–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ramos-Galarza, C.; Ramos, V.; Del Valle, M.; Lepe-Martínez, N.; Cruz-Cárdenas, J.; Acosta-Rodas, P.; Bolaños-Pasquel, M. Executive functions scale for university students: UEF-1. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1192555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Ferguson, H.J.; Brunsdon, V.E.A.; Bradford, E.E.F. The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Salling, Z.N.; Szymkowicz, S.M.; Dotson, V.M. 58 Hippocampal Subregions Predict Executive Function Across the Adult Lifespan. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2023, 29, 466–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Friedman, N.P.; Robbins, T.W. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 2022, 47, 72–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Salas-Gomez, D.; Fernandez-Gorgojo, M.; Pozueta, A.; Diaz-Ceballos, I.; Lamarain, M.; Perez, C.; Kazimierczak, M.; Sanchez-Juan, P. Physical activity is associated with better executive function in university students. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Dias, N.M.; Ávila, B.M.; Costa, D.M.d.; Cardoso, C.O.; Fonseca, R.P. Is it possible to promote executive functions in university students? Evidence of effectiveness of the πFEx-Academics. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 2022, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kolovelonis, A.; Papastergiou, M.; Samara, E.; Goudas, M. Acute effects of exergaming on students’ executive functions and situational interest in elementary physical education. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 1902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Stark, M.D.; Lindo, E.J. Executive functioning supports for college students with an autism Spectrum disorder. Rev. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2023, 10, 604–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Jiang, S.Y.; Shan, H.D.; Zhang, R.T.; Lui, S.S.; Yang, H.X.; Cheung, E.F.; Chan, R.C. Network analysis of executive function, emotion, and social anhedonia. PsyCh J. 2022, 11, 232–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Liu, Y.; Zhu, L.; Cai, K.; Dong, X.; Xiong, X.; Liu, Z.; Chen, A. Relationship between cardiorespiratory fitness and executive function in young adults: Mediating effects of gray matter volume. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Meng, F.; Xie, C.; Qiu, F.; Geng, J.; Li, F. Effects of physical activity level on attentional networks in young adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 5374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Sperduti, M.; Makowski, D.; Arcangeli, M.; Wantzen, P.; Zalla, T.; Lemaire, S.; Dokic, J.; Pelletier, J.; Piolino, P. The distinctive role of executive functions in implicit emotion regulation. Acta Psychol. 2017, 173, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Hilton, D.C.; Canu, W.H.; Jarrett, M.A. The importance of executive functioning for social skills in college students: A relative weights analysis. J. Am. Coll. Health 2022, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Baars, M.A.; Nije Bijvank, M.; Tonnaer, G.H.; Jolles, J. Self-report measures of executive functioning are a determinant of academic performance in first-year students at a university of applied sciences. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Bennasar-Veny, M.; Yañez, A.M.; Pericas, J.; Ballester, L.; Fernandez-Dominguez, J.C.; Tauler, P.; Aguilo, A. Cluster analysis of health-related lifestyles in university students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lönnberg, L.; Ekblom-Bak, E.; Damberg, M. Improved unhealthy lifestyle habits in patients with high cardiovascular risk: Results from a structured lifestyle programme in primary care. Upsala J. Med. Sci. 2019, 124, 94–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Carballo-Fazanes, A.; Rico-Díaz, J.; Barcala-Furelos, R.; Rey, E.; Rodríguez-Fernández, J.E.; Varela-Casal, C.; Abelairas-Gómez, C. Physical Activity Habits and Determinants, Sedentary Behaviour and Lifestyle in University Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. El Ansari, W.; Stock, C.; John, J.; Deeny, P.; Phillips, C.; Snelgrove, S.; Adetunji, H.; Hu, X.; Parke, S.; Stoate, M. Health promoting behaviours and lifestyle characteristics of students at seven universities in the UK. Cent. Eur. J. Public Health 2011, 19, 197–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Assaf, I.; Brieteh, F.; Tfaily, M.; El-Baida, M.; Kadry, S.; Balusamy, B. Students university healthy lifestyle practice: Quantitative analysis. Health Inf. Sci. Syst. 2019, 7, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Dodd, L.J.; Al-Nakeeb, Y.; Nevill, A.; Forshaw, M.J. Lifestyle risk factors of students: A cluster analytical approach. Prev. Med. 2010, 51, 73–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hultgren, B.A.; Turrisi, R.; Cleveland, M.J.; Mallett, K.A.; Reavy, R.; Larimer, M.E.; Geisner, I.M.; Hospital, M.M. Transitions in drinking behaviors across the college years: A latent transition analysis. Addict. Behav. 2019, 92, 108–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Al-Awwad, N.J.; Al-Sayyed, H.F.; Zeinah, Z.A.; Tayyem, R.F. Dietary and lifestyle habits among university students at different academic years. Clin. Nutr. ESPEN 2021, 44, 236–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Chao, D.-P. Health-promoting lifestyle and influencing factors among students from different types of universities in Taiwan. Taiwan Gong Gong Wei Sheng Za Zhi 2022, 41, 312–330. [Google Scholar]
  28. Davoren, M.P.; Demant, J.; Shiely, F.; Perry, I.J. Alcohol consumption among university students in Ireland and the United Kingdom from 2002 to 2014: A systematic review. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Algahtani, F.D. Healthy Lifestyle among Ha’il University Students, Saudi Arabia. Int. J. Pharm. Res. Allied Sci. 2020, 9, 160–167. [Google Scholar]
  30. Pilato, I.B.; Beezhold, B.; Radnitz, C. Diet and lifestyle factors associated with cognitive performance in college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2022, 70, 2230–2236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Serra, M.C.; Dondero, K.R.; Larkins, D.; Burns, A.; Addison, O. Healthy lifestyle and cognition: Interaction between diet and physical activity. Curr. Nutr. Rep. 2020, 9, 64–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Li, S.; Guo, J.; Zheng, K.; Shi, M.; Huang, T. Is sedentary behavior associated with executive function in children and adolescents? A systematic review. Front. Public Health 2022, 10, 832845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Tee, J.Y.H.; Gan, W.Y.; Tan, K.-A.; Chin, Y.S. Obesity and unhealthy lifestyle associated with poor executive function among Malaysian adolescents. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0195934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Chávez-Hernández, M.E. Correlation of executive functions, academic achievement, eating behavior and eating habits in university students of Mexico City. Front. Educ. 2023, 8, 1268302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lin, J.; Wang, K.; Chen, Z.; Fan, X.; Shen, L.; Wang, Y.; Yang, Y.; Huang, T. Associations between objectively measured physical activity and executive functioning in young adults. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2018, 125, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Alghadir, A.H.; Gabr, S.A.; Iqbal, Z.A.; Al-Eisa, E. Association of physical activity, vitamin E levels, and total antioxidant capacity with academic performance and executive functions of adolescents. BMC Pediatr. 2019, 19, 156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Zhao, G.; Sun, K.; Fu, J.; Li, Z.; Liu, D.; Tian, X.; Yang, J.; Zhang, Q. Impact of physical activity on executive functions: A moderated mediation model. Front. Psychol. 2024, 14, 1226667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Di Liegro, C.M.; Schiera, G.; Proia, P.; Di Liegro, I. Physical activity and brain health. Genes 2019, 10, 720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Padilla, C.; Mayas, J.; Ballesteros, S.; Andrés, P. The role of chronic physical exercise and selective attention at encoding on implicit and explicit memory. Memory 2017, 25, 1026–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Sámano, R.; Hernández-Chávez, C.; Chico-Barba, G.; Córdova-Barrios, A.; Morales-del-Olmo, M.; Sordo-Figuero, H.; Hernández, M.; Merino-Palacios, C.; Cervantes-Zamora, L.; Martínez-Rojano, H. Breakfast nutritional quality and cognitive interference in university students from Mexico city. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Magnon, V.; Vallet, G.T.; Dutheil, F.; Auxiette, C. Sedentary lifestyle matters as past sedentariness, not current sedentariness, predicts cognitive inhibition performance among college students: An exploratory study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Loprinzi, P.D.; Nooe, A. Executive function influences sedentary behavior: A longitudinal study. Health Promot. Perspect. 2016, 6, 180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Felez-Nobrega, M.; Hillman, C.H.; Dowd, K.P.; Cirera, E.; Puig-Ribera, A. ActivPAL™ determined sedentary behaviour, physical activity and academic achievement in college students. J. Sports Sci. 2018, 36, 2311–2316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Mardones, L.; Muñoz, M.; Esparza, J.; Troncoso-Pantoja, C. Hábitos alimentarios en estudiantes universitarios de la Región de Bío-Bío, Chile, 2017. Perspect. En Nutr. Humana 2021, 23, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Zacarías, I.; Olivares, S. Alimentación saludable. In Nutrición en el Ciclo Vital; Editorial Mediterráneo: Santiago, Chile, 2014; pp. 431–440. [Google Scholar]
  46. Caamano-Navarrete, F.; Latorre-Roman, P.A.; Parraga-Montilla, J.A.; Alvarez, C.; Delgado-Floody, P. Association between creativity and memory with cardiorespiratory fitness and lifestyle among Chilean schoolchildren. Nutrients 2021, 13, 1799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Balboa-Castillo, T.; Muñoz, S.; Serón, P.; Andrade-Mayorga, O.; Lavados-Romo, P.; Aguilar-Farias, N. Validity and reliability of the international physical activity questionnaire short form in Chilean adults. PLoS ONE 2023, 18, e0291604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. García-Hermoso, A.; Ezzatvar, Y.; Ramírez-Vélez, R.; López-Gil, J.F.; Izquierdo, M. Trajectories of 24-h movement guidelines from middle adolescence to adulthood on depression and suicidal ideation: A 22-year follow-up study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2022, 19, 135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. López-Gil, J.F.; Roman-Viñas, B.; Aznar, S.; Tremblay, M.S. Meeting 24-h movement guidelines: Prevalence, correlates, and associations with socioemotional behavior in Spanish minors. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2022, 32, 881–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. López-Gil, J.F.; Firth, J.; García-Hermoso, A. Is meeting with the 24-h movement recommendations linked with suicidality? Results from a nationwide sample of 44,734 U.S. adolescents. J. Affect. Disord. 2024, 349, 617–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Franklin, P.; Tsujimoto, K.C.; Lewis, M.E.; Tekok-Kilic, A.; Frijters, J.C. Sex differences in self-regulatory executive functions are amplified by trait anxiety: The case of students at risk for academic failure. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 129, 131–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Vilca, L.W. The moderating role of sex in the relationship between executive functions and academic procrastination in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 928425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ismail, S.N.; Azhan, M.A.N.; Omar, S.S.S.; Miswan, M.S.; Zainuddin, N.F. Differences of Cognitive Function Between Genders Among University Students. Malays. J. Sport Sci. Recreat. 2024, 20, 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  54. Ubago-Jiménez, J.L.; Zurita-Ortega, F.; San Román-Mata, S.; Puertas-Molero, P.; González-Valero, G. Impact of Physical Activity Practice and Adherence to the Mediterranean Diet in Relation to Multiple Intelligences among University Students. Nutrients 2020, 12, 2630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Cena, H.; Porri, D.; De Giuseppe, R.; Kalmpourtzidou, A.; Salvatore, F.P.; El Ghoch, M.; Itani, L.; Kreidieh, D.; Brytek-Matera, A.; Pocol, C.B. How healthy are health-related behaviors in university students: The HOLISTic study. Nutrients 2021, 13, 675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Benaich, S.; Mehdad, S.; Andaloussi, Z.; Boutayeb, S.; Alamy, M.; Aguenaou, H.; Taghzouti, K. Weight status, dietary habits, physical activity, screen time and sleep duration among university students. Nutr. Health 2021, 27, 69–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Wierenga, L.M.; Bos, M.G.; van Rossenberg, F.; Crone, E.A. Sex effects on development of brain structure and executive functions: Greater variance than mean effects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019, 31, 730–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Pazzaglia, M.; Chen, E.Y.; Casagrande, M. High-level executive functions: A possible role of sex and weight condition in planning and decision-making performances. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Voyer, D.; Saint Aubin, J.; Altman, K.; Gallant, G. Sex differences in verbal working memory: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2021, 147, 352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Hill, A.C.; Laird, A.R.; Robinson, J.L. Gender differences in working memory networks: A BrainMap meta-analysis. Biol. Psychol. 2014, 102, 18–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Stoet, G. Sex differences in the Simon task help to interpret sex differences in selective attention. Psychol. Res. 2017, 81, 571–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Di, S.; Ma, C.; Wu, X.; Lei, L. Gender differences in behavioral inhibitory control under evoked acute stress: An event-related potential study. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1107935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. He, W.-J.; Wong, W.-C. Gender differences in the distribution of creativity scores: Domain-specific patterns in divergent thinking and creative problem solving. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 626911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Jimenez, M.P.; DeVille, N.V.; Elliott, E.G.; Schiff, J.E.; Wilt, G.E.; Hart, J.E.; James, P. Associations between nature exposure and health: A review of the evidence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Galle, S.A.; Liu, J.; Bonnechère, B.; Amin, N.; Milders, M.M.; Deijen, J.B.; Scherder, E.J.; Drent, M.L.; Voortman, T.; Ikram, M.A. The long-term relation between physical activity and executive function in the Rotterdam Study. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2023, 38, 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. You, Y.; Liu, J.; Wang, D.; Fu, Y.; Liu, R.; Ma, X. Cognitive performance in short sleep young adults with different physical activity levels: A cross-sectional fNIRS study. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Gürdere, C.; Strobach, T.; Pastore, M.; Pfeffer, I. Do executive functions predict physical activity behavior? A meta-analysis. BMC Psychol. 2023, 11, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Chen, C.; Nakagawa, S. Physical activity for cognitive health promotion: An overview of the underlying neurobiological mechanisms. Ageing Res. Rev. 2023, 86, 101868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Haverkamp, B.F.; Wiersma, R.; Vertessen, K.; van Ewijk, H.; Oosterlaan, J.; Hartman, E. Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive outcomes and academic performance in adolescents and young adults: A meta-analysis. J. Sports Sci. 2020, 38, 2637–2660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Escolano-Perez, E.; Bestue, M. Academic achievement in Spanish secondary school students: The inter-related role of executive functions, physical activity and gender. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 1816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Srinivas, N.S.; Vimalan, V.; Padmanabhan, P.; Gulyás, B. An overview on cognitive function enhancement through physical exercises. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Antunes, H.K.; Santos, R.F.; Cassilhas, R.; Santos, R.V.; Bueno, O.F.; Mello, M.T.d. Reviewing on physical exercise and the cognitive function. Rev. Bras. De Med. Do Esporte 2006, 12, 108–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Dong, Z.; Wang, P.; Xin, X.; Li, S.; Wang, J.; Zhao, J.; Wang, X. The relationship between physical activity and trait anxiety in college students: The mediating role of executive function. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 1009540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Kamijo, K.; Takeda, Y. Regular physical activity improves executive function during task switching in young adults. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2010, 75, 304–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Doucette, M.M.; Sánchez Escudero, J.P.; Rhodes, R.E.; Garcia-Barrera, M.A. Associations of physical activity and history of sports participation with subjective and objective measures of executive functioning in university students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2023, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Li, L.; Yu, Q.; Zhao, W.; Herold, F.; Cheval, B.; Kong, Z.; Li, J.; Mueller, N.; Kramer, A.F.; Cui, J. Physical activity and inhibitory control: The mediating role of sleep quality and sleep efficiency. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Fan, H.; Qi, S.; Huang, G.; Xu, Z. Effect of acute aerobic exercise on inhibitory control of college students with smartphone addiction. Evid. -Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2021, 2021, 5530126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Lambourne, K. The relationship between working memory capacity and physical activity rates in young adults. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2006, 5, 149. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  79. Zhao, Q.; Wang, X.; Li, S.-F.; Wang, P.; Wang, X.; Xin, X.; Yin, S.-W.; Yin, Z.-S.; Mao, L.-J. Relationship between physical activity and specific working memory indicators of depressive symptoms in university students. World J. Psychiatry 2024, 14, 148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Mou, H.; Tian, S.; Yuan, Y.; Sun, D.; Qiu, F. Effect of acute exercise on cognitive flexibility: Role of baseline cognitive performance. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2023, 25, 100522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Barenberg, J.; Berse, T.; Dutke, S. Executive functions in learning processes: Do they benefit from physical activity? Educ. Res. Rev. 2011, 6, 208–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Caamaño-Navarrete, F.; Latorre-Román, P.Á.; Párraga-Montilla, J.; Jerez-Mayorga, D.; Delgado-Floody, P. Selective attention and concentration are related to lifestyle in chilean schoolchildren. Children 2021, 8, 856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Festa, F.; Medori, S.; Macrì, M. Move your body, boost your brain: The positive impact of physical activity on cognition across all age groups. Biomedicines 2023, 11, 1765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Wang, X.; Bakulski, K.M.; Paulson, H.L.; Albin, R.L.; Park, S.K. Associations of healthy lifestyle and socioeconomic status with cognitive function in US older adults. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 7513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Erickson, K.I.; Hillman, C.H.; Kramer, A.F. Physical activity, brain, and cognition. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015, 4, 27–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Li, C.-P.; Liu, X.-H.; Wang, X.-J.; He, L.-P. Trait creativity, personality, and physical activity: A structural equation model. Ann. Palliat. Med. 2023, 12, 14149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Pastor-Vicedo, J.C.; León, M.P.; González-Fernández, F.T.; Prieto-Ayuso, A. Effects of physical activity breaks on cognitive function in undergraduate students: A pilot study. Cogent Soc. Sci. 2024, 10, 2326692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Muñoz Ospina, B.; Cadavid-Ruiz, N. The effect of aerobic exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and executive function in college students. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2024, 26, 100578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Yu, M.; Han, X.; Wang, X.; Guan, R. Effects of Physical Exercise on Executive Functions among College Students in China: Exploring the Influence of Exercise Intensity and Duration. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Zavecz, Z.; Nagy, T.; Galkó, A.; Nemeth, D.; Janacsek, K. The relationship between subjective sleep quality and cognitive performance in healthy young adults: Evidence from three empirical studies. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 4855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Gu, Y.; Scarmeas, N. Dietary patterns in Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive aging. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2011, 8, 510–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Reuter, P.R.; Forster, B.L.; Brister, S.R. The influence of eating habits on the academic performance of university students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2021, 69, 921–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Shakersain, B.; Rizzuto, D.; Wang, H.-X.; Faxén-Irving, G.; Prinelli, F.; Fratiglioni, L.; Xu, W. An active lifestyle reinforces the effect of a healthy diet on cognitive function: A population-based longitudinal study. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Comparison of study variables according to sex.
Table 1. Comparison of study variables according to sex.
Total
(n = 150)
Female
(n = 94)
Male
(n = 56)
p-ValueF-Value
Age (y)21.61 ± 3.0821.28 ± 3.1522.18 ± 2.900.0833.05
Cognitive outcomes
Attention (score)471.1 ± 124.40450.45 ± 123.84506.62 ± 118.310.0116.72
Coordination (score) 362.61 ± 145.54346.64 ± 153.04390.08 ± 128.490.0932.86
Reasoning (score)397.13 ± 162.12384.91 ± 169.76418.16 ± 147.340.2501.33
Memory (score)294.19 ± 158.79270.34 ± 146.20335.22 ± 172.260.0215.45
Perception (score)343.91 ± 98.28335.94 ± 97.26357.62 ± 99.490.2161.54
Inhibition (score)406.90 ± 233.12410.92 ± 241.62400.00 ± 219.910.7930.07
Working memory (score)239.68 ± 191.84213.56 ± 171.72284.62 ± 216.750.0374.45
Cognitive flexibility (score)426.13 ± 235.00410.01 ± 241.97453.86 ± 222.140.2961.10
Lifestyle parameters
Diet quality (score)7.37 ± 3.176.96 ± 3.008.07 ± 3.360.0374.41
Physical activity (h/week)2.01 ± 1.761.66 ± 1.382.61 ± 2.140.00110.83
Screen time (h/day)2.63 ± 0.782.51 ± 0.712.84 ± 0.840.0126.44
Sleep (h/day)6.89 ± 1.526.93 ± 1.616.81 ± 1.360.0360.23
The data are presented as means and standard deviations, with statistical significance set at p < 0.05.
Table 2. Association of cognitive dimension scores with lifestyle variables in university students.
Table 2. Association of cognitive dimension scores with lifestyle variables in university students.
OutcomesAttentionCoordination
Beta
(95% CI)
p-ValueStandardized Beta (SE)Beta
(95% CI)
p-ValueStandardized Beta (SE)
Lifestyle parameters
Diet score−5.74 (−12.35; 0.86)p = 0.088−0.14 (3.34)−6.40 (−14.44; 1.62)p = 0.117−0.13 (4.06)
Physical activity (h/day)24.34 (12.46; 36.22)p = 0.0010.35 (6.00)15.06 (0.62; 29.50)p = 0.0410.18 (7.29)
Screen time (h/day)0.48 (−25.34; 26.31)p = 0.971−0.03 (13.23)12.53 (−18.85; 43.93)p = 0.4310.06 (15.86)
Sleep duration (h/day)−3.41 (−16.67; 9.83)p = 0.611−0.04 (6.70)−3.21 (−19.32; 12.89)p = 0.694−0.03 (8.14)
The data shown represent beta (95% CI), standardized beta, and standard error (SE). Values of p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Model was adjusted for age.
Table 3. Association of cognitive dimensions score with lifestyle variables in university students.
Table 3. Association of cognitive dimensions score with lifestyle variables in university students.
OutcomesReasoningPerception
Beta
(95% CI)
p-ValueStandardized Beta (SE)Beta
(95% CI)
p-ValueStandardized Beta (SE)
Lifestyle parameters
Diet score−8.91 (−17.72; −0.11)p = 0.047−0.17 (4.45)1.32 (−4.06; 6.70)p = 0.6280.04 (2.72)
Physical activity (h/day)20.34 (4.52; 36.17)p = 0.0120.22 (8.00)13.81 (4.14; 23.49)p = 0.0050.25 (4.89)
Screen time (h/day)12.86 (−21.54; 47.27)p = 0.4610.06 (17.39)1.87 (−19.15; 22.91)p = 0.8600.01 (10.63)
Sleep duration (h/day) −10.12 (−27.78; 7.53)p = 0.259−0.09 (8.92)−5.80 (−16.60; 4.98)p = 0.289−0.09 (5.45)
The data shown represent beta (95% CI), standardized beta, and standard error (SE). Values of p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Model adjusted for age.
Table 4. Association of cognitive dimensions score with lifestyle variables in university students.
Table 4. Association of cognitive dimensions score with lifestyle variables in university students.
OutcomesMemoryInhibition
Beta
(95% CI)
p-ValueStandardized Beta (SE)Beta
(95% CI)
p-ValueStandardized Beta (SE)
Lifestyle parameters
Diet score−2.24 (−10.80; 6.32)p = 0.605−0.04 (4.33)−8.57 (−21.45; 4.30)p = 0.190−0.11 (6.50)
Physical activity (h/day)23.01 (7.62; 38.40)p = 0.0040.26 (7.78)21.54 (−1.60; 44.68)p = 0.0680.16 (11.69)
Screen time (h/day)20.55 (−12.91; 54.02)p = 0.2260.10 (16.91)16.71 (−33.60; 67.02)p = 0.5120.05 (25.43)
Sleep duration (h/day)−13.80 (−30.98; 3.36)p = 0.114−0.13 (8.68)−15.98 (−41.80; 9.83)p = 0.223−0.10 (13.04)
The data shown represent beta (95% CI), standardized beta, and standard error (SE). Values of p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Model adjusted for age.
Table 5. Association of cognitive dimension scores with lifestyle variables in university students.
Table 5. Association of cognitive dimension scores with lifestyle variables in university students.
OutcomesWorking MemoryCognitive Flexibility
Beta
(95% CI)
p-ValueStandardized Beta (SE)Beta
(95% CI)
p-ValueStandardized Beta (SE)
Lifestyle parameters
Diet score3.61 (−7.15; 14.37)p = 0.5080.05 (5.44)−4.14 (−16.59; 8.31)p = 0.512−0.05 (6.29)
Physical activity (h/day)13.56 (−5.78; 32.90)p = 0.1680.12 (9.77)45.60 (23.22; 67.99)p = 0.0010.34 (11.31)
Screen time (h/day)5.78 (36.27; 47.84)p = 0.7860.02 (21.25)1.13 (47.54; 49.80)p = 0.9630.00 (24.60)
Sleep duration (h/day)−8.71 (−30.29; 12.87)p = 0.426−0.07 (10.90)−17.27 (−42.24; 7.70)p = 0.174−0.11 (12.62)
The data shown represent beta (95% CI), standardized beta, and standard error (SE). Values of p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Model adjusted for age.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Caamaño-Navarrete, F.; Arriagada-Hernández, C.; Fuentes-Vilugrón, G.; Jara-Tomckowiack, L.; Levin-Catrilao, A.; del Val Martín, P.; Muñoz-Troncoso, F.; Delgado-Floody, P. Healthy Lifestyle Related to Executive Functions in Chilean University Students: A Pilot Study. Healthcare 2024, 12, 1022. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12101022

AMA Style

Caamaño-Navarrete F, Arriagada-Hernández C, Fuentes-Vilugrón G, Jara-Tomckowiack L, Levin-Catrilao A, del Val Martín P, Muñoz-Troncoso F, Delgado-Floody P. Healthy Lifestyle Related to Executive Functions in Chilean University Students: A Pilot Study. Healthcare. 2024; 12(10):1022. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12101022

Chicago/Turabian Style

Caamaño-Navarrete, Felipe, Carlos Arriagada-Hernández, Gerardo Fuentes-Vilugrón, Lorena Jara-Tomckowiack, Alvaro Levin-Catrilao, Pablo del Val Martín, Flavio Muñoz-Troncoso, and Pedro Delgado-Floody. 2024. "Healthy Lifestyle Related to Executive Functions in Chilean University Students: A Pilot Study" Healthcare 12, no. 10: 1022. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12101022

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop