Next Article in Journal
How Speededness of a Reasoning Test and the Complexity of Mental Speed Tasks Influence the Relation between Mental Speed and Reasoning Ability
Previous Article in Journal
Achievement in Fundamental Movement Skills, Spatial Abilities, and Mathematics among Lower Key Stage 2 Children
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Time Use and Cognitive Achievement among Adolescents in China: Depression Symptoms as Mediators

1
Graduate School of Education, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China
2
School of Education, Tianjin University, Tianjin 300350, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Intell. 2023, 11(5), 88; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050088
Submission received: 21 March 2023 / Revised: 28 April 2023 / Accepted: 4 May 2023 / Published: 6 May 2023

Abstract

:
Everyone’s time is limited, and there is competition between different aspects of time use; this requires comprehensive consideration of the effects of different aspects of time use on cognitive achievement in adolescents. This study uses a dataset of 11,717 students from a nationally representative large-scale survey project conducted in 2013 to 2014 to clarify the relationship between time use (including working on homework, playing sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping) and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents, and explores the mediating role of depression symptoms in the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement. The results of the correlation analysis show that the average daily time spent on homework, playing sports, and sleeping is significantly positively correlated with cognitive achievement (p < 0.01), while time spent surfing the Internet and watching TV are significantly negatively correlated with cognitive achievement (p < 0.01). The results of the mediating effect model show that depression symptoms play a mediating role in the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents. Specifically, time spent playing sports (indirect effect = 0.008, p < 0.001) and sleeping (indirect effect = 0.015, p < 0.001) have a positive effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators; time spent on homework (indirect effect = −0.004, p < 0.001), surfing the Internet (indirect effect = −0.002, p = 0.046), and watching TV (indirect effect = −0.005, p < 0.001) have a negative effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators. This study contributes to the understanding of the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents.

1. Introduction

Adolescents’ cognitive achievement not only strengthens individual human capital and future competitiveness in the labor market, but also enhances social innovation capacity and promotes economic growth (Cameron and Heckman 1998; Hunter 1986; McLeod and Kaiser 2004; Singh et al. 2019). It is necessary to clarify the mechanisms that contribute to the improvement of adolescents’ cognitive achievement. A growing number of policymakers and parents worldwide are paying attention to the impact of time use on well-being and mental health among adolescents (Borga 2019; Hunt and McKay 2015; Wight et al. 2009). If adolescents allocate a significant proportion of their discretionary time to positive and purposeful constructive activities, this will be beneficial to their healthy growth (Zill 1995). Some studies have analyzed adolescents’ time use. For instance, 81% of schoolchildren aged 11–17 years worldwide do not meet the World Health Organization’s recommendation of one hour of moderate to vigorous physical activity per day, and in China, the figure is 84.3% (Bull et al. 2020; Guthold et al. 2020). In the United States, about 45% of adolescents spend excessive time in front of electronic screens (Onyeaka et al. 2022). However, everyone’s time is limited, and there is competition between different aspects of time use. Considering the impact of only one aspect of time use on cognitive development may not be comprehensive, and all other aspects of time use need to be considered to understand the role of each aspect of time use on adolescents’ cognitive achievement. Thus, clarifying the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement will facilitate the effective utilization of time for adolescent development, and can provide a reference for targeted public health interventions. In this study, cognitive achievement is defined as a comprehensive measure of logical thinking and problem-solving abilities based on standardized cognition tests. Considering the fixed schedules of Chinese adolescents during school hours, we investigate time use outside of school, including time spent on homework, playing sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping.
It is worth analyzing the impact of time use on cognitive achievement mechanisms, as this may shed light on the frequent negative emotions experienced by adolescents in real life (X. Liu et al. 2019b; Pekrun et al. 2002; Pekrun 2006). Depression symptoms are associated with adolescents’ time use and cognitive achievement, and may serve as mediating factors in explaining the relationship between the two. Unfortunately, depression symptoms among adolescents are becoming increasingly prevalent (Cao and Liu 2022; Gao et al. 2022; X. Liu et al. 2019a), with incidence rates rising from 5% at the start of adolescence to as high as 20% at the end of adolescence (Thapar et al. 2012). According to the World Health Organization, one in seven 10- to 19-year-olds worldwide suffers from mental health problems (WHO 2021). The COVID-19 pandemic has exacerbated this trend, with negative time use, such as decreased physical exercise and increased screen time (Neville et al. 2022), leading to cognitive decline and intensified depression symptoms in adolescents (Y. Liu et al. 2021; Xin et al. 2020). Despite this, there is limited research examining the mediating effect of depression symptoms on the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement, particularly in a Chinese context. This study empirically examines the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents and the underlying mechanisms.

1.1. The Relationship between Time Use and Cognitive Achievement

Studies have explored the relationship between different aspects of time use and cognitive achievement. Most of the findings suggest that there is a negative relationship between time spent on the Internet and watching television and cognition. This may be because prolonged exposure to the Internet and television can cause changes in the structure and function of the brain, with potentially detrimental effects on higher-order cognition. Additionally, the variety and transience of stimuli on the Internet and television can easily lead to emotional fluctuations and distraction, resulting in cognitive decline (Kuss and Griffiths 2012; Mills 2014). A large-scale survey of American adolescents has revealed a negative correlation between screen time spent on television and social media and cognitive achievement (Walsh et al. 2020). A national survey of 17,076 American adolescents concluded that excessive screen time was detrimental to cognition, increasing the likelihood of cognitive difficulties, such as attention problems and memory impairment, by almost 1.3 times compared to those who did not overuse screens (Onyeaka et al. 2022). Jürges and Khanam (2021) found that educational activities were beneficial to cognitive achievement, while screen time, including watching TV, playing computer games, and social media use, increased internalization problems in Australian adolescents. Islam et al. (2020) discovered that addictive tendencies toward the Internet and electronic games were negatively associated with academic performance in 1704 Australian children aged 11–17 years, and recommended reducing Internet and gaming time through parental supervision and self-discipline. In addition, Internet use and gaming can also significantly disrupt adolescents’ sleep time (King et al. 2013) and increase the risk of mental disorders and dangerous behaviors (Green et al. 2020; Kliestik et al. 2020; Lăzăroiu et al. 2020; Rikkers et al. 2016).
Previous literature has shown that homework time is an important factor in learning ability (Kumar and Choudhury 2021). Parents and teachers tend to believe that spending time of homework can benefit children’s cognitive development (Cooper et al. 2000). Some research has found a significant positive correlation between these two variables. A study based on 8–12-year-old adolescents in India showed that increasing homework time significantly improved the cognitive achievement of students who were lagging behind, suggesting that increasing homework time is one of the key ways to reduce the quality gap between private and public schools in India (Kumar and Choudhury 2021). Reinforcement theory proposes that continuous stimulation leads to changes in the brain’s cognitive and behavioral responses, increasing the likelihood of that behavior. Homework is an effective means of consolidating what students have learned, and spending time on homework helps to reinforce what they have learned in the classroom, thus having a positive effect on cognitive development (Skinner 1954). This has been demonstrated in studies of adolescents in Ethiopia, Vietnam, and India (Borga 2019). There is also evidence that the positive correlation between the two increases with grade level (Cooper and Valentine 2001; Cooper et al. 2006). However, there is currently no consensus on the effect of homework time on cognition, and some studies have found a negative correlation between the two. Chang et al. (2014) found that time spent on homework was negatively correlated with course outcomes based on a survey of 2342 students, possibly because smarter children require less time to complete their homework.
Most studies suggest that there is a significant positive correlation between sleep time and cognitive development. Adequate sleep time can significantly improve cognitive achievement in adolescents, including performance in tasks related to executive function and multiple cognitive domains (Jones and Harrison 2001; Lo et al. 2016). A meta-analysis of 61 studies based on 71 populations showed that sleep restriction has a significant negative effect on executive function, long-term memory, and attention in cognitive performance (Lowe et al. 2017). A survey of 55 adolescents in the Netherlands found that longer sleep time significantly ameliorated visual spatial processing skills (Dewald-Kaufmann et al. 2013). The results from 8323 American adolescents showed that insufficient sleep (less than 9 h) has lasting negative effects on cognition and brain function (Yang et al. 2022). There are also studies suggesting that the correlation between the two is very weak (Astill et al. 2012) or even negative. For example, some surveys of elderly people in Japan and the Bronx found that respondents with longer sleep time had worse cognitive achievement (Kondo et al. 2021; Schmutte et al. 2007).
Insufficient physical activity time presents a critical challenge for today’s adolescents worldwide. In total, 84.6% of Filipino adolescents do not meet the recommended daily minimum of one hour of physical exercise (Cagas et al. 2022). Only 27% of Thai adolescents achieve the daily threshold of 60 min for moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (Widyastari et al. 2022). Prior literature supports a significant positive association between physical activity time and cognitive achievement. Increasing physical activity time can enhance physical and mental functioning, leading to improved cognition (Hillman et al. 2008; Tomporowski et al. 2008). Children who participate in physical activity have larger basal ganglia and hippocampal brain volumes than inactive children, which is associated with superior cognitive control and memory performance (Chaddock-Heyman et al. 2014). A dearth of physical activity can lead to chronic diseases (such as diabetes and obesity) and mental health problems and impose a substantial economic burden on society (Colditz 1999; Hillman et al. 2008). Some studies have produced inconsistent conclusions, with a review of 58 intervention studies finding no conclusive evidence of the beneficial impact of physical activity time on cognitive achievement (Singh et al. 2019). This was also similar to the findings of a survey of Australian fifth–sixth grade primary school students (Dwyer et al. 1983).
Previous literature also suggests possible gender and grade differences in time use (Lloyd et al. 2008). For instance, a survey of American adolescents showed that 8th graders spent more time watching TV and playing video games than 10th graders, and girls spent less time playing video games than boys (Tang and Patrick 2018). Twenge and Martin (2020) found significant gender differences in digital media use among American and British youth, with females spending more time on the Internet and social media, while males allocated more time to gaming and electronic devices. A study of 237 college students found that younger college students had shorter sleep time, while female college students had poorer sleep quality than males (Tsai and Li 2004). A study in Canada found that there were significant gender differences in weekend time use and that adolescents had less leisure time as their grade level increased (Hilbrecht et al. 2008).
In conclusion, existing research lacks a comprehensive analysis of adolescents’ time use across different aspects. Time is a finite resource, and different aspects of time use compete with each other. A narrow focus on a solitary aspect of time use is not effective, and it is crucial to analyze the impact of multiple aspects on adolescents’ cognitive achievement. According to the characteristics and life trajectories of adolescents, their time use typically includes activities such as homework, sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping. It is therefore imperative to investigate the impacts of these common aspects of time use on cognitive achievement among adolescents and give full consideration to gender and grade differences. This will help parents and teachers to improve their parenting and teaching methods effectively.

1.2. The Mediating Role of Depression Symptoms

Previous literature has demonstrated a correlation between time use and cognitive achievement, yet little is known about the underlying mechanism. School achievement and psychological well-being are both crucial for adolescents, and a range of factors affect these outcomes, such as parental support (Moè et al. 2020), teacher enthusiasm (Moe et al. 2021), and also depression symptoms (Moè 2015). The present study focuses mostly on the latter variable by examining time use on the one hand, and cognitive achievement on the other. Depression symptoms may serve as the mediating factor that helps to explain this relationship. Control-value theory recognizes that emotions play a crucial role in cognitive processing and behavior. Positive emotions are often seen as a driving force for learning and development, resulting in improved learning efficiency and academic performance. Conversely, negative emotions have a detrimental effect, hindering behavior, learning outcomes, and personal growth (Pekrun et al. 2002; Pekrun 2006). Depression symptoms are the most common negative emotional expression among adolescents worldwide. The World Health Organization estimates that the burden of depression will exceed all other diseases by 2030 (Thapar et al. 2012). The negative impact of depression symptoms on adolescents is becoming increasingly apparent (Gao et al. 2020; X. Liu et al. 2022b), with the incidence among Chinese adolescents rising significantly to 57% following the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic (Chen et al. 2021). Unfortunately, few studies have focused on the mediating role of depression symptoms (Lim and Jeong 2022). A study of 562 Puerto Ricans over the age of 60 found that depression symptoms played a complete mediating role in the relationship between perceived daily discrimination and cognitive function (K. Wang et al. 2022). Relevant conclusions drawn from previous studies in other populations cannot be directly applied to the adolescent population, so it is of significance to examine the mediating effect of depression symptoms while taking into account the unique characteristics of adolescence.
Research into the relationship between depression symptoms and cognition has accumulated a substantial body of literature, with the preponderance of evidence indicating that depression symptoms are significant predictors of cognitive decline (Conradi et al. 2011; Lee et al. 2012; Wagner et al. 2012), with 90% of depression patients experiencing impaired cognition (Hollon et al. 2006). A minority of studies have found little association between the two (Goulabchand et al. 2022; Grant et al. 2001). Studies investigating the impact of time allocation for tasks, such as homework and extracurricular activities, on the relationship between depression symptoms and cognition are relatively limited. A survey of Australian adolescents revealed that those with emotional issues or high levels of psychological distress spent the most time on the Internet or playing games (Rikkers et al. 2016). Neuroscientific evidence suggests that chronic sleep deprivation is a risk factor for the onset and progression of depression (Y.-Q. Wang et al. 2015). A study of 82 young adults found that Facebook use was associated with a decrease in subjective well-being. The more frequently people used Facebook, the lower their moment-to-moment experiences of happiness and life satisfaction tended to be (Kross et al. 2013). A review of adolescent mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic found that spending too much time on the Internet and social media can lead to increased symptoms of depression. This highlights how excessive screen time can be detrimental to the mental health of vulnerable adolescents (Oliveira et al. 2022). Although not all studies have confirmed this finding, a synthesis of 25 reviews reveals that the majority of research interprets the association between social media and mental health as weak or inconsistent (Valkenburg et al. 2022). A study of 79 suburban Florida high school students found that those with depression symptoms spent less time on homework and achieved lower average grades (Field et al. 2001). A significant positive association was observed between homework time and depression symptoms among Chinese primary school students in grades 2–6 (Xiao et al. 2022). However, the above literature only examined the relationship between individual time use and depression symptoms separately. Meanwhile, some individual studies have also explored the relationship between time use, cognition, and depression symptoms in specific domains. A survey of 3724 adolescents found that cognitive function moderated the effect of sleep time on depression symptoms (Zhou et al. 2022). A study of 280 retired individuals showed that self-reported depression symptoms were associated with slow stepping reaction time, a connection that could be explained by underlying cognitive impairment (Kvelde et al. 2010).

1.3. Aims and Hypotheses

Given the high prevalence of depression symptoms among adolescents and the supporting studies, depression symptoms may play an important mediating role in the relationship between time use and cognitive development. However, there is a paucity of research that comprehensively covers the impact of different aspects of time use on adolescent cognitive achievement, and even fewer studies have empirically examined the mediating effect of depression symptoms between the two. Furthermore, there is a lack of evidence based on in-depth examination of adolescent populations in China. Additionally, the conclusions of previous studies are often based on small sample surveys, which calls into question the generalizability of the findings. What are the characteristics of time use among Chinese adolescents? Do depression symptoms play a mediating role between time use and cognitive achievement? To address these gaps in the literature, this study aims to clarify the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents and the mediating effect of depression symptoms.
According to the relevant literature and theory, a theoretical research framework was developed (as shown in Figure 1). Based on the literature, the theoretical research framework, and our research objectives, we propose the following research hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1. 
There is a significant positive correlation between adolescent average daily time spent on homework, playing sports, and sleeping and cognitive achievement, and a significant negative correlation between average daily time spent surfing the Internet and watching TV and cognitive achievement.
Hypothesis 2. 
Depression symptoms play a mediating role in the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement among adolescents.
Hypothesis 2.1. 
Time spent on homework has a hindering effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators (more homework time → increases depression symptoms → hinders cognitive achievement).
Hypothesis 2.2. 
Time spent playing sports has a promoting effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators (more time spent playing sports → decreases depression symptoms → boosts cognitive achievement).
Hypothesis 2.3. 
Time spent surfing the Internet has a hindering effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators (more time spent surfing the Internet → increases depression symptoms → hinders cognitive achievement).
Hypothesis 2.4. 
Time spent watching TV has a hindering effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators (more TV time → increases depression symptoms → hinders cognitive achievement).
Hypothesis 2.5. 
Sleep time has a promoting effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators (more sleep time → decreases depression symptoms → boosts cognitive achievement).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The data used in this study were sourced from the 2013–2014 academic year of the China Education Panel Survey (CEPS), which was designed and implemented by the China Survey and Data Center at Renmin University of China. The CEPS is a large-scale, nationally representative survey project. The survey selected 112 schools and 438 classes of seventh and ninth grade students from across the country using a multistage, probability-proportional and scale-based cluster sampling method, stratified by average education level and population proportion. The survey questionnaire was comprehensive, including students’ basic personal information, in-school learning experiences, and extracurricular activities, providing relevant data support for understanding time use, cognition, and depression symptoms among Chinese adolescents. Through data cleaning, a final sample of 11,717 students was retained for empirical analysis. Of these, 5724 were female and 5993 were male, accounting for 48.85% and 51.15% of the sample, respectively; 6161 were in seventh grade and 5556 were in ninth grade, accounting for 52.58% and 47.42% of the sample, respectively.

2.2. Measures

Time use: The time use studied in this research specifically involved the following five aspects: time spent on homework, playing sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping. We specifically used the questions “How much on average did you spend on the following extracurricular activities (doing homework time; playing sports time; surfing on the Internet time; watching TV time; sleep time) from Monday to Friday last week?” and “How much time on average did you spend on the following extracurricular activities (doing homework time; playing sports time; surfing on the Internet time; watching TV time; sleep time) last weekend?” from the CEPS student questionnaire. By performing a calculation, we measured the average daily time use (in minutes) of adolescents in the abovementioned aspects. Based on real-life situations, outliers were removed from each variable: time spent on homework, playing sports, surfing the Internet, and watching TV averaged over 300 min per day; sleep time averaged over 720 min per day; and total time averaged over 1440 min per day.
Depression symptoms: We used the CEPS student questionnaire questions “Did you feel blue in the past seven days”, “Did you feel unhappy in the past seven days”, “Did you feel that life was meaningless in the past seven days”, and “Did you feel sad in the past seven days” to measure depression symptom levels. Each item involved a 5-point assessment (ranging from “1 = never” to “5 = always”). Then, we averaged the scores for these four questions, with higher average scores indicating a higher level of depression symptoms in the student. The reliability coefficient α of the depression symptoms scale was 0.8137.
Cognitive achievement: The CEPS designed a cognition test for students that measures their problem-solving ability and logical thinking without involving rote knowledge. The test covers three dimensions, including language, graphics, arithmetic, and logic, with 20 questions in the seventh grade test and 22 questions in the ninth grade test. In our study, we primarily utilized the data obtained from the first round, wherein we employed the number of questions answered correctly by seventh and ninth grade students in a cognitive assessment test to assess their cognitive achievement (T1). The values for this measure ranged from 0 to 20 for the seventh grade cohort and 0 to 22 for the ninth grade cohort. A higher average score on this assessment test indicated a higher level of cognitive achievement among the participants. Moreover, to address the causal inference limitations inherent in cross-sectional data, we incorporated the cognitive achievement (T2) obtained during the second round of data collection as a robustness check for our conclusions.

2.3. Data Analysis

First, we utilized Stata to analyze and present descriptive statistical data of the variables of Chinese adolescents’ time use (including working on homework, playing sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping), depression symptoms, and cognitive achievement, as well as gender and grade differences among these variables. Then, we conducted bivariate correlation analysis between the variables of time use, depression symptoms, and cognitive achievement. Finally, we ran Mplus and empirically examined the mediating effect of depression symptoms on the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement through a mediation model.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 presents the mean, standard deviation, minimum, maximum, skewness, and kurtosis of the variables time use, depression symptoms, and cognitive achievement, as well as the gender and grade differences between these variables. The descriptive statistics show that the average score for cognitive achievement (T1) is 10.388 and the average score for depression symptoms is 2.054, indicating normal levels of cognition and depression symptoms. The average daily time spent on homework by Chinese adolescents is 126.401 min, the average daily time spent playing sports is 41.022 min, the average daily time spent surfing the Internet is 34.466 min, the average daily time spent watching TV is 48.139 min, and the average daily sleep time is 472.533 min.
Independent sample t-tests reveal that there are significant gender differences in depression symptoms (t = 0.067, p < 0.01) and time spent on homework (t = 14.024, p < 0.01), playing sports (t = −10.874, p < 0.01), surfing the Internet (t = −13.918, p < 0.01), watching TV (t = −4.397, p < 0.01), and sleeping (t = −5.690, p < 0.01), but not in cognitive achievement (T1) (t = −0.087, p = 0.210). Meanwhile, there are significant grade differences in cognitive achievement (T1) (t = 1.818, p < 0.01), depression symptoms (t = −0.143, p < 0.01), and time spent on homework (t = −19.640, p < 0.01), playing sports (t = −1.458, p < 0.1), watching TV (t = 5.785, p < 0.01), and sleeping (t = 41.280, p < 0.01), but not in time spent surfing the Internet (t = −1.250, p = 0.148).
To gain an intuitive understanding of the time use of adolescents of different genders and grades, we also present gender-specific and grade-specific bar charts of different types of time use among Chinese adolescents. As shown in Figure 2, female adolescents (mean = 133.574) spend an average of 14.024 min more per day on homework than male adolescents (mean = 119.55). Female and male adolescents spend, on average, 35.46 min and 46.334 min, respectively, playing sports per day; 27.347 min and 41.266 min, respectively, surfing the Internet per day; 45.889 min and 50.287 min, respectively, watching TV per day; and 469.643 and 475.333 min, respectively, sleeping per day. It can be seen that female adolescents spend less time on average each day playing sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping than male adolescents. As shown in Figure 3, seventh grade and ninth grade students spend an average of 50.882 min and 45.097 min watching TV each day and an average of 492.127 min and 450.847 min sleeping each day, respectively. This demonstrates that seventh grade students spend more time watching TV and sleeping each day than ninth grade students. It is worth noting that ninth grade students (mean = 136.728) spend, on average, almost 20 min more per day on homework than seventh grade students (mean = 117.088). In addition, ninth grade students spend more time on average each day playing sports and surfing the Internet (mean = 41.789; mean = 35.124) than seventh grade students (mean = 40.331; mean = 33.863). In view of these results, we considered gender and grade as control variables in the subsequent analysis.

3.2. Correlation Analysis

Table 2 presents the correlation coefficients between time use (time spent on homework time, playing sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping), depression symptoms, and cognitive achievement. The results show that there was a significant negative correlation (p < 0.01) between depression symptoms and cognitive achievement (T1 and T2) in Chinese adolescents. There was a significant positive correlation (p < 0.01) between the average daily time spent on homework and cognitive achievement (T1 and T2), as well as between time spent on homework and depression symptoms. There was also a significant positive correlation (p < 0.01) between time spent playing sports and cognitive achievement (T1 and T2) and a significant negative correlation (p < 0.01) between time spent playing sports and depression symptoms. A significant negative correlation (p < 0.01) was found between time spent surfing the Internet and cognitive achievement (T1 and T2), and a significant positive correlation (p < 0.01) was found between time spent surfing the Internet and depression symptoms. A significant negative correlation (p < 0.01) was found between time spent watching TV and cognitive achievement (T1 and T2), and a significant positive correlation (p < 0.01) was found between time spent watching TV and depression symptoms. There was a significant positive correlation (p < 0.01) between sleep time and cognitive achievement (T1) and a significant negative correlation (p < 0.01) between sleep time and depression symptoms. This verifies hypothesis 1. Additionally, aside from the insignificant correlation between time spent playing sports and both surfing the Internet (p = 0.496) and watching TV (p = 0.993), all other variables exhibit significant interrelationships.

3.3. Mediation Model

We controlled for gender and grade and used a mediation model and bootstrap method to examine the mediating effect of depression symptoms between time use and cognitive achievement (T1). The model fit indices are good (RMSEA = 0.063, CFI = 0.914, SRMR = 0.039). Table 3 presents the results of the model. In the mediation model, the direct effect of time spent on homework on cognitive achievement (T1) is 0.138 (p < 0.001), with a confidence interval of [0.122, 0.156], indicating a significant direct effect; the indirect effect of depression symptoms between the two is −0.004 (p < 0.001), with a confidence interval of [−0.006, −0.002], indicating a significant indirect effect. The direct effect of time spent playing sports on cognitive achievement (T1) is 0.038 (p < 0.001), with a confidence interval of [0.019, 0.056], indicating a significant direct effect; the indirect effect of depression symptoms between the two is 0.008 (p < 0.001), with a confidence interval of [0.006, 0.011], indicating a significant indirect effect. The direct effect of time spent surfing the Internet on cognitive achievement (T1) is 0.001 (p = 0.899), with a confidence interval of [−0.017, 0.019], indicating an insignificant direct effect; the indirect effect of depression symptoms between the two is −0.002 (p = 0.046), with a confidence interval of [−0.005, 0.000], indicating a significant indirect effect. The direct effect of time spent watching TV on cognitive achievement (T1) is −0.117 (p < 0.001), with a confidence interval of [−0.133, −0.101], indicating a significant direct effect; the indirect effect of depression symptoms between the two is −0.005 (p < 0.001), with a confidence interval of [−0.008, −0.003], indicating a significant indirect effect. The direct effect of sleep time on cognitive achievement (T1) is −0.013 (p = 0.202), with a confidence interval of [−0.032, 0.006], indicating an insignificant direct effect; the indirect effect of depression symptoms between the two is 0.015 (p < 0.001), with a confidence interval of [0.011, 0.019], indicating a significant indirect effect. In addition, both gender (p = 0.025) and grade level (p < 0.001) significantly affect cognitive achievement (T1).
The above results indicate that depression symptoms play a mediating role in the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement (T1) among Chinese adolescents, supporting Hypothesis 2. Specifically, time spent playing sports and sleeping have a positive effect on cognitive achievement (T1) when using depression symptoms as mediators; time spent on homework, surfing the Internet, and watching TV have a negative effect on cognitive achievement (T1) when using depression symptoms as mediators.
To further address the challenge of making causal inferences with cross-sectional data (Baiden et al. 2019), we conducted a robustness check on our research findings using the data from the second wave of cognitive achievement tests. The purpose of this was to demonstrate that our current results are still valid in longitudinal data, as shown in Table 4. By comparing the results from Table 3 and Table 4, we observe that the findings regarding the mediating effect are consistent, thus indicating the reliability of our current results.

4. Discussion

This study comprehensively examines the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents in common activities, including working on homework, playing sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping. Importantly, we clarify the mediating effect of depression symptoms between time use and cognitive achievement. Our findings provide new valuable evidence in a Chinese context that adds to the literature.
The results of our descriptive analysis reveal that, excluding time spent sleeping, adolescents in China spend the most time on homework. The insufficient daily physical activity time among Chinese adolescents (less than one hour) also deserves a high level of attention. This aligns with trends found in previous large-scale global surveys based on approximately 150 countries and the latest reports from other countries, such as the Philippines and Thailand (Cagas et al. 2022; Guthold et al. 2020; Widyastari et al. 2022). Given the importance of physical exercise, it is necessary to take measures to motivate adolescents to actively participate in physical exercise as soon as possible, thus contributing to the happiness and healthy growth of adolescents globally. Additionally, our analysis of the gender and grade differences in time use revealed that compared to male adolescents, female adolescents invested more time in homework, and correspondingly, spent less time sleeping, playing sports, surfing the Internet, and watching TV. This not only reflects differences in time utilization preferences between genders, but also highlights the tendency of individuals to allocate their limited time toward what they value most. Furthermore, this trend can also be observed in China, where female students tend to have higher academic engagement, which may be due to the traditional cultural notion of “emphasizing boys over girls” in certain regions. To achieve high grades and gain recognition, female students may have to work harder through diligent study to secure their futures. Additionally, the largest change in time use among Chinese adolescents from grades 7 to 9 is a significant increase in average daily homework time, accompanied by an almost 1 h reduction in sleep time. In China, where exam-oriented Confucian culture is highly revered, adolescents in middle school face intense academic competition. Increasing grade level often translates to a heavier academic load (Fu et al. 2022), especially as the importance of homework becomes more apparent (Cooper and Valentine 2001; Kumar and Choudhury 2021). Thus, a growing number of middle school students in developing countries have to sacrifice sleep time to improve their learning time and boost their competitiveness within the educational system (Lloyd et al. 2008).
The scarcity of time makes people value the positive effects of utilizing it even more. Our correlational analysis found that the average daily time spent doing homework, playing sports, and sleeping were significantly positively correlated with cognitive achievement, while time spent surfing the Internet and watching TV were significantly negatively correlated with cognitive achievement. Specifically, the relationship between homework time and cognitive achievement aligns with previous research (Borga 2019; Cooper and Valentine 2001; Cooper et al. 2006; Kumar and Choudhury 2021) and accommodates the expectations of teachers and parents (Cooper et al. 2000). According to Skinner’s reinforcement theory, the increase in homework time and the continual repetition of content and knowledge leads to strengthened learning and improved cognitive development and academic outcomes (Skinner 1954). It is worth noting that there is also a significant positive correlation between homework time and depression symptoms, which, in turn, has a significant negative correlation with cognitive achievement. The findings of this study are consistent with previous research on the association between homework time and depression symptoms in Chinese primary school students (Xiao et al. 2022), indicating a common phenomenon in China. This relationship may serve as an indicator of stress in students. If they feel the need to spend more time on homework, it may be due to concerns about their future, meeting parental expectations, and worries about their opportunities for higher education in China, where competition for college positions is fierce. Chronic stress of this type can lead to symptoms of depression. Therefore, the relationship between homework time and cognitive achievement needs further investigation, considering depression symptoms as a mediating factor. The correlation between time spent playing sports and cognitive achievement confirms previous research (Hillman et al. 2008; Tomporowski et al. 2008) but contradicts findings from Singh et al. (2019) and Dwyer et al. (1983). The relationship between sleep time and cognitive achievement is consistent with most previous research (Dewald-Kaufmann et al. 2013; Jones and Harrison 2001; Lo et al. 2016; Lowe et al. 2017; Yang et al. 2022), indicating that adequate sleep benefits the cognition of adolescents. This differs from the conclusions of Kondo et al. (2021) and Schmutte et al. (2007), which may be due to their focus on older individuals. This suggests that the effects of time use on cognitive achievement may vary among different groups and deserves attention from future researchers. In addition, the conclusions regarding the relationship between time spent surfing the Internet and watching TV and cognitive achievement are consistent with previous studies (Islam et al. 2020; Jürges and Khanam 2021; Onyeaka et al. 2022; Walsh et al. 2020), demonstrating that excessive screen time among Chinese adolescents, as with other countries, can harm cognitive development.
Moreover, this study also confirms the mediating role of depression symptoms between time use and cognitive development in Chinese adolescents. Based on the mediating effect model, and after controlling for gender and grade, we found that different aspects of time use had varying effects on the path of cognitive development. Time spent on homework, playing sports, and watching TV could either directly impact cognitive development or indirectly impact it when using depression symptoms as mediators, whereas time spent surfing the Internet and sleeping only impacts cognitive development when using depression symptoms as mediators. Notably, the impact of depression symptoms as mediating variables on cognitive development varies between different time utilizations. Working on homework, surfing the Internet, and watching TV had a negative impact on cognitive development when using depression symptoms as mediators, while playing sports and sleeping had a positive impact on cognitive development when using depression symptoms as mediators. These findings, combined with our previous results, suggest that increasing time spent on homework, surfing the Internet, and watching TV increases the level of depression symptoms and, in turn, reduces cognitive achievement. On the other hand, increasing time spent playing sports and sleeping reduces depression symptoms and enhances cognitive achievement. These findings highlight the complexity of the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement in adolescents and, to some extent, support the role of negative emotions in affecting learning behaviors and cognitive performance (Pekrun et al. 2002; Pekrun 2006), as proposed in control-value theory. Thus, the negative effects of adolescent negative emotions should not be ignored (X. Liu et al. 2022a, 2023b; Thapar et al. 2012).
This study has important implications and references for educational practice. First, under the premise that time is limited and different time uses mutually compete, our results reveal that time use pertaining to academics, physical health, and sleep may be more sensible and effective for promoting cognitive achievement among adolescents. It should be noted that the results of our mediating model show that more time spent on homework does not necessarily equate to better cognitive achievement. We found a significant positive correlation between homework time and depression symptoms, a significant negative correlation between depression symptoms and cognitive achievement, and an adverse effect of time spent on homework on cognitive development using depression as a mediator. This suggests that more homework time leads to higher levels of depression and hinders cognitive development. Future studies may consider conducting randomized intervention trials to further explore the most scientific and reasonable academic tasks and completion times that schools should set. This may also be an urgent issue that policy makers, educators, and parents in other countries with similar education competition and limited resources to China should understand (Cao et al. 2023; X. Liu et al. 2023a). Second, our findings also suggest that the occurrence of depression symptoms among adolescents may be an important signal and a key mediator connecting adolescent behavior and cognition. This also makes up for the lack of attention to the mediating role of depression symptoms in previous literature (Lim and Jeong 2022; K. Wang et al. 2022). Third, in order to enhance the alleviation of depression symptoms among adolescents in educational settings, it is imperative that education management departments, schools, and parents work together to attach great importance to the psychological health development of adolescents worldwide. This can be achieved by fostering a relaxed learning atmosphere through joint efforts between families and schools. It is preferable to identify students at risk of depression as early as possible and to take timely action to improve adolescents’ mental health and cognitive achievement, thereby promoting their comprehensive growth.

5. Limitations

First, limited by the database, the correlations between time use, cognitive achievement, and depression symptoms were obtained based on cross-sectional data. In the future, if there are data for tracking relevant variables, further analysis of the longitudinal relationships between variables will be considered. Second, based on the items available in the database, we used four questions to assess the depression symptom levels of adolescents. It would be beneficial to validate these findings using other depression symptoms scales in future studies. Third, due to the limitations of the database, we were unable to obtain specific details of adolescent time use, such as the types of sporting activities they engage in; these deserve further exploration by relevant researchers in the future. Fourth, data on time use, cognitive achievement, and depression symptoms all came from self-reported reports in student questionnaires, so there may be some measurement bias. Fifth, we conducted a mediation analysis on the large-scale survey data using methods similar to those used in previous analyses. Although we considered a complex survey design for estimating standard errors in the analyses, the current data did not support its inclusion. This highlights an area for future research and improvement. Finally, although the conclusions of this study are instructive for parents, teachers, or other groups concerned with related issues, the relatively small effect sizes obtained from such a large sample should be interpreted with caution. Nevertheless, we believe that the evidence is still very valuable.

6. Conclusions

First, the average daily time spent on homework, playing sports, and sleeping are significantly positively correlated with cognitive achievement, while the time spent surfing the Internet and watching TV are significantly negatively correlated with cognitive achievement.
Second, time spent playing sports and sleeping have a positive effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators.
Third, time spent on homework, surfing the Internet, and watching TV have a negative effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, X.C. and X.L.; methodology, X.C.; writing—original draft preparation, X.C. and X.L.; writing—review and editing, X.C. and X.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Tianjin University.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data used in this study were obtained from the following website: http://ceps.ruc.edu.cn/ (accessed on 7 January 2023).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Astill, Rebecca G., Kristiaan B. Van der Heijden, Marinus H. Van Ijzendoorn, and Eus J. W. Van Someren. 2012. Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: A century of research meta-analyzed. Psychological Bulletin 138: 1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Baiden, Philip, Savarra K. Tadeo, and Kersley E. Peters. 2019. The association between excessive screen-time behaviors and insufficient sleep among adolescents: Findings from the 2017 youth risk behavior surveillance system. Psychiatry Research 281: 112586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Borga, Liyousew Gebremedhin. 2019. Children’s own time use and its effect on skill formation. The Journal of Development Studies 55: 876–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bull, Fiona C., Salih S. Al-Ansari, Stuart Biddle, Katja Borodulin, Matthew P. Buman, Greet Cardon, Catherine Carty, Jean-Philippe Chaput, Sebastien Chastin, and Roger Chou. 2020. World Health Organization 2020 guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour. British Journal of Sports Medicine 54: 1451–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Cagas, Jonathan Y., Marla Frances T. Mallari, Beatriz A. Torre, Mary-Grace D. P. Kang, Yves Y. Palad, Roselle M. Guisihan, Maria Isabela Aurellado, Chessa Sanchez-Pituk, John Guiller P. Realin, and Marvin Luis C. Sabado. 2022. Results from the Philippines’ 2022 report card on physical activity for children and adolescents. Journal of Exercise Science & Fitness 20: 382–90. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cameron, Stephen V., and James J. Heckman. 1998. Life cycle schooling and dynamic selection bias: Models and evidence for five cohorts of American males. Journal of Political Economy 106: 262–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Cao, Xiaojie, and Xin-Qiao Liu. 2022. Artificial intelligence-assisted psychosis risk screening in adolescents: Practices and challenges. World Journal of Psychiatry 12: 1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Cao, Xiaojie, Siduo Ji, and Xinqiao Liu. 2023. Educational Inequity and Skill Formation Differences Experienced by Floating Rural Students in the Process of Urbanization: A Case Study from a School Perspective. Education Sciences 13: 131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chaddock-Heyman, Laura, Charles H. Hillman, Neal J. Cohen, and Arthur F. Kramer. 2014. III. The importance of physical activity and aerobic fitness for cognitive control and memory in children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 79: 25–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chang, Charles B., Daniel Wall, Medha Tare, Ewa Golonka, and Karen Vatz. 2014. Relationships of attitudes toward homework and time spent on homework to course outcomes: The case of foreign language learning. Journal of Educational Psychology 106: 1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Chen, Xu, Han Qi, Rui Liu, Yuan Feng, Wen Li, Mi Xiang, Teris Cheung, Todd Jackson, Gang Wang, and Yu-Tao Xiang. 2021. Depression, anxiety and associated factors among Chinese adolescents during the COVID-19 outbreak: A comparison of two cross-sectional studies. Translational Psychiatry 11: 148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Colditz, Graham A. 1999. Economic costs of obesity and inactivity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 31: S663–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Conradi, Henk Jan, Johan Ormel, and Peter De Jonge. 2011. Presence of individual (residual) symptoms during depressive episodes and periods of remission: A 3-year prospective study. Psychological Medicine 41: 1165–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Cooper, Harris, and Jeffrey C. Valentine. 2001. Using research to answer practical questions about homework. Educational Psychologist 36: 143–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Cooper, Harris, James J. Lindsay, and Barbara Nye. 2000. Homework in the home: How student, family, and parenting-style differences relate to the homework process. Contemporary Educational Psychology 25: 464–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Cooper, Harris, Jorgianne Civey Robinson, and Erika A. Patall. 2006. Does homework improve academic achievement? A synthesis of research, 1987–2003. Review of Educational Research 76: 1–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Dewald-Kaufmann, Julia, Frans Jeroen Oort, and Anne Marie Meijer. 2013. The effects of sleep extension on sleep and cognitive performance in adolescents with chronic sleep reduction: An experimental study. Sleep Medicine 14: 510–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Dwyer, Terry, Wayne E. Coonan, Donald R. Leitch, Basil S. Hetzel, and R. A. Baghurst. 1983. An investigation of the effects of daily physical activity on the health of primary school students in South Australia. International Journal of Epidemiology 12: 308–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Field, Tiffany Diego, and Christopher Sanders. 2001. Adolescent depression and risk factors. Adolescence 36: 491–98. [Google Scholar]
  20. Fu, Yan, Wanting Ren, and Zurong Liang. 2022. Perceived academic stress and depressive symptoms among Chinese adolescents: A moderated mediation analysis of overweight status. Journal of Affective Disorders 296: 224–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Gao, Wenjuan, Siqing Ping, and Xinqiao Liu. 2020. Gender differences in depression, anxiety, and stress among college students: A longitudinal study from China. Journal of Affective Disorders 263: 292–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Gao, Wenjuan, Yunfeng Luo, Xiaojie Cao, and Xinqiao Liu. 2022. Gender differences in the relationship between self-esteem and depression among college students: A cross-lagged study from China. Journal of Research in Personality 97: 104202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Goulabchand, Radjiv, Elodie Castille, Sophie Navucet, Damien Etchecopar-Etchart, Aurélie Matos, Alexandre Maria, Laure Anne Gutierrez, Alain Le Quellec, Nicolas Menjot de Champfleur, and Audrey Gabelle. 2022. The interplay between cognition, depression, anxiety, and sleep in primary Sjogren’s Syndrome patients. Scientific Reports 12: 13176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Grant, Merida M., Michael E. Thase, and John A. Sweeney. 2001. Cognitive disturbance in outpatient depressed younger adults: Evidence of modest impairment. Biological Psychiatry 50: 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Green, Matthew, Maria Kovacova, and Katarina Valaskova. 2020. Smartphone addiction risk, depression psychopathology, and social anxiety. Analysis and Metaphysics 19: 52–58. [Google Scholar]
  26. Guthold, Regina, Gretchen A. Stevens, Leanne M. Riley, and Fiona C. Bull. 2020. Global trends in insufficient physical activity among adolescents: A pooled analysis of 298 population-based surveys with 1· 6 million participants. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health 4: 23–35. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hilbrecht, Margo, Jiri Zuzanek, and Roger C. Mannell. 2008. Time use, time pressure and gendered behavior in early and late adolescence. Sex Roles 58: 342–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hillman, Charles H., Kirk I. Erickson, and Arthur F. Kramer. 2008. Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Hollon, Steven D., Richard C. Shelton, Stephen Wisniewski, Diane Warden, Melanie M. Biggs, Edward S. Friedman, Mustafa Husain, David J. Kupfer, Andrew A. Nierenberg, and Timothy J. Petersen. 2006. Presenting characteristics of depressed outpatients as a function of recurrence: Preliminary findings from the STAR* D clinical trial. Journal of Psychiatric Research 40: 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hunt, Eithne, and Elizabeth Anne McKay. 2015. What can be learned from adolescent time diary research. Journal of Adolescent Health 56: 259–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hunter, John E. 1986. Cognitive ability, cognitive aptitudes, job knowledge, and job performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior 29: 340–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Islam, Md Irteja, Raaj Kishore Biswas, and Rasheda Khanam. 2020. Effect of internet use and electronic game-play on academic performance of Australian children. Scientific Reports 10: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Jones, Kay, and Yvonne Harrison. 2001. Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Medicine Reviews 5: 463–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Jürges, Hendrik, and Rasheda Khanam. 2021. Adolescents’ time allocation and skill production. Economics of Education Review 85: 102178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. King, Daniel L., Michael Gradisar, Aaron Drummond, Nicole Lovato, Jason Wessel, Gorica Micic, Paul Douglas, and Paul Delfabbro. 2013. The impact of prolonged violent video-gaming on adolescent sleep: An experimental study. Journal of Sleep Research 22: 137–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kliestik, Tomas, Joan Scott, Hussam Musa, and Petr Suler. 2020. Addictive smartphone behavior, anxiety symptom severity, and depressive stress. Analysis and Metaphysics 19: 45–51. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kondo, Risa, Ichiro Miyano, Sangyoon Lee, Hiroyuki Shimada, and Hiroaki Kitaoka. 2021. Association between self-reported night sleep duration and cognitive function among older adults with intact global cognition. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 36: 766–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Kross, Ethan, Philippe Verduyn, Emre Demiralp, Jiyoung Park, David Seungjae Lee, Natalie Lin, Holly Shablack, John Jonides, and Oscar Ybarra. 2013. Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS ONE 8: e69841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kumar, Deepak, and Pradeep Kumar Choudhury. 2021. Do private schools really produce more learning than public schools in India? Accounting for student’s school absenteeism and the time spent on homework. International Journal of Educational Development 83: 102395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kuss, Daria J., and Mark D. Griffiths. 2012. Internet and gaming addiction: A systematic literature review of neuroimaging studies. Brain Sciences 2: 347–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kvelde, Tasha, Mirjam Pijnappels, Kim Delbaere, Jacqueline C. T. Close, and Stephen R. Lord. 2010. Physiological and cognitive mediators for the association between self-reported depressed mood and impaired choice stepping reaction time in older people. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences 65: 538–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Lăzăroiu, George, Maria Kovacova, Anna Siekelova, and Jaromir Vrbka. 2020. Addictive behavior of problematic smartphone users: The relationship between depression, anxiety, and stress. Review of Contemporary Philosophy 19: 50–56. [Google Scholar]
  43. Lee, Rico S. C., Daniel F. Hermens, Melanie A. Porter, and M. Antoinette Redoblado-Hodge. 2012. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 140: 113–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Lim, Shin-Il, and Sookyung Jeong. 2022. The Relationship between Korean Parents’ Smartphone Addiction and That of Their Children: The Mediating Effects of Children’s Depression and Social Withdrawal. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19: 5593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Liu, Xinqiao, Siqing Ping, and Wenjuan Gao. 2019a. Changes in undergraduate students’ psychological well-being as they experience university life. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16: 2864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Liu, Xinqiao, Wenjuan Gao, and Luxi Chen. 2023a. Does Pre-Service Teacher Preparation Affect Students’ Academic Performance? Evidence from China. Education Sciences 13: 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Liu, Xinqiao, Xiaojie Cao, and Wenjuan Gao. 2022a. Does low self-esteem predict anxiety among Chinese college students? Psychology Research and Behavior Management 15: 1481–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Liu, Xinqiao, Xueheng Gao, and Siqing Ping. 2019b. Post-1990s college students academic sustainability: The role of negative emotions, achievement goals, and self-efficacy on academic performance. Sustainability 11: 775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Liu, Xinqiao, Yu-Xin Guo, and Yi Xu. 2023b. Risk factors and digital interventions for anxiety disorders in college students: Stakeholder perspectives. World Journal of Clinical Cases 11: 1442–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Liu, Xinqiao, Yu-Xin Guo, Wen-Jie Zhang, and Wen-Juan Gao. 2022b. Influencing factors, prediction and prevention of depression in college students: A literature review. World Journal of Psychiatry 12: 860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Liu, Yan, Song Yue, Xiaoran Hu, Jin Zhu, Zifan Wu, JianLi Wang, and Yili Wu. 2021. Associations between feelings/behaviors during COVID-19 pandemic lockdown and depression/anxiety after lockdown in a sample of Chinese children and adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders 284: 98–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lloyd, Cynthia B., Monica Grant, and Amanda Ritchie. 2008. Gender differences in time use among adolescents in developing countries: Implications of rising school enrollment rates. Journal of Research on Adolescence 18: 99–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Lo, June C., Ju Lynn Ong, Ruth L. F. Leong, Joshua J. Gooley, and Michael W. L. Chee. 2016. Cognitive performance, sleepiness, and mood in partially sleep deprived adolescents: The need for sleep study. Sleep 39: 687–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Lowe, Cassandra J., Adrian Safati, and Peter A. Hall. 2017. The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: A meta-analytic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 80: 586–604. [Google Scholar]
  55. McLeod, Jane D., and Karen Kaiser. 2004. Childhood emotional and behavioral problems and educational attainment. American Sociological Review 69: 636–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Mills, Kathryn L. 2014. Effects of Internet use on the adolescent brain: Despite popular claims, experimental evidence remains scarce. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18: 385–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Moe, Angelica, Anne C. Frenzel, Lik Au, and Jamie L. Taxer. 2021. Displayed enthusiasm attracts attention and improves recall. British Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 911–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Moè, Angelica, Idit Katz, Rinat Cohen, and Marianna Alesi. 2020. Reducing homework stress by increasing adoption of need-supportive practices: Effects of an intervention with parents. Learning and Individual Differences 82: 101921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Moè, Angelica. 2015. Perceived control mediates the relations between depressive symptoms and academic achievement in adolescence. The Spanish Journal of Psychology 18: E70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Neville, Ross D., Kimberley D. Lakes, Will G. Hopkins, Giampiero Tarantino, Catherine E. Draper, Rosemary Beck, and Sheri Madigan. 2022. Global changes in child and adolescent physical activity during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatrics 9: 886–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Oliveira, Júlia Meller Dias de, Luciana Butini, Patrícia Pauletto, Karyn Munyk Lehmkuhl, Cristine Miron Stefani, Michele Bolan, Eliete Guerra, Bruce Dick, Graziela De Luca Canto, and Carla Massignan. 2022. Mental health effects prevalence in children and adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review. Worldviews on Evidence-Based Nursing 19: 130–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Onyeaka, Henry K., Chioma Muoghalu, Philip Baiden, Lucinda Okine, Hannah S. Szlyk, JaNiene E. Peoples, Erin Kasson, MSW Patricia Cavazos-Rehg, Joseph Firth, and John Torous. 2022. Excessive screen time behaviors and cognitive difficulties among adolescents in the United States: Results from the 2017 and 2019 national youth risk behavior survey. Psychiatry Research 316: 114740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Pekrun, Reinhard. 2006. The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology Review 18: 315–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Pekrun, Reinhard, Thomas Goetz, Wolfram Titz, and Raymond P. Perry. 2002. Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educational Psychologist 37: 91–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Rikkers, Wavne, David Lawrence, Jennifer Hafekost, and Stephen R. Zubrick. 2016. Internet use and electronic gaming by children and adolescents with emotional and behavioural problems in Australia–results from the second Child and Adolescent Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing. BMC Public Health 16: 399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Schmutte, Timothy, Shelby Harris, Ross Levin, Richard Zweig, Mindy Katz, and Richard Lipton. 2007. The relation between cognitive functioning and self-reported sleep complaints in nondemented older adults: Results from the Bronx aging study. Behavioral Sleep Medicine 5: 39–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Singh, Amika S., Emi Saliasi, Vera Van Den Berg, Léonie Uijtdewilligen, Renate H. M. De Groot, Jelle Jolles, Lars B. Andersen, Richard Bailey, Yu-Kai Chang, and Adele Diamond. 2019. Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive and academic performance in children and adolescents: A novel combination of a systematic review and recommendations from an expert panel. British Journal of Sports Medicine 53: 640–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Skinner, Burrhus Frederic. 1954. The science of learning and the art of teaching. Harvard Educational Review 24: 86–97. [Google Scholar]
  69. Tang, Sandra, and Megan E. Patrick. 2018. Technology and interactive social media use among 8th and 10th graders in the US and associations with homework and school grades. Computers in Human Behavior 86: 34–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Thapar, Anita, Stephan Collishaw, Daniel S. Pine, and Ajay K. Thapar. 2012. Depression in adolescence. The Lancet 379: 1056–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Tomporowski, Phillip D., Catherine L. Davis, Patricia H. Miller, and Jack A. Naglieri. 2008. Exercise and children’s intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educational Psychology Review 20: 111–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Tsai, Ling-Ling, and Sheng-Ping Li. 2004. Sleep patterns in college students: Gender and grade differences. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 56: 231–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Twenge, Jean M., and Gabrielle N. Martin. 2020. Gender differences in associations between digital media use and psychological well-being: Evidence from three large datasets. Journal of Adolescence 79: 91–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Valkenburg, Patti M., Adrian Meier, and Ine Beyens. 2022. Social media use and its impact on adolescent mental health: An umbrella review of the evidence. Current Opinion in Psychology 44: 58–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Wagner, Stefanie, Bettina Doering, Isabella Helmreich, Klaus Lieb, and André Tadić. 2012. A meta-analysis of executive dysfunctions in unipolar major depressive disorder without psychotic symptoms and their changes during antidepressant treatment. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 125: 281–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Walsh, Jeremy J., Joel D. Barnes, Mark S. Tremblay, and Jean-Philippe Chaput. 2020. Associations between duration and type of electronic screen use and cognition in US children. Computers in Human Behavior 108: 106312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Wang, Kaipeng, Dale Dagar Maglalang, Bongki Woo, Carson M. De Fries, Leslie K. Hasche, and Luis M. Falcón. 2022. Perceived discrimination and cognitive function among older Puerto Ricans in Boston: The mediating role of depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Wang, Yi-Qun, Rui Li, Meng-Qi Zhang, Ze Zhang, Wei-Min Qu, and Zhi-Li Huang. 2015. The neurobiological mechanisms and treatments of REM sleep disturbances in depression. Current Neuropharmacology 13: 543–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. WHO. 2021. Adolescent Mental Health. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/adolescent-mental-health (accessed on 20 January 2023).
  80. Widyastari, Dyah Anantalia, Pairoj Saonuam, Kornkanok Pongpradit, Narakorn Wongsingha, Panya Choolers, Sarocha Kesaro, Wittika Thangchan, Pott Pongpaopattanakul, Khwansupanat Phankasem, and Muhammadrorfee-E Musor. 2022. Results from the Thailand 2022 report card on physical activity for children and youth. Journal of Exercise Science & Fitness 20: 276–82. [Google Scholar]
  81. Wight, Vanessa R., Joseph Price, Suzanne M. Bianchi, and Bijou R. Hunt. 2009. The time use of teenagers. Social Science Research 38: 792–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Xiao, Pei, Kaiheng Zhu, Qi Liu, Xinyan Xie, Qi Jiang, Yanan Feng, Xiaoqian Wu, Jun Tang, and Ranran Song. 2022. Association between developmental dyslexia and anxiety/depressive symptoms among children in China: The chain mediating of time spent on homework and stress. Journal of Affective Disorders 297: 495–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Xin, Meiqi, Sitong Luo, Rui She, Yanqiu Yu, Lijuan Li, Suhua Wang, Le Ma, Fangbiao Tao, Jianxin Zhang, and Junfeng Zhao. 2020. Negative cognitive and psychological correlates of mandatory quarantine during the initial COVID-19 outbreak in China. American Psychologist 75: 607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Yang, Fan Nils, Weizhen Xie, and Ze Wang. 2022. Effects of sleep duration on neurocognitive development in early adolescents in the USA: A propensity score matched, longitudinal, observational study. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health 6: 705–12. [Google Scholar]
  85. Zhou, Tong, Rui Li, Yan Shi, Gang Tian, and Yan Yan. 2022. The associations between sleep duration, cognitive function, and depressive symptoms: An analysis of Chinese adolescents from China Family Panel Studies. Journal of Affective Disorders 319: 252–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Zill, Nicholas. 1995. Adolescent time use, risky behavior, and outcomes: An analysis of national data. Adolescents 11: 138. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Theoretical research framework. (Note: T1 refers to the 2013–2014 academic year and T2 refers to the 2014–2015 academic year.)
Figure 1. Theoretical research framework. (Note: T1 refers to the 2013–2014 academic year and T2 refers to the 2014–2015 academic year.)
Jintelligence 11 00088 g001
Figure 2. Mean and standard deviation of different aspects of time use by gender (female and male).
Figure 2. Mean and standard deviation of different aspects of time use by gender (female and male).
Jintelligence 11 00088 g002
Figure 3. Mean and standard deviation of different aspects of time use by grade (7th grade and 9th grade).
Figure 3. Mean and standard deviation of different aspects of time use by grade (7th grade and 9th grade).
Jintelligence 11 00088 g003
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of cognitive achievement, depression symptoms, and time use and their gender and grade differences.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of cognitive achievement, depression symptoms, and time use and their gender and grade differences.
VariablesNMeanStandard DeviationMinMaxSkewnessKurtosisGender DifferenceGrade Difference
Cognitive achievement (T1)11,71710.3883.7530220.0072.564−0.0871.818 ***
Cognitive achievement (T2)10,75021.0728.969035−0.9153.2710.726 ***-
Depression symptoms11,5262.0540.794150.9284.2270.067 ***−0.143 ***
Time spent on homework 11,717126.40168.07503000.212.5314.024 ***−19.640 ***
Time spent playing sports 11,71741.02242.01103001.4836.275−10.874 ***−1.458 *
Time spent surfing the Internet 11,71734.46646.70703002.1828.918−13.918 ***−1.250
Time spent watching TV 11,71748.13948.18703001.6846.676−4.397 ***5.785 ***
Sleep time11,717472.55375.1630720−1.1928.636−5.690 ***41.280 ***
Note: *** 1% significance level, * 10% significance level; T-tests were used to determine gender and grade differences; the times in the table represent minutes per day. Gender Difference is defined as the difference between the values of the variable for females and males. Grade Difference is defined as the difference between the values of the variable for 7th and 9th graders. T1 refers to the 2013–2014 academic year and T2 refers to the 2014–2015 academic year.
Table 2. Correlation analysis of cognitive achievement, depression symptoms, and time use.
Table 2. Correlation analysis of cognitive achievement, depression symptoms, and time use.
Variables(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)
(1) Cognitive achievement (T1)1
-
(2) Cognitive achievement (T2)0.326 ***
[0.319, 0.357]
1
-
(3) Depression symptoms−0.105 ***
[−0.124, −0.087]
−0.096 ***
[−0.115, −0.077]
1
-
(4) Time spent on homework 0.108 ***
[0.090,0.127]
0.105 ***
[0.086, 0.124]
0.055 ***
[0.037, 0.073]
1
-
(5) Time spent playing sports 0.048 ***
[0.030,0.066]
0.047 ***
[0.028, 0.066]
−0.084 ***
[−0.102, −0.066]
0.028 ***
[0.010, 0.046]
1
-
(6) Time spent surfing the Internet −0.044 ***
[−0.062,−0.026]
−0.108 ***
[−0.127, −0.09]
0.045 ***
[0.027, 0.063]
−0.115 ***
[−0.134, −0.097]
0.006
[−0.012, 0.024]
1
-
(7) Time spent watching TV −0.120 ***
[−0.139,−0.102]
−0.124 ***
[−0.144, −0.106]
0.053 ***
[0.035, 0.071]
−0.109 ***
[−0.128, −0.091]
0.000
[−0.018, 0.018]
0.226 ***
[0.212, 0.248]
1
-
(8) Sleep time0.043 ***
[0.025,0.061]
0.014
[−0.005, 0.033]
−0.157 ***
[−0.177, −0.140]
−0.216 ***
[−0.238, −0.201]
−0.009
[−0.027, 0.009]
−0.055 ***
[−0.073,−0.037]
0.024 **
[0.006, 0.042]
1
-
Note: *** 1% significance level, ** 5% significance level. The 95% confidence interval calculated using the Fisher transformation method for the correlation coefficient is presented below; T1 refers to the 2013–2014 academic year and T2 refers to the 2014–2015 academic year.
Table 3. Mediating effect of depression symptoms on the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement (T1) among Chinese adolescents.
Table 3. Mediating effect of depression symptoms on the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement (T1) among Chinese adolescents.
PathwaysEstimate95% CIS.E.Estimate/S.E.p Value
LowerUpper
Direct effects
Time spent on homework → cognitive achievement (T1)0.1380.1220.1560.00915.9810.000
Time spent playing sports → cognitive achievement (T1)0.0380.0190.0560.0093.9800.000
Time spent surfing the Internet → cognitive achievement (T1)0.001−0.0170.0190.0090.1270.899
Time spent watching TV → cognitive achievement (T1)−0.117−0.133−0.1010.008−14.3140.000
Sleep time → cognitive achievement (T1)−0.013−0.0320.0060.010−1.2760.202
Indirect effects
Time spent on homework → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T1)−0.004−0.006−0.0020.001−3.6860.000
Time spent playing sports → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T1)0.0080.0060.0110.0016.2300.000
Time spent surfing the Internet → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T1)−0.002−0.0050.0000.001−1.9970.046
Time spent watching TV → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T1)−0.005−0.008−0.0030.001−4.7810.000
Sleep time → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T1)0.0150.0110.0190.0027.5850.000
Note: T1 refers to the 2013–2014 academic year and T2 refers to the 2014–2015 academic year.
Table 4. Indirect effect of depression symptoms on the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement (T2) among Chinese adolescents.
Table 4. Indirect effect of depression symptoms on the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement (T2) among Chinese adolescents.
PathwaysEstimate95% CIS.E.Estimate/S.E.p Value
LowerUpper
Time spent on homework → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T2)−0.004−0.007−0.0020.001−3.3220.001
Time spent playing sports → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T2)0.0090.0050.0120.0024.9750.000
Time spent surfing the Internet → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T2)−0.002−0.0050.0000.001−1.9230.054
Time spent watching TV → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T2)−0.005−0.009−0.0030.001−4.3160.000
Sleep time → depression symptoms → cognitive achievement (T2)0.0150.0100.0210.0035.5240.000
Note: T1 refers to the 2013–2014 academic year and T2 refers to the 2014–2015 academic year.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cao, X.; Liu, X. Time Use and Cognitive Achievement among Adolescents in China: Depression Symptoms as Mediators. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050088

AMA Style

Cao X, Liu X. Time Use and Cognitive Achievement among Adolescents in China: Depression Symptoms as Mediators. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(5):88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050088

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cao, Xiaojie, and Xinqiao Liu. 2023. "Time Use and Cognitive Achievement among Adolescents in China: Depression Symptoms as Mediators" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 5: 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050088

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop