Next Article in Journal
Cross-National Comparisons of Internalizing Problems in a Cohort of 8952 Adolescents from Five European Countries: The EU NET ADB Survey
Next Article in Special Issue
Physical Fitness Perception and Physical Education Enjoyment in 11- to 12-Year-Old Children
Previous Article in Journal
The Relationship between Eosinophil Density in the Colonic Mucosa and Eosinophil Blood Count in Children: A Cross-Sectional Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Reference Values of Absolute and Relative Handgrip Strength in Chilean Schoolchildren with Intellectual Disabilities
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Brief Report

The Effects of Chinese Parenting Belief on Preschoolers’ Temperament and Secure Attachment

Department of Early Childhood Education, National Pingtung University, Pingtung 900391, Taiwan
Children 2023, 10(1), 9; https://doi.org/10.3390/children10010009
Submission received: 24 November 2022 / Revised: 17 December 2022 / Accepted: 19 December 2022 / Published: 21 December 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Early Childhood Education Development)

Abstract

:
In the past, there were a few studies investigating the effects of parenting belief on preschoolers’ temperament and secure attachment. There were some inconsistencies; some effects were also unclear. A total of 2164 parents of three-year-old preschoolers were selected for a panel study named National Longitudinal Study of Child Development and Care. At first, confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to examine construct validity of Chinese parenting belief, preschoolers’ temperament, and secure attachment. Consecutively, the effects of Chinese parenting belief on preschoolers’ temperament and secure attachment were investigated through structural equation modeling. Results showed: (1) construct validity of Chinese parenting belief (composed of Guan, Jiao, achievement, and Chi beliefs), preschoolers’ temperament (composed of extraversion, effortful control, and negative affection temperament) and secure attachment were good. (2) The Guan belief showed no effects on all temperaments and secure attachment. (3) Only Jiao belief contributed to the development of extraversion, effortful control, negative affection, and secure attachment. (4) The Achievement belief had a detrimental effect on the development of secure attachment, but it had no effects on others. (5) The Chi belief can increase the development of negative affection temperament, but it had no effects on the rest.

1. Introduction

The effects of parenting style on several outcomes of children (e.g., achievement) is a widely studied topic [1,2]. In contrast, research focusing on parenting beliefs, a moderator between the relationships between parenting style and outcomes, have received relatively less attention. The effects of parenting style may be also vary from culture to culture [3,4]. It means that whether parenting style may take effect is conditioned simultaneously on parenting beliefs and culture. In this regard, the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on several outcomes (e.g., temperament and attachment) were less clear, and it may deserve more discussions.
Parenting beliefs refer to the intentions, expectations, and values toward children while parents are rearing and educating their children. Four Chinese parenting beliefs are salient in Chinese culture. They are the discipline belief (namely “Guan”), the guidance belief (namely “Jiao”), the achievement belief, and the shame belief (namely “Chi”). The Guan belief refers to parents’ belief that control is necessary for cultivating children’s positive behaviors [5,6,7,8]. Chinese parents gave more punishments, reprimands, admonishments, and emphasized absolute obedience [9,10,11,12]. It has been demonstrated that guan will significantly increase children’s anti-social behaviors [13]. The Jiao belief refers to parents tend to adopt autonomous and supportive guidance or instruction to support children’s positive development [14,15,16]. It was found that this belief may be beneficial for children to perform behaviors corresponding to parents’ expectations [17]. The achievement belief refers to parents’ expecting or even demanding that their children get better grades or perform better in school to honor them or their family [18,19]. The Chi belief refers to parents usually reminding their children they will feel shameful for their children’s inappropriate behaviors [20,21,22], and it was considered to have more negative effects on children’s psychological wellbeing [23].
Researchers demonstrated three types of temperament for preschoolers; they are extraversion, effortful control, and negative affection, respectively [24,25]. Extraversion temperament reflects the preferred amount of physical activity (e.g., prefer dynamic activities more than static activities), reaction intensity (e.g., actively engage in new activities), and the tendency of approach and avoidance of children (e.g., more like to play with different children). Effortful control temperament reflects children’s adaptability, distraction, the span of attention, and persistence in a certain activity. Negative affection temperament reflects children’s response threshold and emotionality (e.g., subject to anger to others’ verbal provocation). Parenting beliefs are influential factors for temperament [26].
By adopting the framework of parenting style and incorporating Western samples, there was found a positive relationship between authoritarian belief and negative affection temperament [27,28]. Similarly, it was also indicated that punitive parenting increases negative affectivity [29]. Another study further found that authoritative belief may positively predict infants’ extraversion temperament [30,31] and effortful control temperament [28,30]. It was found that both authoritative belief and permissive belief positively predicted effortful control temperament [32,33]. There were some evidence that indicated that authoritative belief has no effect on negative affectivity [34,35]. By adopting the same framework of parenting style but incorporating Chinese or Eastern samples, it was found that there was a positive relationship between the authoritative parenting of Chinese parents and the extraversion temperament, but authoritarian parenting of Chinese parents was negatively related to extraversion temperament [36] and positively predicted effortful control temperament [37]. In another study, the authoritarian parenting of Chinese and Indian parents was found to negatively predict effortful control temperament, and positive predict negative affection temperament [38,39]. However, the authoritative parenting of Chinese parents was unable to predict these two kinds of temperament [38]. However, these effects of parenting on temperament are based on the framework of parenting style which was constructed based on Western culture. It is less clear whether the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on temperament would be similar to the effects of parenting style.
By adapting another framework closed to Chinese parenting belief, there were some evidences showed that the “Guan” belief may promote children to develop extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament and negative affection temperament [40]. However, it was also found that the Guan belief of the Chinese mother was unrelated to [41] or negatively related to negative affection temperament [42], whereas the Jiao belief was demonstrated to reduce negative affection temperament [43], but its effects on extraversion temperament and effortful control temperament were unclear. In addition, the achievement belief was negatively related to extraversion temperament, but positive related to effortful control temperament [44]. On the contrary, some researchers found that parental educational involvement is protective factor in academic adjustment (e.g., the feeling of belonging in the educational environment and prosocial behaviors) [45]. It suggested that the achievement belief may be beneficial for promoting the development of adaptive temperaments (i.e., extraversion temperament and effortful control temperament) and for inhibiting the development of maladaptive temperament (i.e., negative affection temperament). However, its direct effects of the “achievement” belief on negative affection temperament were also far from clear. Finally, the Chi belief was unrelated to extraversion temperament [46], and effortful control temperament [37,46], but it was positively related to negative affection temperament [46,47].
Parenting belief is also influential for secure attachment. Attachment play an important role in one’s socio-emotional development, and it refers to the affect connections between preschoolers and main caregivers; secure attachment in the infant stage increasingly formed intensive positive affection to regulate their emotions and behaviors based on the responsive reactions of their caregivers [48,49]. There are three styles of attachment; they are secure, insecure ambivalent, and insecure avoidant attachment, respectively. Secure attachment is developed when parents are responsive to children’s physical or psychological needs. Insecure ambivalent attachment is produced when parents are selectively responded to children’s certain needs regardless of others. Insecure avoidant attachment is occurred when parents have no response to children’s needs. Secure attachment may influence cognition, emotion, life accommodation, and intimate relationships with others in adolescent period [50,51], while the other two attachment styles may lead to several maladaptive outcomes (e.g., higher negative emotionality) [52]. The development of secure attachment may be challenged when young children initially encounter an environmental transition from the family to the preschool setting. It is particularly important to clarify the protective and detrimental effects of parenting belief on the development of secure attachment. Researchers found a positive relationship between authoritative belief and secure attachment [53], and between the “Guan” belief and secure attachment [42]. Similarly, a meta-analysis including a Chinese sample also demonstrated that the Jiao belief may be beneficial for developing secure attachment [54]. However, some researchers found that both “Guan” belief and “Jiao” belief were unrelated to secure attachment [55]. Few studies investigate the effect of the “achievement” belief on secure attachment. Inferring indirectly from empirical evidence, it indicates that the lower parents possess achievement belief the more responsiveness they give to their children, and the responsiveness is beneficial to the development of secure attachment. It suggests that achievement belief may be negatively related to secure attachment [18]. Similarly, Xu et al. demonstrated that Chi belief has a positive relationship with insecure attachment; it implied that Chi belief may also have a negative relationship with secure attachment [22].
Apparently, findings regarding the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on both temperaments and secure attachment were not only inconclusive, but some were unclear. Further investigations are needed to contribute to our understanding about better Chinese parenting beliefs for cultivating children’s adaptive temperaments and secure attachment. Consequently, the main purposes of present study were twofold:
  • To investigate the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on preschoolers’ temperaments.
  • To clarify the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on preschoolers’ secure attachment.

2. Methodology

2.1. Participants

Data is retrieved from National Longitudinal Study of Child Development and Care (NLSCDC) database in Taiwan. This panel study was reviewed and approved by the National Taiwan University and National Taiwan Normal University Subjects Institutional Review Boards. One of the main purposes of NLSCDC is to investigate the effects of the family psychological environment on the development of cognition and social emotion for young children aged from birth to age 8. Stratified two-stage probability proportional to size sampling was applied in this panel study. All the 358 administration areas in Taiwan were taken as primary sampling units, and they were integrated to form 19 stratifications: 7 in the northeast, 7 in the middle, 3 in the southeast, and 2 in the west of Taiwan. Individuals were served as the secondary sampling units, and they were selected based on household registration information. There were 1140, 709, 303 and 48 parents paired with their three-year-old children selected from the northeast, the middle, the southeast, and the west of Taiwan, respectively. A total of 2164 representative parents and their three-year-old children (1113 boys and 1051 girls) were included in this study [56]. About 4/5 mothers and 1/5 fathers participated to complete the questionnaires. There were about 1/6 fathers and 1/8 mothers who held a master’s degree. Participants were informed that all of their responses were kept confidential, and their treatments in the preschools would not be influenced by their participation. Three-year-old children were selected because this age is the first year for most of children enroll in informal school settings. They experienced a transition from the family to a totally strange environment, and they have to adapt themselves to various new conditions. Consequently, they may experience considerable emotional disturbances, and their socio-emotional development may be affected to some extent. This study was concerned with investigating how parenting beliefs exert effects on their temperament and secure attachment.

2.2. Instruments

There were three instruments used in this study. They were developed by and were used in NLSCDC to understand the effects of family psychological environment on socio-emotional development as one of the main purposes of this panel study, which corresponded to the main purposes of this study. All instruments were validated by reliability analysis, factor analysis, and item response analysis [56].

2.2.1. Chinese Parenting Belief Scale

There are nine items in the Chinese parenting belief scale; it is composed of four subscales: the Guan belief, Jiao belief, Achievement belief, and Chi belief subscales, respectively. There are three items for measuring Guan belief. A sample item is: chide preschooler will let them progress. There are two items for measuring Jiao belief; a sample item is: Preschooler should look at me when I am talking with him/her. There are two items for measuring Achievement belief; a sample item is: I feel sense of achievement once they perform better on leaning. Two items are designed to measure Chi belief; a sample item is: I should feel shame if preschooler is unruly outside. A four-point Likert’s scale design was used, and parents were required to choose one from the four options on a 1 (“very disagree”) to 4 (“very disagree”) scale. The internal consistency coefficients for the four subscales are 0.70, 0.72, 0.73 and 0.74, respectively.

2.2.2. Preschooler Temperament Scale

There are 11 items in the preschooler temperament scale; it is composed of three subscales: extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, and negative affection temperament subscales, respectively. There are three items for measuring extraversion temperament. A sample item is: Comparing to static activities, preschooler is more like dynamic activities. There are four items for measuring effortful control temperament; a sample item is: Preschooler consistently engaged in an activity or played a toy for a long time. There are four items for measuring negative affection temperament; a sample item is: Preschooler get angry easily. A five-point Likert’s scale design was used, and parents were required to choose one from the five options on a 1 (“never”) to 5 (“always”) scale. The internal consistency coefficients for three subscales are 0.66, 0.63 and 0.60, respectively.

2.2.3. Preschooler Secure Attachment Scale

There are four items in the preschooler secure attachment scale; a sample item is: Preschooler will look for me when I am not staying with him/her. A five-point Likert’s scale design was used; parents were required to choose one from the five options on a 1 (“never”) to 5 (“always”) scale. Preschoolers who score lower on this scale are highly securely attached to their parents. The internal consistency coefficient is 0.66.

2.3. Analysis

Mplus 8.0 is used to analyze the measurement model and the structural model by using structural equation modeling (SEM). Several goodness of fit indices were used to evaluate model fit. They are χ2, RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation), CFI (comparative fit index), TLI (Tucker–Lewis index), and SRMR (standardized root mean square residual). Non-significant χ2 values indicated model fit to the data, but it is easily affect by a larger sample size. Therefore, other indices are mainly used to evaluate model fit. RMSEA ≤ 0.06, and CFI and TLI ≥ 0.95 was considered a model that fit the data well. A value of 0.06 < RMSEA ≤ 0.08, and CFI and TLI ≥ 0.90 was considered a model fit that was acceptable [57,58]. Confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) for three constructs were conducted in advance. CFAs were conducted by using MLR (Maximum likelihood with robust standard errors). After that, SEM was implemented to investigate the structural relationships among latent variables. In the hypothesized SEM model, there are four exogenous latent variables (Guan belief, Jiao belief, Achievement belief and Chi belief) and four endogenous latent variables (extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, negative affection temperament and secure attachment). The effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on preschoolers’ temperament and secure attachment were investigated in this model.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary Analysis

Item means of Chinese parenting beliefs ranged from 2.40 to 3.23 (SD ranged from 0.56 to 0.76; correlation coefficients ranged from −0.05 to 0.57). Means of items for preschooler’s temperament ranged from 2.76 to 4.29 (SD ranged from 0.82 to 1.32; correlation coefficients ranged from −0.04 to 0.43). Means of items for preschooler’s secure attachment ranged from 3.49 to 4.62 (SD ranged from 0.66 to 1.29; correlation coefficients ranged from 0.18 to 0.31).

3.2. The Goodness of Fit of Measurement Model

The goodness of fit of measurement model was evaluated to assure that items correctly loaded on their latent constructs before investigating the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on temperament and secure attachment. The measurement model for Chinese parenting belief was composed of four latent variables. CFA results for this model showed that each of the fit indices met the criteria for a good-fitting model: χ2(21, N = 2164) = 127.59, p < 0.05, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.048 (90% CI ranged from 0.041 to 0.057). Intercorrelation coefficients among these four latent variables ranged from 0.26 to 0.64 (95% CI ranged from 0.20 to 0.70, 1 is not included in this range). CFA results for the measurement model of Chinese parenting beliefs demonstrated a good construct validity and latent variables in this model were distinguishable. Consequently, this measurement model could be further used to investigate their effects to temperament and secure attachment.
Measurement model for preschooler’s temperament composed of three latent variables. CFA results for this model showed that each of the fit indices generally met the criteria for an acceptable model: χ2(2, N = 2164) = 235.25, p < 0.05, CFI = 0.93, TLI = 0.91, RMSEA = 0.047 (90%CI ranged from 0.041 to 0.053). Intercorrelation coefficients among these four latent variables ranged from 0.12 to 0.51 (95% CI ranged from 0.04 to 0.58, 1 is not included in this range). CFA results for the measurement model for preschooler’s secure attachment also met the criteria for a good-fitting model: χ2(41, N = 2164) = 22.25, p < 0.05, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.90, RMSEA = 0.068 (90% CI ranged from 0.045 to 0.095). Correlations among all latent variables ranged from 0.12 to 0.64 (ps ranged from 0.00 to 0.02 < 0.05).
In general, CFA results for these two measurement models of preschooler’s temperament and secure attachment also demonstrated good construct validities, and latent variables in the measurement model of temperament were distinguishable. Consequently, these measurement models could be used in further analysis.
The overall structural model, including three measurement models for each latent variable, generally met the criteria for a good-fitting model: χ2(224, N = 2164) = 834.03, p < 0.05, CFI = 0.93, TLI = 0.91, RMSEA = 0.035 (90% CI ranged from 0.033 to 0.038). It suggested that the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on temperament and secure attachment could be investigated based on this good fitted structural model. Table 1 shows the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on temperament and secure attachment (Figure 1 shows the graphical representation).

3.2.1. The Effects of “Guan” Belief on Temperament and Secure Attachment

As it can be seen from Table 1, Guan belief shows no effects on extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, negative affection temperament, and secure attachment (β1, β2, β3, and β4 are −0.01, −0.04, 0.04 and 0.03, respectively, ps = 0.91, 0.43, 0.43, and 0.56 > 0.05).

3.2.2. The Effects of “Jiao” Belief on Temperament and Secure Attachment

In contrast to above findings, Jiao belief shows positive effects on extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, negative affection temperament, and secure attachment (β5, β6, β7 and β8 are 0.16, 0.20, 0.11 and 0.25, respectively, ps = 0.00, 0.00, 0.01 and 0.00 < 0.05).

3.2.3. The Effects of “Achievement” Belief on Temperament and Secure Attachment

Achievement belief shows no effects on extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, and negative affection temperament (β9, β10, and β11 are 0.02, −0.03, and 0.00, respectively, ps = 0.76, 0.68 and 0.95 > 0.05). However, it has a negative effect on secure attachment (β12 is −0.14, p = 0.03 < 0.05).

3.2.4. The Effects of “Chi” Belief on Temperament and Secure Attachment

Chi belief shows no effects on extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, and secure attachment (β13, β14, and β16 are 0.04, −0.04, and 0.02, respectively, ps = 0.43, 0.36 and 0.75 > 0.05). However, it has positive effect on negative affection temperament (β15 is 0.09, p = 0.04 < 0.05).

4. Discussion and Conclusion

It has been argued that parenting has considerably differences between Western and Eastern country [59], and Chinese parenting belief cannot be fully understood by the theoretical framework of parenting style which was developed based on Western samples in the Western countries [60]. Chinese parenting beliefs were constructed according to related researches [5,6,14,15,18,19,20,21,22,41]. Results of CFAs are clearly showed that Chinese parenting belief are composed of four dimensions as expectation, they are Guan belief, Jiao belief, Achievement belief, and Chi belief, respectively. Moreover, they are different constructs which may reflect different social meanings in Chinese society. It indicated that the construed Chinese parenting beliefs was supported by present study. In addition, although temperament construct is proposed grounded in Western culture, its contents are also supported by Chinese preschooler sample.
The Guan belief was found to have no effects on preschooler’s temperaments and secure attachment. These findings are inconsistent with those of the researchers who adapted parenting type as a research framework. Specifically, the meaning of the Guan belief is closed to authoritarian belief, but it showed neither a positive relationship with negative affection temperament [25,30], nor a negative relationship with extraversion temperament [36], or effortful control temperament [38]. In contrast, these findings are very similar to those studies that adapted Chinese parenting beliefs as a research framework and incorporated Chinese parents as samples. Specifically, researchers found that the Guan belief was unrelated to negative affection temperament and secure attachment [41,55]. Present findings showed that there are two null effects of the Guan belief on extraversion temperament and effortful control temperament, which expand our current understanding and use it to encourage parents to discard this parenting belief when they are educating and caring for their children. In addition, these results only implied that there were no temporary effects of the Guan belief on temperament and secure attachment. However, traditionally, the guan belief implied that parents knew the importance of punishments, reprimands, and admonishment for cultivating the conformity to society. It may direct children to fear of engaging in social activities to avoid doing something wrong, to persist in learning activities that are demanding, and to inhibit their negative affections [5]. Moreover, harsh discipline may be also detrimental for cultivating secure attachment. Consequently, in the long-term, it was unclear whether it contributes to the formation of effortful control temperament and has negative effects on extraversion, negative affection temperament, and secure attachment.
In contrast to the effects of the Guan belief, the Jiao belief was found to have positive effects on preschooler’s temperaments and secure attachment. These finding were relatively consistent with former researches [30,32,33,36,53,54]. However, the positive effect of the Jiao belief on negative affection temperament was unexpected, and it contradicted former research findings which indicated that the Jiao belief may contribute to reduce the influences of negative affection temperament [43]. It may be implied that there are moderators (e.g., environmental transition) between the Jiao belief and negative affection temperament. For example, 3-year-old preschoolers had just started to enroll in kindergarten in Taiwan, and they were unfamiliar to this new learning environment. They may produce emotional fluctuations when they situate in this strange environment, and this context may exert considerable positive effects on negative affection temperament. Consequently, the expected negative effect of the Jiao belief on negative affection temperament was overturned by the possibly positive contextual effect. These findings contributed to make conclusions on the positive effects of the Jiao belief on extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, and secure attachment. In addition, it also suggested further studies were needed to clarify the effect on negative affection temperament.
The achievement belief was found to have no effects on extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, and negative affection temperament. It contradicted former findings that indicated its negative relationship with extraversion temperament and its positive relationship with effortful control temperament [44]. In Chinese society, parents may long to see their children become successful and have a bright future to get superior social status to make their parents feel proud or honor their clan [61], and this belief may direct children to focus on cognitive performances by using various motivational strategies to approach success (e.g., persistence on a learning task to complete it). It is expected that the achievement belief may contribute to the formation of effortful control temperament, but this effect was not found. It may be implied that achievement belief may need more time to exert its positive effect on effortful control temperament, or it may be also suggested that this effect is significant in the achievement context (e.g., formal schooling) involving more social comparison or competition. Moreover, it is reasonable that there are no effects of achievement belief on extraversion and negative affection temperament because the achievement belief is only expected to direct children’s cognitive performances rather than social or affection domain. The negative effect of the achievement belief on secure attachment was consistent with the hypothesis which was inferred from Chen and Chou’s finding [18]. These findings simultaneously contributed to expand our understanding of the unclear relationship between the achievement belief and negative affection temperament, to provide information on the negative effect of achievement on secure attachment, and to encourage future researchers to investigate the contradictory results.
Finally, the Chi belief was found to have no effects on extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, and secure attachment. It was quite consistent with former researchers who found that the Chi belief was unrelated to extraversion temperament and effortful control temperament [46], but it was positively related to negative affection temperament [46,47]. Chinese parents were heavily influenced by Confucian traditions; it suggested that Chinese parents taught their children to conduct themselves so as to not disgrace to their parents and clan. It makes children sensitive to social norms and feel shame when they fail to obey these rules [37]. Theoretically, the Chi parenting may guide parents to demand their children to adapt to social rules, and in turn to cultivate effortful control temperament. Unexpectedly, the Chi belief has no effect on effortful control temperament. It implied that this effect may take time to be revealed. In addition, it has been demonstrated that there were emotional contagions among people [62], and the positive effect of the Chi belief on negative affection temperament suggested that Chinese parents may transmit negative emotional cues to their children unconsciously. Consequently, this belief imperceptibly fosters children’s negative affection temperament. Furthermore, it was found that one of the results is not consistent with the hypothesis which inferred from Xu et al.’s study, suggesting the Chi belief may exert a negative effect on secure attachment [22]. These findings may contribute to understand the effects of the Chi belief on extraversion temperament, effortful control temperament, and negative affection temperament. It will also help future studies to further clarify the effects of Chi on secure attachment.
In general, practically, the Jiao belief may be encouraged with caution because it was not clear whether it may contribute to promoting negative affection temperament. In contrast, parents’ achievement belief and Chi belief may be discouraged to prevent their potential negative effects on adaptive temperament (e.g., effortful control temperament) and/or secure attachment, and to promote possible positive effects on maladaptive temperament (e.g., negative affection temperament) and/or secure attachment.

5. Limitations

Present findings may reflect short-term phenomena due to the nature of the cross-sectional study. It was unclear whether these effects may disappear or reveal in any expected or unexpected directions as time goes on. For example, the null effects of the Guan belief on negative affection temperament may be temporary and may be turned into negative in the future. More studies were clearly needed to focus on the longitudinal effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on the development of temperaments and secure attachment. In addition, some interest findings (e.g., the positive effect of the Jiao belief on negative affection temperament) may be conditioned on other factors (e.g., the contextual transition) which was not expected and included in this study. These potential factors should be considered in future studies to further clarify present findings.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study, due to publicly available data with no protected health information being used in this article.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

This article used publicly available data with no protected health information.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank the reviewers for their comments that assisted in the revising of this original manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

There is no potential conflict of interest to be disclosed. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Brown, L.; Iyengar, S. Parenting Styles: The Impact on Student Achievement. In Family Factors and the Educational Success of Children; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2010; pp. 11–35. [Google Scholar]
  2. Kuppens, S.; Ceulemans, E. Parenting Styles: A Closer Look at a Well-Known Concept. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2019, 28, 168–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Sangawi, H.S.; Adams, J.; Reissland, N. The Effects of Parenting Styles on Behavioral Problems in Primary School Children: A Cross-Cultural Review. Asian Soc. Sci. 2015, 11, 171–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Smetana, J.G. Current Research on Parenting Styles, Dimensions, and Beliefs. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 15, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Chao, R.K. Beyond Parental Control and Authoritarian Parenting Style: Understanding Chinese Parenting through the Cultural Notion of Training. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 1111–1119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Luo, W.; Aye, K.M.; Hogan, D.; Kaur, B.; Chan, M.C.Y. Parenting Behaviors and Learning of Singapore Students: The Mediational Role of Achievement Goals. Motiv. Emot. 2013, 37, 274–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Chen, J.J.; Sun, P.; Yu, Z. A Comparative Study on Parenting of Preschool Children between the Chinese in China and Chinese Immigrants in the United States. J. Fam. Issues 2017, 38, 1262–1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sun, L.; Fu, Z.; Lee, P.; Gong, X. Chinese Parenting Beliefs in the Intergenerational Transmission of Parental Psychological Control amongst Chinese Families with Adolescents. Curr. Psychol. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chan, S.M.; Bowes, J.; Wyver, S. Chinese Parenting in Hong Kong: Links among Goals, Beliefs and Styles. Early Child Dev. Care 2009, 179, 849–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chuang, S.S.; Su, Y. Do We See Eye to Eye? Chinese Mothers’ and Fathers’ Parenting Beliefs and Values for Toddlers in Canada and China. J. Fam. Psychol. 2009, 23, 331–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Darling, N.; Steinberg, L. Parenting Style as Context: An Integrative Model. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 113, 487–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Wu, P.; Robinson, C.C.; Yang, C.; Hart, C.H. Similarities and Differences in Mothers’ Parenting of Preschoolers in China and the United States. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2002, 26, 481–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Gámez-Guadix, M.; Straus, M.A.; Carrobles, J.A.; Muñoz-Rivas, M.J.; Almendros, C. Corporal Punishment and Long-Term Behavior Problems: The Moderating Role of Positive Parenting and Psychological Aggression. Psicothema 2010, 22, 529–536. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  14. Cheah, C.S.; Leung, C.Y.; Zhou, N. Understanding “Tiger Parenting” through the Perceptions of Chinese Immigrant Mothers: Can Chinese and U.S. Parenting Coexist? Asian Am. J. Psychol. 2013, 4, 30–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Kim, S.Y.; Shen, Y.; Huang, X.; Wang, Y.; Orozco-Lapray, D. Chinese American Parents’ Acculturation and Enculturation, Bicultural Management Difficulty, Depressive Symptoms, and Parenting. Asian Am. J. Psychol. 2014, 5, 298–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Lee, E.H.; Zhou, Q.; Ly, J.; Main, A.; Tao, A.; Chen, S.H. Neighborhood Characteristics, Parenting Styles, and Children’s Behavioral Problems in Chinese American Immigrant Families. Cultur. Divers. Ethnic Minor. Psychol. 2014, 20, 202–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Crockenberg, S.; Litman, C. Autonomy as Competence in 2-Year-Olds: Maternal Correlates of Child Defiance, Compliance, and Self-Assertion. Dev. Psychol. 1990, 26, 961–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Chen, M.T.; Chou, L.T. Parents’ High Expectations of Children to Become Successful? The Parenting Belief Mediating Effect between Socioeconomic Status and Home Learning Environment. Bull. Educ. Psychol. 2020, 52, 365–388. [Google Scholar]
  19. Wang, H.H. From “Taking Pride in Me” to “Proving My Own Worth”: An Exploratory Study of the Impact of College Students’ Perceived Parental Expectations on Their Patterns of Career Choices. J. Guid. Couns. 2005, 37, 79–97. [Google Scholar]
  20. Lin, W.Y.; Wang, C.W. Chinese Parents’ Parenting Beliefs: Harsh Discipline or Maltreatment. Indig. Psychol. Res. Chin. Soc. 1995, 3, 2–92. [Google Scholar]
  21. Liu, T.H. Child-Rearing Ideas of College-Educated Mothers. J. Hsin Chu Teach. Coll. 2001, 14, 335–405. [Google Scholar]
  22. Xu, Y.; Farver, J.A.M.; Zhang, Z.; Zhang, Q.; Yu, L.; Cai, B. Mainland Chinese Parenting Styles and Parent–Child Interaction. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2005, 29, 524–531. [Google Scholar]
  23. Helwig, C.C.; To, S.; Wang, Q.; Liu, C.; Yang, S. Judgments and Reasoning about Parental Discipline Involving Induction and Psychological Control in China and Canada. Child Dev. 2014, 85, 1150–1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Putnam, S.P.; Rothbart, M.K. Development of Short and Very Short Forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. J. Pers. Assess. 2006, 87, 102–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Rothbart, M.K.; Ahadi, S.A.; Hershey, K.L.; Fisher, P. Investigations of Temperament at Three to Seven Years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1394–1408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Mills, S.A. Parents’ Beliefs about Children’s Cognitive Development. Child Dev. 1988, 59, 259–285. [Google Scholar]
  27. Laukkanen, J.; Ojansuu, U.; Tolvanen, A.; Alatupa, S.; Aunola, K. Child’s Difficult Temperament and Mothers’ Parenting Styles. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2014, 23, 312–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Checa, P.; Abundis-Gutierrez, A. Parenting and Temperament Influence on School Success in 9–13 Year Olds. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Zubizarreta, A.; Calvete, E.; Hankin, B.L. Punitive Parenting Style and Psychological Problems in Childhood: The Moderating Role of Warmth and Temperament. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2018, 28, 233–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Belsky, J.; Fish, M.; Isabella, R.A. Continuity and Discontinuity in Infant Negative and Positive Emotionality: Family Antecedents and Attachment Consequences. Dev. Psychol. 1991, 27, 421–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hu, Q.X.; Feng, Q.S. Parenting Style and Prosocial Behaviour among Chinese Preschool Children: A Moderation Model. Early Child Dev. Care 2022, 192, 1444–1461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kochanska, G.; Murray, K.T.; Harlan, E.T. Effortful Control in Early Childhood: Continuity and Change, Antecedents, and Implications for Social Development. Dev. Psychol. 2000, 36, 220–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Bernier, A.; Carlson, S.M.; Whipple, N. From External Regulation to Self-Regulation: Early Parenting Precursors of Young Children’s Executive Functioning. Child Dev. 2010, 81, 326–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Wittig, S.M.O.; Rodriguez, C.M. Emerging Behavior Problems: Bidirectional Relations between Maternal and Paternal Parenting Styles with Infant Temperament. Dev. Psychol. 2019, 55, 1190–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Wittig, S.M.O.; Rodriguez, C.M. Interaction between Maternal and Paternal Parenting Styles with Infant Temperament in Emerging Behavior Problems. Infant Behav. Dev. 2019, 57, 1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Porter, C.L.; Hart, C.H.; Yang, C.; Robinson, C.C.; Olsen, S.F.; Zeng, Q.; Olsen, J.A.; Jin, S. A Comparative Study of Child Temperament and Parenting in Beijing, China and the Western United States. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2005, 29, 541–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Huang, C.Y.; Cheah, C.S.L.; Lamb, M.E.; Zhou, N. Associations between Parenting Styles and Perceived Child Effortful Control within Chinese Families in the United States, the United Kingdom, and Taiwan. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2017, 48, 795–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lee, E.H.; Zhou, Q.; Eisenberg, N.; Wang, Y. Bidirectional Relations between Temperament and Parenting Styles in Chinese Children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2012, 37, 57–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Sahithya, B.R.; Raman, V. Parenting Style, Parental Personality, and Child Temperament in Children with Anxiety Disorders—A Clinical Study from India. Indian J. Psychol. Med. 2021, 43, 382–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Spitzer, M.; Fischbacher, U.; Herrnberger, B.; Gron, G.; Fehr, E. The Neural Signature of Social Norm Compliance. Neuron 2007, 56, 185–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Huang, S.C.; Lee, L.W. A Follow up Study of the Relationship of Maternal Parenting Behaviors and Young Children’s Independent Completion on Task. Bull. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 42, 209–227. [Google Scholar]
  42. Ang, C.S.; Sin, A.B.J. Retrospective Reports of Perceived ‘Guan’ Parenting: Relationships to Adult Attachment Styles, Emotion Regulation, and Self-Esteem. J. Genet. Psychol. 2021, 182, 163–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Kyrios, M.; Prior, M. Temperament, Stress and Family Factors in Behavioural Adjustment of 3–5-Year-Old Children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1990, 13, 67–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Pinquart, M.; Ebeling, M. Parental Educational Expectations and Academic Achievement in Children and Adolescents-a Meta-Analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 32, 463–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Serna, C.; Martínez, I. Parental Involvement as a Protective Factor in School Adjustment among Retained and Promoted Secondary Students. Sustainability 2019, 11, 7080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Hughes, S.O.; Shewchuk, R.M. Child Temperament, Parent Emotions, and Perceptions of the Child’s Feeding Experience. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2012, 6, 64–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Cook, D.R. Shame, Attachment, and Addictions: Implications for Family Therapists. Contemp. Fam. Ther. 1991, 13, 405–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ainsworth, M.S. Infant-Mother Attachment. Am. Psychol. 1979, 34, 932–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1969; Volume 1: Attachment. [Google Scholar]
  50. Feeney, J.A.; Noller, P. Attatchment Style as a Predictor of Adult Romantic Relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 58, 281–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Laible, D. Attachment with Parents and Peers in Late Adolescence: Links with Emotional Competence and Social Behavior. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2007, 43, 1185–1197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Delgado, E.; Serna, C.; Martínez, I.; Cruise, E. Parental Attachment and Peer Relationships in Adolescence: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2022, 19, 1064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Karavasilis, L.; Doyle, A.B.; Karavasilis, D. Associations between Parenting Style and Attachment to Mother in Middle Childhood and Adolescence. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2003, 27, 153–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Huang, S.M.; Chou, L.T.; Yeh, M.F. Maternal Factors and the Attachment of Children in Taiwan: A Meta-Analysis. Bull. Educ. Psychol. 2008, 40, 39–62. [Google Scholar]
  55. Su, C.W.; Kung, M.C. Mother’s Attachment Experiences and Her Child-Rearing Practice as Related to Attachment of Preschool Children. Bull. Educ. Psychol. 1994, 42, 1–33. [Google Scholar]
  56. Chang, C.J. Kids in Taiwan: National Longitudinal Study of Child Development and Care (KIT): KIT-M36 at 36 Months Old (D00168) [Data File]; Survey Research Data Archive, Center for Survey Research, Research Center for Humanities and Social Sciences, Academia Sinica: Taipei City, Taiwan, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  57. Hu, L.-T.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Wang, J.; Wang, X. Structural Equation Modeling: Application Using Mplus; Wiley: West Sussex, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  59. Wang, Q.; Chang, L. Parenting and Child Socialization in Contemporary China. In The Oxford Handbook of Chinese Psychology; Bond, M.H., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2009; pp. 53–67. [Google Scholar]
  60. Lu, H.J.; Chang, I. Parenting and Socialization of Only Children in Urban China: An Example of Authoritative Parenting. J. Genet. Psychol. 2013, 174, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hwang, K.K. The Contemporary Transformation of Confucian Values: Theoretical Values and Empirical Research. Indig. Psychol. Res. Chin. Soc. 1995, 3, 276–338. [Google Scholar]
  62. Olszanowski, M.; Wróbel, M.; Hess, U. Mimicking and Sharing Emotions: A Re-Examination of the Link between Facial Mimicry and Emotional Contagion. Cogn. Emot. 2020, 34, 367–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The graphical representation of the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on temperament and secure attachment. * p < 0.05.
Figure 1. The graphical representation of the effects of Chinese parenting beliefs on temperament and secure attachment. * p < 0.05.
Children 10 00009 g001
Table 1. The structural coefficients of Chinese parenting beliefs on preschooler’s temperament and secure attachment.
Table 1. The structural coefficients of Chinese parenting beliefs on preschooler’s temperament and secure attachment.
Structural PathsCoefficients
Guan” belief→Extraversion temperament (β1)−0.01
Guan” belief→Effortful control temperament (β2)−0.04
Guan” belief→Negative affection temperament(β3)0.04
Guan” belief→Secure attachment (β4)0.03
Jiao” belief→Extraversion temperament (β5)0.16 *
Jiao” belief→Effortful control temperament (β6)0.20 *
Jiao” belief→Negative affection temperament (β7)0.11 *
Jiao” belief→Secure attachment (β8)0.25 *
Achievement” belief→Extraversion temperament (β9)0.02
Achievement” belief→Effortful control temperament (β10)−0.03
Achievement” belief→Negative affection temperament (β11)0.00
Achievement” belief→Secure attachment (β12)−0.14 *
Chi” belief→Extraversion temperament (β13)0.04
Chi” belief→Effortful control temperament (β14)−0.04
Chi” belief→Negative affection temperament(β15)0.09 *
Chi” belief→Secure attachment (β16)0.02
Note: Coefficients are completely standardized coefficients. * p < 0.05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wu, C.C. The Effects of Chinese Parenting Belief on Preschoolers’ Temperament and Secure Attachment. Children 2023, 10, 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10010009

AMA Style

Wu CC. The Effects of Chinese Parenting Belief on Preschoolers’ Temperament and Secure Attachment. Children. 2023; 10(1):9. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10010009

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wu, Chung Chin. 2023. "The Effects of Chinese Parenting Belief on Preschoolers’ Temperament and Secure Attachment" Children 10, no. 1: 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10010009

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop