Next Article in Journal
Quantitative and Qualitative Analysis of Psychosocial Factors Affecting Women’s Entrepreneurship
Next Article in Special Issue
The Effect of the Secret Agent Society Group Program on Parent-Teacher Agreement Regarding Children’s Social Emotional Functioning
Previous Article in Journal
Association between Exposure to Domestic Violence during Childhood and Depressive Symptoms in Middle and Older Age: A Longitudinal Analysis in China
Previous Article in Special Issue
Emojis Are Comprehended Better than Facial Expressions, by Male Participants
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Emotional Dysfunction and Interoceptive Challenges in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders

Department of Psychology, Universidad Francisco de Vitoria, 28223 Pozuelo de Alarcón, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(4), 312; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13040312
Submission received: 1 March 2023 / Revised: 26 March 2023 / Accepted: 31 March 2023 / Published: 5 April 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Social Processing in People with or without Autism)

Abstract

:
People with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) frequently show impaired sensory processing in different senses, including the interoceptive system. Recent findings suggest that interoception is a fundamental component of emotional experience and that impaired interoception is associated with alexithymia. This study aims to explore the association and interrelation between interoceptive confusion, alexithymia, and the capacity for emotional regulation among a sample of 33 adults with ASD compared to a control group of 35 adults with neurotypical development and its mutual impact. The participants answered a series of questionnaires addressing these three variables. The results showed (1) significant differences between the groups in all dimensions, with dysfunctional emotional regulation, impaired interoception, and alexithymia in the ASD group, (2) significant correlations between interoceptive confusion, emotional clarity, and alexithymia in the ASD group but only positive correlations between interoceptive confusion and alexithymia in the CG, and (3) that emotional clarity, alexithymia, and autism explain 61% of the variance in interoceptive confusion. These results are in line with previous studies and suggest that training interoceptive ability may enhance emotional clarity and reduce alexithymia among those diagnosed with ASD, with significant implications in the planning of treatment.

1. Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by difficulties in social interaction and communication, as well as restricted or repetitive behaviors or interests. Since the publication of the DSM-5 [1], it also includes anomalies in sensorial perception as diagnostic criteria given that individuals with ASD often display inexact sensorial processing, ranging from a lack of response to exaggerated reactions to sensorial stimuli [2,3]; thus, the clinical picture of ASD may vary in its symptomology and severity [4].
Emotional self-regulation (ER) is defined as the ability to control and modify one’s own emotional state, acting in consequence and facilitating adaptive behavior [5,6]. The process of controlling one’s own emotions can be automatic or intentional and may be a response to a current or antecedent situation [7]. ER strategies may be adaptive or maladaptive, the latter referring to psychopathologies and problematic behavior, and more prevalent among those with ASD [8].
Emotional regulation requires the recognition of one’s own emotional state, that is, the ability to identify and describe internal bodily sensations that signal a lack of physical homeostasis, spurring action to restore balance [6,9]. Similarly, the awareness of internal signals leads to an adequate understanding of one’s emotions, permitting the development of adequate emotional regulation strategies [6].
The research has shown that inadequate ER and maladaptive ER strategies are prevalent among those with ASD, both in children and adults [7,10,11]. Studies have found that children with ASD are less likely to employ adaptive coping strategies compared to children without ASD [12,13] and, in moments of frustration, show higher levels of negative affect, which persists despite calming efforts [12]. The research has also found that adolescents with ASD have a greater propensity for maladaptive ER strategies, such as avoidance, negation, rumination, and emotional numbness [14,15]. Similar results have been seen in adults with ASD, who more commonly use strategies such as suppression [16], resignation, and avoidance [12] compared to neurotypical adults [17].
This suggests that those with ASD lack the motivational component of ER, an essential aspect in emotional control. This is apparently reinforced by low emotional perception and self-control. Thus, those with ASD appear to lack the emotional self-perception necessary for effective ER [10]. Other aspects of impaired ER are intrinsic to ASD, such as difficulties in maintaining perspective and problem solving, lower response inhibition, deficits in emotional awareness, and the inadequate interpretation of social signals [7].
The research has identified a connection between alexithymia and ASD given that both types of clinical cases manifest similar difficulties. Individuals with alexithymia often have difficulties in recognizing emotions, establishing social relations, understanding the intentions of others, presenting limitations in moral decision making, etc. These characteristics, in conjunction with impaired communication and social skills, are commonly observed among those with ASD [18,19]. Alexithymia and ASD constitute a personality trait that makes them struggle when verbalizing internal states due to the lack of awareness and clarity that allows for understanding body sensations and translating them into feelings [18].
Studies have also shown that those with ASD and alexithymia derive less enjoyment in social interactions [20] and tend to feel and show less empathy [21]. However, alexithymia is not considered a psychiatric category in itself (that is, not included in the DSM-5) but rather a group of symptoms manifested in various other clinical diagnoses (eating disorders, addiction, mood disorders, ASD, etc.), which vary in severity depending on the individual [22,23]. The role of alexithymia with the broader symptomology of ASD is unclear [19], but the rates of alexithymia in the ASD population range from 40% to 65% [24]. Recently, Ben Hassen et al. explained that difficulties in ER, commonly associated with ASD, may in fact be due to the presence of alexithymia rather than ASD per se [25].
Because adequate emotional awareness is the foundation of ER [26], the socio-emotional deficiencies common among those with ASD may be related to difficulties in perceiving internal bodily signals [27]. There has been growing research into the notion of interoception, also referred to as the ‘eighth sense’. It is now clear that human beings are not limited to seven senses (sight, smell, taste, touch, hearing, proprioceptive, and vestibular senses) [6]. Interoception is associated with the awareness of physiological sensations, such as pain, body temperature, hunger, thirst, heart rate and breathing, muscular tension, etc., the internal condition of the body [28,29]. The interoceptive sense has receptors in various parts of the body, in the organs, skin, and muscles which capture sensorial information and send it to the brain, specifically the insular cortex or insula, where they are translated into a clear message of what the body is feeling and experiencing [28].
Interoception is a fundamental component of emotional experience. Poor interoception inhibits a person’s awareness of internal signals, and hence an inability to deploy effective ER strategies [6,28,30]. Moreover, recent findings suggest that alexithymia can best be characterized as a general failure of interoception rather than a specific affective impairment [31].
Empirical evidence of the relation between interoception and ASD is limited and inconsistent; however, impaired or deficient interception is common among those with ASD [6] who show much lower levels of interoceptive awareness than those without ASD [32,33], specifically in their awareness of internal physical sensations, such as thirst [34]. Persons with ASD show hyposensitivity, with difficulties in perceiving and identifying bodily sensations, such as pain, etc. [3,35].
Based on this theoretical framework, impaired interoception, the inability to identify and describe internal signals, can lead to alexithymia [19,31], found to be highly prevalent among those with ASD [36] and a contributing factor in maladaptive ER [7,37], aggravating the disorder and further complicating emotional self-expression and social relationships [31,32,38,39].
It was not until very recently that interoception scales were tested in the ASD population even though impaired interoception may be closely related to many of the deficits identified among this population, such as alexithymia and impaired emotional regulation, leading to difficulties in social relations. The aim of this study is to explore the relation between interoceptive capacity, emotional regulation, and alexithymia among people with ASD. Establishing evidence of this phenomenon is the first step toward developing interventions and effective treatments to enhance the quality of life of individuals with ASD and their families.
The present study has the following objectives: (1) to determine the characteristics of interoceptive confusion, alexithymia, and emotional regulation among a sample of participants with ASD, and to identify areas where this group scores significantly from the control group with neurotypical development (CG); (2) to analyze the association between the interoception scale with scales of alexithymia and the ER subscales (attention, clarity, and emotional repair) for each group (ASD and CG); and (3) to explore the relative impact of the variables alexithymia, ER subscales (attention, clarity, and emotional repair), and autism as a predictor of interoceptive confusion.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The initial sample was 160 participants, men and women; 70 of the participants were eliminated as they did not meet the inclusion criteria or dropped out during the test.
The final sample consisted of 68 men, of whom 48.53% (n = 33) were adults diagnosed with ASD between 21 and 58 years of age (M = 34.29; SD = 10.89) and 51.47% (n = 35) were adults with neurotypical development between the ages of 18 and 58 (M = 33.43; SD = 13.25). Although 22 women completed the questionnaires, they were not included in the analysis due to the acknowledged differences in clinical presentation between men and women [40], especially in relation to difficulties in emotional regulation and empathy.
The inclusion criteria for the ASD group were (a) being over the age of 18; (b) having a confirmed diagnosis of ASD; (c) having the necessary language ability to complete the self-evaluation questionnaire individually and with a score above 6 in the AQ-10 [41] (described below). The inclusion criteria for the CG were (a) being over the age of 18; (b) not being diagnosed with ASD or other clinical diagnosis; and (c) scoring below 6 in the screening version of the AQ-10 test.
The education levels of the participants were evaluated as an indirect measure of intelligence and verbal proficiency. For the CG, 5.7% (n = 2) had a primary education, 2.9% (n = 1) secondary education, 11.4% (n = 4) a baccalaureate, 8.6% (n = 3) had vocational training, and 71.4% (n = 25) had a university degree. For the ASD group, 3.0% (n = 1) had primary education, 12.1% (n = 4) secondary education, 18.2% (n = 6) a baccalaureate, 33.3% (n = 11) had vocational training, and 33.3% (n = 11) had a university degree. We assumed, therefore, that they were all equally capable of answering the survey.

2.2. Procedure

After receiving approval of the Ethics Committee of the Universidad Francisco de Vitoria (8/2021), the research was conducted through various entities in contact with persons with ASD and through social media. The questionnaires were completed online, and all participants provided their informed consent. Participation was voluntary and anonymous. Data were obtained using non-probability and snowball sampling methods.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Sociodemographic Questionnaire

Collecting data about age, gender, level of education (no education, primary, secondary, baccalaureate, vocational training, university education), clinical diagnosis (yes, no, I don’t know, I don’t want to answer), age of diagnosis.

2.3.2. Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ-10)

The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) is a 50-item questionnaire which evaluates symptoms of autism in 5 domains: social skills, communication skills, attention to detail, attention switching, and imagination [42]. In 2012, Allison et al. developed the AQ-10, a reduced version of 10 items [41]. The responses are scored on a 4-point Likert-type scale (1 = definitely disagree and 4 = definitely agree). Scores higher than 6 are considered an indication of possible ASD. The AQ-10 showed good internal consistency in the original study (α= 0.85). In the present sample, the AQ-10 showed adequate reliability (α = 0.69).

2.3.3. Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS)

This is a trait scale that evaluates meta-knowledge of emotional states, specifically the ability to perceive and regulate one’s own emotions [43]. It consists of 24 items on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree) that address three key dimensions of emotional intelligence with 8 items each: (1) emotional attention, (2) emotional clarity, and (3) emotional repair. According to the test, scores considered Limited for men in each subscale was as follows: emotional attention < 21, emotional clarity < 25, and emotional repair < 23. Scores were considered adequate when they range from 22 to 33 for emotional attention, 26 to 35 for emotional clarity, and 24 to 35 for emotional repair [43]. It was validated to Spanish population by Fernández-Berrocal et al. [44] The scale shows adequate internal consistency, α = 0.90, α = 0.90, and α = 0.86, respectively. For this research, the scale showed excellent internal consistency, both overall (α = 0.96) and in its respective subscales (α = 0.92, α = 0.95, and α = 0.92).

2.3.4. Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20)

This is a self-reporting, 20-item instrument using a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree) which evaluates alexithymia in 3 cognitive-affective areas: (1) difficulty in identifying feelings, (2) difficulty describing feelings, and (3) externally oriented thinking. Scores between 52 and 60 indicate possible alexithymia and a score of 61 indicates the presence of alexithymia [45]. The higher the score, the greater the inability to recognize one’s own emotions. It has a Spanish version by Martínez-Sánchez [46]. Internal consistency of the tool for this study was considered excellent (α = 0.82).

2.3.5. Interoceptive Sensory Questionnaire (ISQ)

A self-reporting scale that measures interoceptive confusion among adults with ASD, using language that is natural and understandable for this population [38]. The tool consists of 20 items using a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = totally untrue for me and 7 = total true for me). A score of 70 indicates interoceptive confusion, that is, greater difficulty in interception. Scores equal to or above 94 indicate a high degree of interoceptive confusion. The questionnaire was only available in its original language (English); a process of reverse translation was used to translate it into Spanish for the purposes of this study. The internal consistency of the instrument for the present study was excellent (α = 0.96), in line with data from [38].

2.4. Data Analysis

IBM’s SPSS Statistics software, version 25, was used for statistical analysis. Given the features of the sample, non-parametric method was used. Firstly, a descriptive analysis was made of the principal research variables, followed by a Mann–Whitney U test to analyze the differences between the groups for each scale. To analyze the relation between the variables, Spearman’s coefficient was used for each group. Finally, a multiple stepwise linear regression test was conducted to determine the predictive value of the variables for interoception.

3. Results

Firstly, the descriptive statistics were analyzed for the scales of interoceptive confusion, alexithymia, and ER for each group (ASD and CG). Table 1 shows that the scores of the ASD group were above the clinical cut-off point (≥ 61 for alexithymia; <21 for emotional attention; <25 for emotional clarity; and <23 for emotional repair) (see Table 1).
The analysis showed statistically significant differences between the groups (p < 0.01) in all dimensions of the study. The ASD group scored significantly higher in the scales of interoception (z = −5.18) and alexithymia (z = −4.18), while the CG scored higher in all the subscales for emotional regulation: emotional attention (z = −3.16), emotional clarity (z = −4.60), and emotional repair (z = −4.13) (see Table 1).
A non-parametric Spearman correlation test was used to analyze the association between the interoception scale with the scales of alexithymia and the subscales of ER for each group, which showed significant inverse correlations in the ASD group between interoceptive confusion and emotional clarity and a significant direct correlation with alexithymia (see Table 2). For the CG, a significant direct correlation was found between interoceptive confusion and alexithymia (see Table 3).
Finally, a multiple stepwise lineal regression was conducted to identify the principal predictors of interoception. A Durbin–Watson test was first performed to confirm the correlation of the variables, with a result of 1.71. The regression showed an R2 corrected equal to 0.61, indicating that the variables emotional clarity, alexithymia, and autism predict 61% of the variable interoception, with greater interceptive confusion corresponding to less emotional clarity (ER subscale) and with greater difficulties in alexithymia and autism corresponding to greater interoceptive confusion (See Table 4). The results of an ANOVA test confirm the goodness of fit of the model (p < 0.05).

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to evaluate interoceptive skills in relation to alexithymia among adult men with ASD, as these variables are associated with poor ER strategies [32,47,48].
Our study found that only the ASD group was suggestive of interoceptive confusion, in line with the presence of alexithymia, as well as limited emotional regulation. There were also statistically significant differences between the ASD and CG groups in the scales, confirming higher scores in the scales of alexithymia and interoceptive confusion and lower scores in the ER subscales for the ASD group. These results are in line with those of previous studies which found that people with ASD show greater levels of interoceptive confusion [34,49,50,51,52,53], greater symptoms of alexithymia [22,54], and worse ER strategies [7,16].
Regarding the association between the interoception scores, alexithymia, and the ER subscales, the study found a significant correlation in the ASD group between the interoceptive confusion and the alexithymia scale (positive relation) and the ER subscale of emotional clarity (inverse relation) in contrast with the results in the CG in which a significant correlation was found between the scale of interoception and alexithymia (positive correlation). Hence, these results are in coherence with those who found that alexithymia is closely associated with impaired ER because ER strategies necessarily require an initial awareness and identification of emotions [47,48]. This lack of internal emotional understanding also impairs the awareness of the feelings and emotions of others, hindering social interaction [18,55]. In fact, studies have found that persons with high scores in alexithymia often make use of ineffective ER strategies, such as suppression [48,54]. Along the same line, although scarce, some previous researchers found a relation between the presence of alexithymia and interoception both in the ASD population [31,56,57,58] and the non-clinical population [59]. Researchers also found that greater interoceptive confusion corresponds to poor ER strategies [6,16,60,61].
Scientific evidence showed that the interoceptive difficulties found among individuals with ASD are usually classified into three patterns [52]. Interoceptive hyposensitivity is where the person is incapable of noting the internal bodily signals or emotional state unless they are very intense; interoceptive hypersensitivity is where the person perceives internal sensations quickly and intensely, or feels various sensations at the same time, causing difficulties in determining which signals are the most important. However, there is a third pattern of interoceptive impairment that includes poor discrimination (sometimes accompanied by a lack of verbal accuracy), where the person is unable to identity the exact internal feeling because the experience is a vague or generalized sensation, with a limited ability to identity the meaning of this feeling [6,62]. This third phenomenon has led to confusing results in several studies [63,64]. As in the present study, sometimes these three patterns of interoception impediments make it hard to find a clear linear association within the data set, although the differences between the groups appeared at first.
One of our significant findings relates to the controversial relations between alexithymia, autism, ER, and interoception. Our study joins others [65] that know these variables tend to be correlated even though they are not yet considered diagnostic criteria and the relationship between them is not clear. One question underlying this study is which variables can be related to others so that future intervention can be more effective.
The other finding is that although these associations between these variables may be intuitive, the present study shows they did not occur in the same way in each group. Emotional regulation seems to be a more cognitive-based skill in the control group and a more physical-based skill in the ASD group.
Finally, it may be affirmed that emotional clarity (ER subscale), alexithymia, and autism are predictive variables of interoceptive confusion for 61% of the total sample. In contrast, the variables attention and emotional repair (ER subscales) are not predictors of interoception. These results suggest that greater levels of emotional clarity correspond to better interoception, while symptoms of alexithymia and autism correspond to greater interoceptive confusion. The present study adds evidence to the direct impact that interoceptive confusion may have on emotional dysfunction. In clinical practice, this may indicate a greater probability of interoceptive confusion among those showing signs of alexithymia and autism. The inverse relation between emotional clarity and interoception suggests that greater difficulties in emotional clarity (an inadequate understanding of one’s own emotions) might reflect higher levels of interoceptive confusion. Assessing interoception prior to intervention and integrating the results into the therapy may help diminish the alexithymia and emotional difficulties for those with ASD. Previous research has shown that training to develop interoception (improving interoceptive awareness) can enhance the capacity for effective emotional regulation [63,65,66].
These findings suggest that interoceptive awareness is the basis of more complex constructs, such as the recognition and expression of emotional states and ER. The perception and understanding of internal sensations is clearly a fundamental prerequisite for adequate emotional regulation [26,67,68]. Thus, interoceptive ability can be enhanced by the development of emotional clarity, reducing alexithymia among those diagnosed with ASD. This issue may have important implications for the prevention of social challenges that are extended to intimate relationships and identity when it comes to adolescence and adulthood [69,70]
On the whole, this study shows that a more precise description with a clinical approach should be considered when assessing and developing intervention programs for those with ASD to effectively address these difficulties, including training in interoceptive awareness, reducing alexithymia, and developing adaptive ER strategies [63].

Limitations and Future Research

The present research is not without its limitations. The sample was relatively small, without the possibility of analyzing the variables of the study by gender. Future research should ideally use larger samples including women for more complete and generalizable results. Additionally, longitudinal studies of ASD populations should explore the possibility that the understanding of emotional and physical signals can vary throughout the life cycle. The present study offers data on adults only, and it is therefore unclear if these results can be extrapolated to children with ASD.
Another limitation was the use of self-reporting evaluation tools which produce biased or inaccurate results. Given that those with ASD often have difficulties in perceiving and identifying internal signals, analyzing problems, and expressing emotions [18,32,71], it may be instructive to use different evaluation instruments to compare scores based on mixed methods.
Additionally, this study was the first to use the Interoceptive Sensory Questionnaire with a Spanish sample. The good internal consistency (α = 0.95) indicates that this may be an effective tool for clinical assessment prior to intervention [52].

5. Conclusions

The study found that adult men with ASD have difficulty identifying internal bodily signals, with limitations in their ability to identify and regulate their emotional and physical state. We propose that future research explores the possibility of developing intervention programs based on increasing interoceptive awareness, focusing specifically on emotional clarity and alexithymia as these variables predict and influence interoceptive ability.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.B. and D.R.; methodology, C.M. and D.R.; validation, S.B.; formal analysis, S.B., C.M. and D.R.; investigation, D.R.; resources, S.B.; data curation, D.R.; writing—original draft, S.B., C.M. and D.R.; writing—review and editing, S.B., C.M. and D.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was partially funded by Universidad Francisco de Vitoria.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Universidad Francisco de Vitoria (protocol code 8/2021 and date of approval 19 February 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We thank the adults who participated in this study and the professionals from the following associations who cooperated in the study: Specialisterne, Fundación Àngel Rivière, and Asociación Asperger Madrid.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5: Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales, 5th ed.; Editrial Medica Panamericana S.A.: Madrid, Spain, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  2. Case-Smith, J.; Weaver, L.L.; Fristad, M.A. A systematic review of sensory processing interventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 2015, 19, 133–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Elwin, M.; Ek, L.; Schröder, A.; Kjellin, L. Autobiographical accounts of sensing in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2012, 26, 420–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Málaga, I.; Blanco Lago, R.; Hedrera-Fernández, A.; Álvarez-Álvarez, N.; Oreña-Ansonera, V.A.; Baeza-Velasco, M. Prevalencia de los Trastornos del Espectro Autista en niños en Estados Unidos, Europa y España: Coincidencias y discrepancias. Medicina 2019, 79, 1. [Google Scholar]
  5. Gross, J.J.; Thompson, R.A. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation; Gross, J.J., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 3–26. [Google Scholar]
  6. Mahler, K. Interoception: The Eighth Sensory System; AAPC Publishing: Shawnee, KS, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  7. Mazefsky, C.A.; White, S.W. Emotion regulation: Concepts & Practice in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2014, 23, 15–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Conner, C.M.; White, S.W.; Beck, K.B.; Golt, J.; Smith, I.C.; Mazefsky, C.A. Improving emotion regulation ability in autism: The Emotional Awareness and Skills Enhancement (EASE) program. Autism 2019, 23, 1273–1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Hervás, A. Desregulación emocional y trastornos del espectro autista. Rev. Neurol. 2017, 64, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Mazefsky, C.A.; Herrington, J.; Siegel, M.; Scarpa, A.; Maddox, B.B.; Scahill, L.; White, S.W. The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child. Psychiatry 2013, 52, 679–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Tajik-Parvinchi, D.J.; Farmus, L.; Cribbie, R.; Albaum, C.; Weiss, J.A. Clinical and parental predictors of emotion regulation following cognitive behaviour therapy in children with autism. Autism 2020, 24, 851–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Jahromi, L.B.; Meek, S.E.; Ober-Reynolds, S. Emotion regulation in the context of frustration in children with high functioning autism and their typical peers. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 2012, 53, 1250–1258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Konstantareas, M.M.; Stewart, K. Affect regulation and temperament in children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2006, 36, 143–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Khor, A.S.; Melvin, G.A.; Reid, S.C.; Gray, K.M. Coping, daily hassles and behavior and emotional problems in adolescents with high-functioning autism/Asperger’s disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 593–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mazefsky, C.A.; Borue, X.; Day, T.N.; Minshew, N.J. Emotion regulation patterns in adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: Comparison to typically developing adolescents and association with psychiatric symptoms. Autism Res. 2014, 7, 344–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Samson, A.C.; Huber, O.; Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation in Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning autism. Emotion 2012, 12, 659–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Jahromi, L.B.; Bryce, C.I.; Swanson, J. The importance of self- regulation for the school and peer engagement of children with high-functioning autism. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2013, 7, 235–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Morie, K.P.; Jackson, S.; Zhai, Z.W.; Potenza, M.N.; Dritschel, B. Mood Disorders in High-Functioning Autism: The Importance of Alexithymia and Emotional Regulation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 49, 2935–2945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Poquérusse, J.; Pastore, L.; Dellantonio, S.; Esposito, G. Alexithymia and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Complex Relationship. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Gebauer, L.; Skewes, J.; Westphael, G.; Heaton, P.; Vuust, P. Intact brain processing of musical emotions in autism spectrum disorder, but more cognitive load and arousal in happy vs. sad music. Front. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Luminet, O.; Vermeulen, N.; Demaret, C.; Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M. Alexithymia and levels of processing: Evidence for an overall deficit in remembering emotion words. J. Res. Pers. 2006, 40, 713–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Paula-Pérez, I.; Martos-Pérez, J.; Llorente-Comí, M. Alexitimia y síndrome de Asperger. Rev. Neurol. 2010, 50 (Suppl. 3), S85–S90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Sifneos, P.E. Alexithymia: Past and present. Am. J. Psychiatry 1996, 153, 137–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hill, E.; Berthoz, S.; Frith, U. Brief report: Cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2004, 34, 229–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Ben Hassen, N.; Molins, F.; Garrote-Petisco, D.; Serrano, M.Á. Emotional regulation deficits in autism spectrum disorder: The role of alexithymia and interoception. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2022, 132, 104378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gross, J.J. Emotion Regulation: Current Status and Future Prospects. Psychol. Inq. 2015, 26, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Quattrocki, E.; Friston, K. Autism, oxytocin and interoception. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 47, 410–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Craig, A.D. How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 655–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Fuchs, T.; Koch, S.C. Embodied affectivity: On moving and being moved. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Garfinkel, S.N.; Seth, A.K.; Barrett, A.B.; Suzuki, K.; Critchley, H.D. Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 2015, 104, 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Brewer, R.; Cook, R.; Bird, G. Alexithymia: A General Deficit of Interoception. R. Soc. Open. Sci. 2016, 3, 150664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Garfinkel, S.N.; Tiley, C.; O’Keeffe, S.; Harrison, N.A.; Seth, A.K.; Critchley, H.D. Discrepancies between dimensions of interoception in autism: Implications for emotion and anxiety. Biol. Psychol. 2016, 114, 117–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Mash, L.E.; Schauder, K.B.; Cochran, C.; Park, S.; Cascio, C.J. Associations Between Interoceptive Cognition and Age in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Typical Development. J. Cogn. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 16, 23–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Fiene, L.; Brownlow, C. Investigating Interoception and Body Awaraness in Adults with and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Res. 2015, 8, 709–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Edelson, S.M. Understanding Challenging Behaviors in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Multi-Component, Interdisciplinary Model. J. Pers. Med. 2022, 12, 1127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Berthoz, S.; Hill, E.L. The validity of using self-reports to assess emotion regulation abilities in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 2005, 20, 291–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Shah, P.; Catmur, C.; Bird, G. Emotional decision-making in autism spectrum disorder: The roles of interoception and alexithymia. Mol. Autism 2016, 7, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Fiene, L.; Ireland, M.J.; Brownlow, C. The Interoception Sensory Questionnaire (ISQ): A Scale to Measure Interoceptive Challenges in Adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2018, 48, 3354–3366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Zamariola, G.; Frost, N.; Van Oost, A.; Corneille, O.; Luminet, O. Relationship between interoception and emotion regulation: New evidence from mixed methods. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 246, 480–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Nichols, S.; Moravcik, G.M.; Pulver Tetenbaum, S. Girls Growing Up on the Autism Spectrum. What Parents and Professionals Should Know about the Pre-Teen and Teenage Years; Jessica Kingsley: Philadephia, PA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  41. Allison, C.; Auyeung, B.; Baron-Cohen, S. Toward brief “Red Flags” for autism screening: The Short Autism Spectrum Quotient and the Short Quantitative Checklist for Autism in toddlers in 1,000 cases and 3,000 controls [corrected]. J. Am. Acad. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 2012, 51, 202–212.e7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Baron-Cohen, S.; Wheelwright, S.; Skinner, R.; Martin, J.; Clubley, E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2001, 31, 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Salovey, P.; Mayer, J.D.; Goldman, S.L.; Turvey, C.; Palfai, T.P. Emotional Attention, Clarity, and Repair: Exploring Emotional Intelligence Using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1995; pp. 125–154. [Google Scholar]
  44. Fernández-Berrocal, P.; Extremera, N.; Ramos, N. Validity and reliability of the Spanish modified version of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Psychol Rep. 2004, 94, 751–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J.D. The Revised Toronto Alexithymia Scale: Some reliability, validity, and normative data. Psychother. Psychosom. 1992, 57, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Martínez-Sánchez, F. Adaptación española de la escala de Alexitimia de Toronto (TAS-20). Clínica Salud 1996, 7, 19–32. [Google Scholar]
  47. Shah, P.; Hall, R.; Catmur, C.; Bird, G. Alexithymia, not autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex 2016, 81, 215–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Swart, M.; Kortekaas, R.; Aleman, A. Dealing with feelings: Characterization of trait alexithymia on emotion regulation strategies and cognitive-emotional processing. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e5751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Ben-Sasson, A.; Hen, L.; Fluss, R.; Cermak, S.A.; Engel-Yeger, B.; Gal, E. A meta-analysis of sensory modulation symptoms in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2009, 39, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Crane, L.; Goddard, L.; Pring, L. Sensory processing in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 2009, 13, 215–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Elwin, M.; Ek, L.; Kjellin, L.; Schröder, A. Too much or too little: Hyper-and hypo-reactivity in high-functioning autism spectrum conditions. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 38, 232–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Trevisan, D.A.; Parker, T.; McPartland, J.C. First-Hand Accounts of Interoceptive Difficulties in Autistic Adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2021, 51, 3483–3491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Kinnaird, E.; Stewart, C.; Tchanturia, K. Investigating alexithymia in autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. Psychiatry 2019, 55, 80–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Bird, G.; Cook, R. Mixed emotions: The contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Transl. Psychiaty 2013, 3, e285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Connelly, M.; Deney, D.R. Regulation of emotions during experimental stress in alexithymia. J. Psychosom. Res. 2007, 62, 649–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Ernst, J.; Böker, H.; Hättenschwiler, J.; Schüpbach, D.; Northoff, G.; Seifritz, E.; Grimm, S. The Association of Interoceptive Awareness and Alexithymia with Neurotransmitter Concentrations in Insula and Anterior Cingulate. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 857–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Murphy, J.; Catmur, C.; Bird, G. Alexithymia Is Associated with a Multidomain, Multidimensional Failure of Interoception: Evidence from Novel Tests. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2018, 147, 398–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Trevisan, D.A.; Altschuler, M.R.; Bagdasarov, A.; Carlos, C.; Duan, S.; Hamo, E.; Kala, S.; McNair, M.L.; Parker, T.; Stahl, D.; et al. A Meta-Analysis on the Relationship Between Interoceptive Awareness and Alexithymia: Distinguishing Interoceptive Accuracy and Sensibility. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2019, 128, 765–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Gaggero, G.; Bizzego, A.; Dellantonio, S.; Pastore, L.; Lim, M.; Esposito, G. Clarifying the relationship between alexithymia and subjective interoception. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0261126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Füstös, J.; Gramann, K.; Herbert, B.M.; Pollatos, O. On the embodiment of emotion regulation: Interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2012, 8, 911–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Schaan, L.; Schulz, A.; Nuraydin, S.; Bergert, C.; Hilger, A.; Rach, H.; Hechler, T. Interoceptive accuracy, emotion recognition, and emotion regulation in preschool children. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2019, 138, 47–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Myles, B.S.; Mahler, K.; Robbins, L.A. Sensory Issues and High-Functioning Autism Spectrum and Related Disorders: Practical Solutions for Making Sense of the World, 2nd ed.; AAPC Publishing: Shawnee, KS, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  63. Zetler, N.K.; Cermak, S.A.; Engel-Yeger, B.; Gal, E. Somatosensory discrimination in people with autism spectrum disorder: A scoping review. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2019, 73, 7305205010p1–7305205010p14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Nicholson, T.; Williams, D.; Carpenter, K.; Kallitsounaki, A. Interoception is impaired in children, but not adults, with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 49, 3625–3637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Mahler, K.; Hample, K.; Jones, C.; Sensenig, J.; Thomasco, P.; Hilton, C. Impact of an Interoception-Based Program on Emotion Regulation in Autistic Children. Occup. Ther. Int. 2022, 9328967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Remmers, C.; Topolinski, S.; Koole, S.L. Why being mindful may have more benefits than you realize: Mindfulness improves both explicit and implicit mood regulation. Mindfulness 2016, 7, 829–837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Critchley, H.D.; Garfinkel, S.N. Interoception and emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 17, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Pollatos, O.; Schandry, R. Emotional processing and emotional memory are modulated by interoceptive awareness. Cogn. Emot. 2008, 22, 272–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Øien, R.A.; Cicchetti, D.V.; Nordahl-Hansen, A. Gender Dysphoria, Sexuality and Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Systematic Map Review. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2018, 48, 4028–4037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Strang, J.F.; Kenworthy, L.; Dominska, A.; Sokoloff, J.; Kenealy, L.E.; Berl, M.; Walsh, K.; Menvielle, E.; Slesaransky-Poe, G.; Kim, K.E.; et al. Increased gender variance in autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2014, 43, 1525–1533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Meza-Concha, N.; Arancibia, M.; Salas, F.; Behar, R.; Salas, G.; Silva, H.; Escobar, R. Towards a neurobiological understanding of alexithymia. Medwave 2017, 17, e6960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Mann–Whitney U test for the ASD and CG groups.
Table 1. Mann–Whitney U test for the ASD and CG groups.
VariableASD GroupGC GroupUZp
MMeSDMMeSD
Interoceptive confusion68.9067.0027.9635.6933.0012.01155.50−5.180.000
Alexithymia63.15 *62.009.3050.8050.0012.15237.00−4.180.000
Emotional
attention
20.67 *20.009.0327.1728.007.27320.50−3.160.002
Emotional clarity20.72 *17.008.0930.7729.006.53203.00−4.600.000
Emotional
repair
21.00 *21.007.9629.5430.006.29241.50−4.130.000
M = mean; SD = standard deviation; U = Mann–Whitney U statistic; Z = Z statistic; p = level of significance; * = scores beyond clinical cut-off point.
Table 2. Spearman’s correlation for the ASD group.
Table 2. Spearman’s correlation for the ASD group.
Interoceptive ConfusionEmotional AttentionEmotional ClarityEmotional RepairAlexithymia
Interoceptive confusion1−0.195−0.578 **−0.2860.502 **
Emotional
attention
10.656 **0.2980.176
Emotional
clarity
10.569 **−0.165
Emotional
repair
1−0.121
Alexithymia 1
** The correlation is significant at level 0.01.
Table 3. Spearman’s correlation for the CG.
Table 3. Spearman’s correlation for the CG.
Interoceptive ConfusionEmotional AttentionEmotional ClarityEmotional RepairAlexithymia
Interoceptive confusion10.008−0.0900.1430.563 **
Emotional
attention
1−2750.463 **0.081
Emotional
clarity
10.332−0.146
Emotional
repair
10.196
Alexithymia 1
** The correlation is significant at level 0.01.
Table 4. Multiple stepwise linear regression coefficients for interoception.
Table 4. Multiple stepwise linear regression coefficients for interoception.
Included VariableR2R2 CorrectedF (sig.)Βtp
Step 30.7820.61133.55 (0.000)
Emotional clarity −0.328−4.250.000
Alexithymia 0.3754.350.000
Autism 0.2532.680.009
R2 = determination coefficient; F (sig.) = F significance; β = regression coefficient; t = Student’s t; p = level of significance.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bonete, S.; Molinero, C.; Ruisanchez, D. Emotional Dysfunction and Interoceptive Challenges in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 312. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13040312

AMA Style

Bonete S, Molinero C, Ruisanchez D. Emotional Dysfunction and Interoceptive Challenges in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(4):312. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13040312

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bonete, Saray, Clara Molinero, and Daniela Ruisanchez. 2023. "Emotional Dysfunction and Interoceptive Challenges in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 4: 312. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13040312

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop